Biology:History of attachment theory
Attachment theory, originating in the work of John Bowlby, is a psychological, evolutionary and ethological theory that provides a descriptive and explanatory framework for understanding interpersonal relationships between human beings.
In order to formulate a comprehensive theory of the nature of early attachments, Bowlby explored a range of fields including evolution by natural selection, object relations theory (psychoanalysis), control systems theory, evolutionary biology and the fields of ethology and cognitive psychology.[1] There were some preliminary papers from 1958 onwards but the full theory is published in the trilogy Attachment and Loss, 1969- 82. Although in the early days Bowlby was criticised by academic psychologists and ostracised by the psychoanalytic community,[2] attachment theory has become the dominant approach to understanding early social development and given rise to a great surge of empirical research into the formation of children's close relationships.[3]
Brief description of theory
In infants, behavior associated with attachment is primarily a process of proximity seeking to an identified attachment figure in situations of perceived distress or alarm, for the purpose of survival. Infants become attached to adults who are sensitive and responsive in social interactions with the infant, and who remain as consistent caregivers for some months during the period from about six months to two years of age. During the later part of this period, children begin to use attachment figures (familiar people) as a secure base to explore from and return to. Parental responses lead to the development of patterns of attachment which in turn lead to 'internal working models' which will guide the individual's feelings, thoughts, and expectations in later relationships.[4] Separation anxiety or grief following serious loss are normal and natural responses in an attached infant.
The human infant is considered by attachment theorists to have a need for a secure relationship with adult caregivers, without which normal social and emotional development will not occur. However, different relationship experiences can lead to different developmental outcomes. Mary Ainsworth developed a theory of a number of attachment patterns or "styles" in infants in which distinct characteristics were identified; these were secure attachment, avoidant attachment, anxious attachment and, later, disorganized attachment. In addition to care-seeking by children, peer relationships of all ages, romantic and sexual attraction, and responses to the care needs of infants or sick or elderly adults may be construed as including some components of attachment behavior.
Earlier theories
A theory of attachment is a framework of ideas that attempt to explain attachment, the almost universal human tendency to prefer certain familiar companions over other people, especially when ill, injured, or distressed.[5] Historically, certain social preferences, like those of parents for their children, were explained by reference to instinct, or the moral worth of the individual.[6]
The concept of infants' emotional attachment to caregivers has been known anecdotally for hundreds of years. Most early observers focused on the anxiety displayed by infants and toddlers when threatened with separation from a familiar caregiver.[6][7] Psychological theories about attachment were suggested from the late nineteenth century onward.[8] Freudian theory attempted a systematic consideration of infant attachment and attributed the infant's attempts to stay near the familiar person to motivation learned through feeding experiences and gratification of libidinal drives. In the 1930s, the British developmentalist Ian Suttie put forward the suggestion that the child's need for affection was a primary one, not based on hunger or other physical gratifications.[9] A third theory prevalent at the time of Bowlby's development of attachment theory was "dependency". This approach posited that infants were dependent on adult caregivers but that dependency was, or should be outgrown as the individual matured. Such an approach perceived attachment behaviour in older children as regressive whereas within attachment theory older children and adults remain attached and indeed a secure attachment is associated with independent exploratory behaviour rather than dependence.[10] William Blatz, a Canadian psychologist and teacher of Bowlby's colleague Mary Ainsworth, was among the first to stress the need for security as a normal part of personality at all ages, as well as normality of the use of others as a secure base and the importance of social relationships for other aspects of development.[11]
Current attachment theory focuses on social experiences in early childhood as the source of attachment in childhood and in later life.[12] Attachment theory was developed by Bowlby as a consequence of his dissatisfaction with existing theories of early relationships.[13]
Early developments
Bowlby was influenced by the beginnings of the object relations school of psychoanalysis and in particular, Melanie Klein, although he profoundly disagreed with the psychoanalytic belief then prevalent that saw infants' responses as relating to their internal fantasy life rather than to real life events. As Bowlby began to formulate his concept of attachment, he was influenced by case studies by Levy, Powdermaker, Lowrey, Bender and Goldfarb.[14] An example is the one by David Levy that associated an adopted child's lack of social emotion to her early emotional deprivation.[15] Bowlby himself was interested in the role played in delinquency by poor early relationships, and explored this in a study of young thieves.[16] Bowlby's contemporary René Spitz proposed that "psychotoxic" results were brought about by inappropriate experiences of early care.[17] A strong influence was the work of James and Joyce Robertson who filmed the effects of separation on children in hospital. They and Bowlby collaborated in making the 1952 documentary film A Two-Year Old Goes to the Hospital illustrating the impact of loss and suffering experienced by young children separated from their primary caretakers. This film was instrumental in a campaign to alter hospital restrictions on visiting by parents.[18]
In his 1951 monograph for the World Health Organization, Maternal Care and Mental Health, Bowlby put forward the hypothesis that "the infant and young child should experience a warm, intimate, and continuous relationship with his mother (or permanent mother substitute) in which both find satisfaction and enjoyment" and that not to do so may have significant and irreversible mental health consequences. This proposition was both influential in terms of the effect on the institutional care of children, and highly controversial.[19] There was limited empirical data at the time and no comprehensive theory to account for such a conclusion.[20]
Attachment theory
Following the publication of Maternal Care and Mental Health, Bowlby sought new understanding from such fields as evolutionary biology, ethology, developmental psychology, cognitive science and control systems theory and drew upon them to formulate the innovative proposition that the mechanisms underlying an infants tie emerged as a result of evolutionary pressure.[13] He realised that he had to develop a new theory of motivation and behaviour control, built on up-to-date science rather than the outdated psychic energy model espoused by Freud.[8] Bowlby expressed himself as having made good the "deficiencies of the data and the lack of theory to link alleged cause and effect" in "Maternal Care and Mental Health" in his later work "Attachment and Loss" published between 1969 and 1980.[21]
Bowlby's first official representations were carried out for the relationship theory in three very controversial lectures in 1957 by the British Psychoanalytical Society in London.[22] The formal origin of attachment theory can be traced to the publication of two 1958 papers, one being Bowlby's The Nature of the Child's Tie to his Mother, in which the precursory concepts of "attachment" were introduced, and Harry Harlow's The Nature of Love, based on the results of experiments which showed, approximately, that infant rhesus monkeys spent more time with soft mother-like dummies that offered no food than they did with dummies that provided a food source but were less pleasant to the touch.[23][24][25][26] Bowlby followed this up with two more papers, Separation Anxiety (1960a), and Grief and Mourning in Infancy and Early Childhood (1960b).[27][28] At about the same time, Bowlby's former colleague, Mary Ainsworth was completing extensive observational studies on the nature of infant attachments in Uganda with Bowlby's ethological theories in mind. Mary Ainsworth's innovative methodology and comprehensive observational studies informed much of the theory, expanded its concepts and enabled some of its tenets to be empirically tested.[29] Attachment theory was finally presented in 1969 in Attachment the first volume of the Attachment and Loss trilogy.[30] The second and third volumes, Separation: Anxiety and Anger and Loss: Sadness and Depression followed in 1972 and 1980 respectively.[31][32] Attachment was revised in 1982 to incorporate more recent research.[33]
Ethology
Bowlby's attention was first drawn to ethology when he read Lorenz's 1952 publication in draft form although Lorenz had published much earlier work.[34] Soon after this he encountered the work of Tinbergen,[35] and began to collaborate with Robert Hinde.[36][37] In 1953 he stated "the time is ripe for a unification of psychoanalytic concepts with those of ethology, and to pursue the rich vein of research which this union suggests".[38]
Konrad Lorenz had examined the phenomenon of "imprinting" and felt that it might have some parallels to human attachment. Imprinting, a behavior characteristic of some birds and a very few mammals, involves rapid learning of recognition by a young bird or animal exposed to a conspecific or an object or organism that behaves suitably. The learning is possible only within a limited age period, known as a critical period. This rapid learning and development of familiarity with an animate or inanimate object is accompanied by a tendency to stay close to the object and to follow when it moves; the young creature is said to have been imprinted on the object when this occurs. As the imprinted bird or animal reaches reproductive maturity, its courtship behavior is directed toward objects that resemble the imprinting object. Bowlby's attachment concepts later included the ideas that attachment involves learning from experience during a limited age period, and that the learning that occurs during that time influences adult behavior. However, he did not apply the imprinting concept in its entirety to human attachment, nor assume that human development was as simple as that of birds. He did, however, consider that attachment behavior was best explained as instinctive in nature, an approach that does not rule out the effect of experience, but that stresses the readiness the young child brings to social interactions.[39] Some of Lorenz's work had been done years before Bowlby formulated his ideas, and indeed some ideas characteristic of ethology were already discussed among psychoanalysts some time before the presentation of attachment theory.[40]
Psychoanalysis
Bowlby's view of attachment was also influenced by psychoanalytical concepts and the earlier work of psychoanalysts. In particular he was influenced by observations of young children separated from familiar caregivers, as provided during World War II by Anna Freud and her colleague Dorothy Burlingham.[41]
Observations of separated children's grief by René Spitz were another important factor in the development of attachment theory.[42] However, Bowlby rejected psychoanalytical explanations for early infant bonds. He rejected both Freudian "drive-theory", which he called the Cupboard Love theory of relationships, and early object-relations theory as both in his view failed to see the attachment as a psychological bond in its own right rather than an instinct derived from feeding or sexuality.[43] Thinking in terms of primary attachment and neo-darwinism, Bowlby identified as what he saw as fundamental flaws in psychoanalysis, namely the overemphasis of internal dangers at the expense of external threat, and the picture of the development of personality via linear "phases" with "regression" to fixed points accounting for psychological illness. Instead he posited that several lines of development were possible, the outcome of which depended on the interaction between the organism and the environment. In attachment this would mean that although a developing child has a propensity to form attachments, the nature of those attachments depends on the environment to which the child is exposed.[44]
Internal working model
The important concept of the internal working model of social relationships was adopted by Bowlby from the work of the philosopher Kenneth Craik,[45] who had noted the adaptiveness of the ability of thought to predict events, and stressed the survival value of and natural selection for this ability. According to Craik, prediction occurs when a "small-scale model" consisting of brain events is used to represent not only the external environment, but the individual's own possible actions. This model allows a person to mentally try out alternatives and to use knowledge of the past in responding to the present and future. At about the same time that Bowlby was applying Craik's ideas to the study of attachment, other psychologists were using these concepts in discussion of adult perception and cognition.[46]
Cybernetics
The theory of control systems (cybernetics), developing during the 1930s and '40s, influenced Bowlby's thinking.[47] The young child's need for proximity to the attachment figure was seen as balancing homeostatically with the need for exploration. The actual distance maintained would be greater or less as the balance of needs changed; for example, the approach of a stranger, or an injury, would cause the child to seek proximity when a moment before he had been exploring at a distance.
Behavioural development and attachment
Behaviour analysts have constructed models of attachment. Such models are based on the importance of contingent relationships. Behaviour analytic models have received support from research.[48] and meta-analytic reviews.[49]
Developments
Although research on attachment behaviors continued after Bowlby's death in 1990, there was a period of time when attachment theory was considered to have run its course. Some authors argued that attachment should not be seen as a trait (lasting characteristic of the individual), but instead should be regarded as an organizing principle with varying behaviors resulting from contextual factors.[50] Related later research looked at cross-cultural differences in attachment, and concluded that there should be re-evaluation of the assumption that attachment is expressed identically in all humans.[51] In a recent study conducted in Sapporo, Behrens, et al., 2007 found attachment distributions consistent with global norms using the six-year Main & Cassidy scoring system for attachment classification.[52][53]
Interest in attachment theory continued, and the theory was later extended to adult romantic relationships by Cindy Hazan and Phillip Shaver.[54][55][56] Peter Fonagy and Mary Target have attempted to bring attachment theory and psychoanalysis into a closer relationship by way of such aspects of cognitive science as mentalization, the ability to estimate what the beliefs or intentions of another person may be.[47] A "natural experiment" has permitted extensive study of attachment issues, as researchers have followed the thousands of Romanian orphans who were adopted into Western families after the end of Nicolae Ceauşescu's regime. The English and Romanian Adoptees Study Team, led by Michael Rutter, has followed some of the children into their teens, attempting to unravel the effects of poor attachment, adoption and new relationships, and the physical and medical problems associated with their early lives. Studies on the Romanian adoptees, whose initial conditions were shocking, have in fact yielded reason for optimism. Many of the children have developed quite well, and the researchers have noted that separation from familiar people is only one of many factors that help to determine the quality of development.[57]
Neuroscientific studies are examining the physiological underpinnings of observable attachment style, such as vagal tone which influences capacities for intimacy,[58] stress response which influences threat reactivity (Lupien, McEwan, Gunnar & Heim, 2009),[59] as well as neuroendocrinology such as oxytocin,.[60][61] These types of studies underscore the fact that attachment is an embodied capacity not only a cognitive one.
Effects of changing times and approaches
Some authors have noted the connection of attachment theory with Western family and child care patterns characteristic of Bowlby's time. The implication of this connection is that attachment-related experiences (and perhaps attachment itself) may alter as young children's experience of care change historically. For example, changes in attitudes toward female sexuality have greatly increased the numbers of children living with their never-married mothers and being cared for outside the home while the mothers work.
This social change, in addition to increasing abortion rates, has also made it more difficult for childless people to adopt infants in their own countries, and has increased the number of older-child adoptions and adoptions from third-world sources. Adoptions and births to same-sex couples have increased in number and even gained some legal protection, compared to their status in Bowlby's time.[62]
One focus of attachment research has been on the difficulties of children whose attachment history was poor, including those with extensive non-parental child care experiences. Concern with the effects of child care was intense during the so-called "day care wars" of the late 20th century, during which the deleterious effects of day care were stressed.[63] As a beneficial result of this controversy, training of child care professionals has come to stress attachment issues and the need for relationship-building through techniques such as assignment of a child to a specific care provider. Although only high-quality child care settings are likely to follow through on these considerations, nevertheless a larger number of infants in child care receive attachment-friendly care than was the case in the past, and emotional development of children in nonparental care may be different today than it was in the 1980s or in Bowlby's time.[64]
Finally, any critique of attachment theory needs to consider how the theory has connected with changes in other psychological theories. Research on attachment issues has begun to include concepts related to behaviour genetics and to the study of temperament (constitutional factors in personality), but it is unusual for popular presentations of attachment theory to include these. Importantly, some researchers and theorists have begun to connect attachment with the study of mentalization or Theory of Mind, the capacity that allows human beings to guess with some accuracy what thoughts, emotions, and intentions lie behind behaviours as subtle as facial expression or eye movement.[65] The connection of theory of mind with the internal working model of social relationships may open a new area of study and lead to alterations in attachment theory.[66]
Reception
1950s to the 1970s
The maternal deprivation hypothesis, attachment theory's precursor, was enormously controversial. Ten years after the publication of the hypothesis, Ainsworth listed nine concerns that she felt were the chief points of controversy.[67] Ainsworth separated the three dimensions of maternal deprivation into lack of maternal care, distortion of maternal care and discontinuity of maternal care. She analysed the dozens of studies undertaken in the field and concluded that the basic assertions of the maternal deprivation hypothesis were sound although the controversy continued.[68] As the formulation of attachment theory progressed, critics commented on empirical support for the theory and for the possible alternative explanations for results of empirical research.[69] Wootton questioned the suggestion that early attachment history (as it would now be called) had a lifelong impact.[70]
In 1957 found the young relationship theory in the DDR (East Germany) by an essay of James Robertson in the Zeitschrift für ärztliche Fortbildung (magazine for a medical further education) and Eva Schmidt-Kolmer carried out some journal extracts from Bowlby's essay Maternal Care and mental Health for WHO.[71] In the following period it came to extensive comparative development psychological in the DDR at the end of the fifties. Examinations between family-bound babies and small children, day and week hayracks-as well as Institution children. The findings could do with regard to the morbidity for the family-bound children, the physical and emotional development as well as adaption disturbances at change of environment. After the construction of the Berlin Wall 1961 it didn't come to any additional publications in the DDR Relationship theory and comparative investigations with family-bound children. The previous ones Research results weren't published further and got like the relationship theory into oblivion in the DDR in the subsequent years.[72]
In the 1970s, problems with the emphasis on attachment as a trait (a stable characteristic of an individual) rather than as a type of behaviour with important organising functions and outcomes, led some authors to consider that "attachment (as implying anything but infant-adult interaction) [may be said to have] outlived its usefulness as a developmental construct..." and that attachment behaviours were best understood in terms of their functions in the child's life.[50] Children may achieve a given function, such as a sense of security, in many different ways and the various but functionally comparable behaviours should be categorized as related to each other. This way of thinking saw the secure base concept (the organisation of exploration of an unfamiliar situation around returns to a familiar person) as "central to the logic and coherence of attachment theory and to its status as an organizational construct."[73] Similarly, Thompson pointed out that "other features of early parent-child relationships that develop concurrently with attachment security, including negotiating conflict and establishing cooperation, also must be considered in understanding the legacy of early attachments."[74]
Specific disciplines
Psychoanalysis
From an early point in the development of attachment theory, there was criticism of the theory's lack of congruence with the various branches of psychoanalysis. Like other members of the British object-relations group, Bowlby rejected Melanie Klein's views that considered the infant to have certain mental capacities at birth and to continue to develop emotionally on the basis of fantasy rather than of real experiences. But Bowlby also withdrew from the object-relations approach (exemplified, for example, by Anna Freud), as he abandoned the "drive theory" assumptions in favor of a set of automatic, instinctual behaviour systems that included attachment. Bowlby's decisions left him open to criticism from well-established thinkers working on problems similar to those he addressed.[75][76][77] Bowlby was effectively ostracized from the psychoanalytic community.[2] More recently some psychoanalysts have sought to reconcile the two theories in the form of attachment-based psychotherapy, a therapeutic approach.
Ethology
Ethologists expressed concern about the adequacy of some of the research on which attachment theory was based, particularly the generalisation to humans from animal studies.[78][79] Schur, discussing Bowlby's use of ethological concepts (pre-1960) commented that these concepts as used in attachment theory had not kept up with changes in ethology itself.[80]
Ethologists and others writing in the 1960s and 1970s questioned the types of behaviour used as indications of attachment, and offered alternative approaches. For example, crying on separation from a familiar person was suggested as an index of attachment.[81] Observational studies of young children in natural settings also provided behaviours that might be considered to indicate attachment; for example, staying within a predictable distance of the mother without effort on her part and picking up small objects and bringing them to the mother, but usually not other adults.[82] Although ethological work tended to be in agreement with Bowlby, work like that just described led to the conclusion that "[w]e appear to disagree with Bowlby and Ainsworth on some of the details of the child's interactions with its mother and other people". Some ethologists pressed for further observational data, arguing that psychologists "are still writing as if there is a real entity which is 'attachment', existing over and above the observable measures."[83]
Robert Hinde expressed concern with the use of the word "attachment" to imply that it was an intervening variable or a hypothesised internal mechanism rather than a data term. He suggested that confusion about the meaning of attachment theory terms "could lead to the 'instinct fallacy' of postulating a mechanism isomorphous with the behaviours, and then using that as an explanation for the behaviour". However, Hinde considered "attachment behaviour system" to be an appropriate term of theory language which did not offer the same problems "because it refers to postulated control systems that determine the relations between different kinds of behaviour."[84]
Cognitive development
Bowlby's reliance on Piaget's theory of cognitive development gave rise to questions about object permanence (the ability to remember an object that is temporarily absent) and its connection to early attachment behaviours, and about the fact that the infant's ability to discriminate strangers and react to the mother's absence seems to occur some months earlier than Piaget suggested would be cognitively possible.[85] More recently, it has been noted that the understanding of mental representation has advanced so much since Bowlby's day that present views can be far more specific than those of Bowlby's time.[86]
Behaviourism
In 1969, Gewirtz discussed how mother and child could provide each other with positive reinforcement experiences through their mutual attention and therefore learn to stay close together; this explanation would make it unnecessary to posit innate human characteristics fostering attachment.[87] Learning theory saw attachment as a remnant of dependency and the quality of attachment as merely a response to the caregivers cues. Behaviourists saw behaviours such as crying as a random activity that meant nothing until reinforced by a caregivers response therefore frequent responses would result in more crying. To attachment theorists, crying is an inborn attachment behaviour to which the caregiver must respond if the infant is to develop emotional security. Conscientious responses produce security which enhances autonomy and results in less crying. Ainsworth's research in Baltimore supported the attachment theorists view.[88] In the last decade, behaviour analysts have constructed models of attachment based on the importance of contingent relationships. These behaviour analytic models have received some support from research[48] and meta-analytic reviews.[49]
Methodology
There has been critical discussion of conclusions drawn from clinical and observational work, and whether or not they actually support tenets of attachment theory. For example, Skuse based criticism of a basic tenet of attachment theory on the work of Anna Freud with children from Theresienstadt, who apparently developed relatively normally in spite of serious deprivation during their early years. This discussion concluded from Freud's case and from some other studies of extreme deprivation that there is an excellent prognosis for children with this background, unless there are biological or genetic risk factors.[89] The psychoanalyst Margaret Mahler interpreted ambivalent or aggressive behaviour of toddlers toward their mothers as a normal part of development, not as evidence of poor attachment history.[90]
Some of Bowlby's interpretations of the data reported by James Robertson were eventually rejected by the researcher, who reported data from 13 young children who were cared for in ideal circumstances during separation from their mothers. Robertson noted, "...Bowlby acknowledges that he draws mainly upon James Robertson's institutional data. But in developing his grief and mourning theory, Bowlby, without adducing non-institutional data, has generalized Robertson's concept of protest, despair and denial beyond the context from which it was derived. He asserts that these are the usual responses of young children to separation from the mother regardless of circumstance..."; however, of the 13 separated children who received good care, none showed protest and despair, but "coped with separation from the mother when cared for in conditions from which the adverse factors which complicate institutional studies were absent".[91] In the second volume of the trilogy, Separation, published two years later, Bowlby acknowledged that Robertsons foster study had caused him to modify his views on the traumatic consequences of separation in which insufficient weight was given to the influence of skilled care from a familiar substitute.[92]
Some authors have questioned the idea of attachment patterns, thought to be measured by techniques like the Strange Situation Protocol. Such techniques yield a taxonomy of categories considered to represent qualitative difference in attachment relationships (for example, secure attachment versus avoidant). However, a categorical model is not necessarily the best representation of individual difference in attachment. An examination of data from 1139 15-month-olds showed that variation was continuous rather than falling into natural groupings.[93] This criticism introduces important questions for attachment typologies and the mechanisms behind apparent types, but in fact has relatively little relevance for attachment theory itself, which "neither requires nor predicts discrete patterns of attachment."[94] As was noted above, ethologists have suggested other behavioural measures that may be of greater importance than Strange Situation behaviour.
1980s on
Following the argument made in the 1970s that attachment should not be seen as a trait (lasting characteristic of the individual), but instead should be regarded as an organising principle with varying behaviours resulting from contextual factors,[50] later research looked at cross-cultural differences in attachment, and concluded that there should be re-evaluation of the assumption that attachment is expressed identically in all humans.[51] Various studies appeared to show cultural differences but a 2007 study conducted in Sapporo in Japan found attachment distributions consistent with global norms using the six-year Main & Cassidy scoring system for attachment classification.[52][53]
Recent critics such as J. R. Harris, Steven Pinker and Jerome Kagan are generally concerned with the concept of infant determinism (Nature versus nurture) and stress the possible effects of later experience on personality.[95]Cite error: Closing </ref>
missing for <ref>
tag
Concern about attachment theory has been raised with regard to the fact that infants often have multiple relationships, within the family as well as in child care settings, and that the dyadic model characteristic of attachment theory cannot address the complexity of real-life social experiences.[96]
See also
Notes
- ↑ Simpson JA (1999). "Attachment Theory in Modern Evolutionary Perspective". Handbook of Attachment:Theory, Research and Clinical Applications. New York: Guilford Press. pp. 115–140. ISBN 978-1-57230-087-3. https://archive.org/details/handbookofattach0000unse/page/115.
- ↑ 2.0 2.1 Rutter M (1995). "Clinical Implications of attachment Concepts: Retrospect and Prospect". Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry 36 (4): 549–571. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.1995.tb02314.x. PMID 7650083.
- ↑ Schaffer R (2007). Introducing Child Psychology. Blackwell. ISBN 978-0-631-21628-5.
- ↑ "Internal Working Models in Attachment Relationships: A Construct Revisited". Handbook of Attachment:Theory, Research and Clinical Applications. Guilford press. 1999. pp. 89–114. ISBN 978-1-57230-087-3. https://archive.org/details/handbookofattach0000unse/page/89.
- ↑ Mercer p. 37
- ↑ 6.0 6.1 Fildes V (1988). "Wet nursing". Midwife, Health Visitor & Community Nurse 22 (7): 241–7. PMID 3537643.
- ↑ de Saussure RA (1940). "JB Felix Descuret". Psychoanalytic Study of the Child 2: 417–424.
- ↑ 8.0 8.1 Bretherton I (1992). "The Origins of Attachment Theory: John Bowlby and Mary Ainsworth". Developmental Psychology 28 (5): 759. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.28.5.759.
- ↑ Suttie I (1935). The origins of love and hate. London: Penguin. ISBN 978-0-415-21042-3.
- ↑ Prior and Glaser p. 20
- ↑ "William Emet Blatz". Portraits of pioneers in psychology. II. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. 1996. pp. 199–212. ISBN 978-0-8058-2198-7.
- ↑ Mercer p. 23
- ↑ 13.0 13.1 Cassidy J (1999). "The Nature of a Childs Ties". Handbook of Attachment:Theory, Research and Clinical Applications. New York: Guilford Press. pp. 3–20. ISBN 978-1-57230-087-3. https://archive.org/details/handbookofattach0000unse/page/3.
- ↑ Bowlby J (1951). Maternal Care and Mental Health. 3. Geneva: World Health Organisation. 355–533. ISBN 978-1-56821-757-4. "With monotonous regularity each put his finger on the child's inability to make relationships as being the central feature from which all other disturbances sprang, and on the history of institutionalisation or, as in the case quoted, of the child's being shifted about from one foster-mother to another as being its cause"
- ↑ Levy, David M. (1937). "Primary Affect Hunger". American Journal of Psychiatry (American Psychiatric Association Publishing) 94 (3): 643–652. doi:10.1176/ajp.94.3.643. ISSN 0002-953X.
- ↑ Bowlby J (1944). "Forty-four juvenile thieves: Their characters and home life". International Journal of Psychoanalysis 25 (19–52): 107–127. "sometimes referred to by Bowlby's colleagues as "Ali Bowlby and the Forty Thieves"".
- ↑ Spitz RA (1951). "The psychogenic diseases in infancy". Psychoanalytic Study of the Child 6: 255–275. doi:10.1080/00797308.1952.11822915.
- ↑ Schwartz J (1999). Cassandra's Daughter: A History of Psychoanalysis. Viking/Allen Lane. p. 225. ISBN 978-0-670-88623-4.
- ↑ "Preface". Deprivation of Maternal Care: A Reassessment of its Effects. Public Health Papers. Geneva: World Health Organization.
- ↑ Bowlby (1988) p. 24
- ↑ Bowlby J (December 1986), "This Week's Citation Classic Classic", Current Contents 50 (18), http://www.garfield.library.upenn.edu/classics1986/A1986F063100001.pdf, retrieved 2008-07-13
- ↑ Bretherton, Inge: Developmental Psychology (1992), 28, p. 759-775
- ↑ Bowlby J (1958). "The nature of the child's tie to his mother". Int J Psychoanal 39 (5): 350–73. PMID 13610508.
- ↑ Bowlby J (2005). The Making and Breaking of Affectional Bonds. Routledge Classics. ISBN 978-0-415-35481-3.
- ↑ Harlow H (1958). "The Nature of Love". American Psychologist 13 (12): 573–685. doi:10.1037/h0047884. http://psychclassics.yorku.ca/Harlow/love.htm.
- ↑ ""When strangers meet": John Bowlby and Harry Harlow on attachment behavior". Integrative Psychological & Behavioral Science 42 (4): 370–388. 2008. doi:10.1007/s12124-008-9079-2. ISSN 1936-3567. PMID 18766423.
- ↑ Bowlby J (1960). "Separation anxiety". Int J Psychoanal 41: 89–113. PMID 13803480.
- ↑ Bowlby J (1960). "Grief and mourning in infancy and early childhood". The Psychoanalytic Study of the Child 15: 9–52. doi:10.1080/00797308.1960.11822566.
- ↑ Bretherton I (1992). "The Origins of Attachment Theory: John Bowlby and Mary Ainsworth". Developmental Psychology 28 (5): 759–775. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.28.5.759.
- ↑ Bowlby J (1969). Attachment. Attachment and loss. Vol. I. London: Hogarth. ISBN 978-0-465-00543-7. https://archive.org/details/attachmentloss00bowl.
- ↑ Bowlby J (1973). Separation: Anxiety & Anger. Attachment and Loss (vol. 2); (International psycho-analytical library no.95). London: Hogarth Press. ISBN 978-0-7126-6621-3. OCLC 8353942.
- ↑ Bowlby J (1980). Loss: Sadness & Depression. Attachment and Loss (vol. 3); (International psycho-analytical library no.109). London: Hogarth Press. ISBN 978-0-465-04238-8. OCLC 59246032.
- ↑ Bowlby J (1999). Attachment. Attachment and Loss Vol. I (2nd ed.). New York: Basic Books. NLM 8412414. ISBN 978-0-465-00543-7. OCLC 11442968. https://archive.org/details/attachmentloss00bowl.
- ↑ Lorenz KZ (1937). "The companion in the bird's world". The Auk 54 (3): 245–273. doi:10.2307/4078077.
- ↑ Tinbergen N (1951). The study of instinct. Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-857722-5.
- ↑ "John Bowlby and ethology: An annotated interview with Robert Hinde". Attachment & Human Development 9 (4): 321–335. 2007. doi:10.1080/14616730601149809. ISSN 1469-2988. PMID 17852051.
- ↑ Holmes p. 62
- ↑ Bowlby J (1953). Critical phases in the development of social responses in man and other animals. New Biology. London: Penguin.
- ↑ Bowlby (1969) 2nd ed. pp. 220–223
- ↑ Hartmann H (1950). "Psychoanalysis and developmental psychology". Psychoanalytic Study of the Child 5: 7–17. doi:10.1080/00797308.1950.11822880.
- ↑ War and children. Medical War Books. 1943. ISBN 978-0-8371-6942-2. https://archive.org/details/Freud_Burlingham_1943_War_and_Children_k.
- ↑ Spitz R (1945). Hospitalism: An Inquiry into the Genesis of Psychiatric Conditions in Early Childhood.
- ↑ Holmes pp. 62–3
- ↑ Holmes pp. 64–5
- ↑ Craik K (1943). The Nature of Explanation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-0-521-09445-0.
- ↑ Johnson-Laird PN (1983). Mental models. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. ISBN 978-0-674-56881-5.
- ↑ 47.0 47.1 "Attachment theory and cognitive science: commentary on Fonagy and Target". J Am Psychoanal Assoc 55 (2): 457–67; discussion 493–501. 2007. doi:10.1177/00030651070550021401. PMID 17601100.
- ↑ 48.0 48.1 "Relationship between parental contingent-responsiveness and attachment outcomes". Bridges 2 (4): 1–17. 2004.
- ↑ 49.0 49.1 "Caregiver Sensitivity, Contingent Social Responsiveness, and Secure Infant Attachment". Journal of Early and Intensive Behavior Intervention 5 (1): 40–56. 2008. doi:10.1037/h0100409.
- ↑ 50.0 50.1 50.2 "Attachment as an organizational construct". Child Development 48 (4): 1184–1199. 1977. doi:10.2307/1128475.
- ↑ 51.0 51.1 "The Efe forager infant and toddler's pattern of social relationships: Multiple and simultaneous". Developmental Psychology 28 (4): 568–577. 1992. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.28.4.568.
- ↑ 52.0 52.1 "Mothers' attachment status as determined by the Adult Attachment Interview predicts their 6-year-olds' reunion responses: a study conducted in Japan". Dev Psychol 43 (6): 1553–67. November 2007. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.43.6.1553. PMID 18020832. http://content.apa.org/journals/dev/43/6/1553.
- ↑ 53.0 53.1 "Categories of response to reunion with the parent at age 6: Predictable from infant attachment classifications and stable over a 1-month period". Developmental Psychology 24 (3): 415–426. 1988. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.24.3.415.
- ↑ "Romantic love conceptualized as an attachment process". J Pers Soc Psychol 52 (3): 511–24. March 1987. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.52.3.511. PMID 3572722. http://content.apa.org/journals/psp/52/3/511.
- ↑ "Love and work: An attachment theoretical perspective". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 59 (2): 270–280. 1990. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.59.2.270.
- ↑ "Attachment as an organizational framework for research on close relationships". Psychological Inquiry 5: 1–22. 1994. doi:10.1207/s15327965pli0501_1.
- ↑ Rutter M (Jan–Feb 2002). "Nature, nurture, and development: From evangelism through science toward policy and practice". Child Development 73 (1): 1–21. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00388. ISSN 0009-3920. PMID 14717240.
- ↑ Porges S (2011). The polyvagal theory: Neurophysiological foundations of emotions, attachment, communication, self-regulation. Norton.
- ↑ "Effects of stress throughout the lifespan on the brain, behaviour and cognition". Nature Reviews Neuroscience 10 (6): 434–445. 2009. doi:10.1038/nrn2639. PMID 19401723.
- ↑ Carter CS (1998). "Neuroendocrine perspectives on social attachment and love". Psychoneuroendocrinology 23 (8): 779–818. doi:10.1016/s0306-4530(98)00055-9. PMID 9924738.
- ↑ Uvnas-Moberg K (1996). "Neuroendocrinology of the mother-child interaction". Trends in Endocrinology and Metabolism 7 (4): 126–131. doi:10.1016/1043-2760(96)00036-7. PMID 18406738.
- ↑ Mercer pp. 152–56
- ↑ "Nonmaternal care in the first year of life and the security of infant-parent attachment". Child Dev 59 (1): 157–67. February 1988. doi:10.2307/1130397. PMID 3342709.
- ↑ Mercer pp. 160–63
- ↑ Affect regulation, mentalization, and the development of the self. New York: Other Press. 2002. ISBN 978-1-59051-161-9.
- ↑ Mercer pp. 165–68
- ↑ Ainsworth MD (1962). "The effects of maternal deprivation: A review of findings and controversy in the context of research strategy". Deprivation of Maternal Care: A Reassessment of its Effects. Public Health Papers. 14. Geneva: World Health Organization. pp. 97–165. "1) The vagueness of the term "maternal deprivation" used in the description of a child's history of attachment experiences. 2) The lack of clarity of the theory's implications for experiences with multiple caregivers. 3) The implications for the theory of the degree of variability following "deprivation". 4) The question of what specific effects result from "deprivation". 5) The question of individual differences in children's reactions to separation or loss. 6) The question of the degree of permanence of specific effects of "deprivation". 7) The question of delinquency as an infrequent outcome of separation and loss. 8) The question of specifics of deprivation and whether these have to do with the caregiver or the more general environment. 9) Controversies having to do with the effects of genetic defects or of brain damage on the developmental outcome.".
- ↑ Karen pp. 123–24
- ↑ Karen pp. 115–18
- ↑ Wootton B (1959). Social science and social pathology. London: Allen and Unwin. ISBN 978-0-313-20339-8.
- ↑ Robertson, J.: Über den Verlust mütterlicher Fürsorge in früher Kindheit. In: Zeitschrift für ärztliche Fortbildung 1957, 21/22
- ↑ Plückhahn, Jens: Dauerheime für Säuglinge und Kleinkinder in der DDR aus dem Blickwinkel der Bindungstheorie. Diplomarbeit FH Potsdam, Potsdam 2012, S. 60 und S. 101ff.; Bundesarchiv Berlin-Lichterfelde - Ministerium für Gesundheitswesen der DDR BArch DQ 1/13585 u.a.m.; Zeitschrift für ärztliche Fortbildung in der DDR 1957,21/22, S. 895ff. / 1958,7, S. 307ff. / 1959,22, S. 1443ff. / 1960,21, S. 1220ff. u.a.m.
- ↑ "A secure base from which to explore close relationships". Child Development 71 (1): 164–72. 2000. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00130. PMID 10836570.
- ↑ Thompson RA (2000). "The legacy of early attachments". Child Development 71 (1): 145–52. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00128. PMID 10836568.
- ↑ "Attachment and psychoanalysis: Time for a reunion". Social Development 7 (1): 92–119. 1998. doi:10.1111/1467-9507.00053.
- ↑ Cassidy J (1998). "Commentary on Steele and Steele: Attachment and object relations theories and the concept of independent behavioral systems". Social Development 7 (1): 120–26. doi:10.1111/1467-9507.00054.
- ↑ "Debate: Attachment and psychoanalysis: Time for a reunion". Social Development 7 (1): 92–119. 1998. doi:10.1111/1467-9507.00053.
- ↑ "Animal models of human behavior: Their application to the study of attachment". The development of attachment and affiliative systems. New york: Plenum. 1982. pp. 31–42. ISBN 978-0-306-40849-6.
- ↑ Blurton-Jones N, ed (1972). "Human non-verbal behaviour: A means of communication". Ethological studies of child behaviour. Cambridge University Press. pp. 37–64. ISBN 978-0-521-09855-7. "...it must be emphasized that data derived from species other than man can be used only to suggest hypotheses that may be worth applying to man for testing by critical observations. In the absence of critical evidence derived from observing man such hypotheses are no more than intelligent guesses. There is a danger in human ethology... that interesting, but untested, hypotheses may gain the status of accepted theory. [One author] has coined the term 'ethologism' as a label for the present vogue [in 1970]... for uncritically invoking the findings from ethological studies of other species as necessary and sufficient explanations... Theory based on superficial analogies between species has always impeded biological understanding... We conclude that a valid ethology of man must be based primarily on data derived from man, and not on data obtained from fish, birds, or other primates".
- ↑ Schur M (1960). "Discussion of Dr. John Bowlby's paper". Psychoanalytic Study of the Child 15: 63–84. doi:10.1080/00797308.1960.11822568. PMID 13749000. "Bowlby...assumes the fully innate, unlearned character of most complex behavior patterns...(whereas recent animal studies showed)...both the early impact of learning and the great intricacy of the interaction between mother and litter"...(and applies)..."to human behavior an instinct concept which neglects the factor of development and learning far beyond even the position taken by Lorenz [the ethological theorist] in his early propositions".
- ↑ "The development of social attachment in infancy". Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, Serial No. 94 29 (3). 1964.
- ↑ Anderson JW (1972). "Attachment behaviour out of doors". in Blurton-Jones N. Ethological studies of child behaviour. Cambridge University Press. pp. 199–216. ISBN 978-0-521-09855-7.
- ↑ Blurton-Jones N, ed (1972). "Behaviour of children and their mothers at separation and greeting". Ethological studies of child behaviour. Cambridge University Press. pp. 217–48. ISBN 978-0-521-09855-7.
- ↑ Hinde R (1982). Ethology. Oxford University Press. p. 229. ISBN 978-0-00-686034-1.
- ↑ Fraiberg S (1969). "Libidinal object constancy and mental representation". Psychoanalytic Study of the Child 24: 9–47. doi:10.1080/00797308.1969.11822685. PMID 5353377.
- ↑ "The attachment working models concept: among other things, we build script-like representations of secure base experiences". Attachment & Human Development 8 (3): 185–97. September 2006. doi:10.1080/14616730600856016. PMID 16938702.
- ↑ Gewirtz N (1969). "Potency of a social reinforcer as a function of satiation and recovery". Developmental Psychology 1: 2–13. doi:10.1037/h0026802.
- ↑ Karen pp. 166–73
- ↑ Skuse D (October 1984). "Extreme deprivation in early childhood--II. Theoretical issues and a comparative review". Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 25 (4): 543–72. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.1984.tb00172.x. PMID 6480730.
- ↑ Mahler MS (1971). "A study of the separation-individuation process and its possible application to borderline phenomena in the psychoanalytic situation". Psychoanalytic Study of the Child 26: 403–24. doi:10.1080/00797308.1971.11822279. PMID 5163236.
- ↑ "Young children in brief separation. A fresh look". Psychoanalytic Study of the Child 26: 264–315. 1971. doi:10.1080/00797308.1971.11822274. PMID 5163230.
- ↑ Karen pp. 82–86
- ↑ "Are infant attachment patterns continuously or categorically distributed? A taxometric analysis of strange situation behavior". Developmental Psychology 39 (3): 387–404. May 2003. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.39.3.387. PMID 12760508. http://content.apa.org/journals/dev/39/3/387.
- ↑ "Are there really patterns of attachment? Comment on Fraley and Spieker (2003)". Developmental Psychology 39 (3): 417–22; discussion 423–9. May 2003. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.39.3.417. PMID 12760512. http://content.apa.org/journals/dev/39/3/417.
- ↑ Harris JR (1998). The Nurture Assumption: Why Children Turn Out the Way They Do. New York: Free press. ISBN 978-0-684-84409-1.
- ↑ McHale JP (2007). "When infants grow up in multiperson relationship systems". Infant Mental Health Journal 28 (4): 370–92. doi:10.1002/imhj.20142. PMID 21512615.
References
- Bowlby J (1969). Attachment. Attachment and loss. Vol. I. London: Hogarth. ISBN 978-0-465-00543-7. https://archive.org/details/attachmentloss00bowl. (page numbers refer to Pelican edition 1971)
- Bowlby J (1999). Attachment. Attachment and Loss Vol.I (2nd ed.). New York: Basic Books. NLM 8412414. ISBN 978-0-465-00543-7. OCLC 11442968. https://archive.org/details/attachmentloss00bowl.
- Bowlby J (1988). A Secure Base: Clinical Applications of Attachment Theory. London: Routledge. ISBN 978-0-415-00640-8.
- Holmes J (1993). John Bowlby & Attachment Theory. Makers of modern psychotherapy. Routledge. ISBN 978-0-415-07729-3.
- Mercer, J (2006). Understanding attachment: Parenting, child care, and emotional development. Westport, CT: Praeger Publishers. ISBN 978-0-275-98217-1. OCLC 61115448. https://archive.org/details/understandingatt0000merc.
- Understanding Attachment and Attachment Disorders: Theory, Evidence and Practice. Child and Adolescent Mental Health, RCPRTU. Jessica Kingsley Publishers. 2006. ISBN 978-1-84310-245-8.
- Van der Horst FCP (2011). John Bowlby - From Psychoanalysis to Ethology. Unraveling the roots of attachment theory. Oxford, UK: Wiley-Blackwell. ISBN 978-0-470-68364-4.
External links
- Richard Karen. 'Becoming Attached'. The Atlantic Monthly February, 1990.
- Review of Richard Karen. Becoming Attached: First Relationships and How They Shape Our Capacity to Love.
- Rene Spitz's film "Psychogenic Disease in Infancy" (1957)
Original source: https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/History of attachment theory.
Read more |