Biology:Major histocompatibility complex and sexual selection
The major histocompatibility complex in sexual selection concerns how major histocompatibility complex (MHC) molecules allow for immune system surveillance of the population of protein molecules in a host's cells. In 1976, Yamazaki et al. demonstrated a sexual selection mate choice by male mice for females of a different MHC[clarify].
Major histocompatibility complex genes, which control the immune response and effective resistance against pathogens, have been able to maintain an extremely high level of allelic diversity throughout time and throughout different populations. Studies suggest that the MHC is involved in mate choice for many vertebrates through olfactory cues. There are several proposed hypotheses that address how MHC-associated mating preferences could be adaptive and how an unusually large amount of allelic diversity has been maintained in the MHC.[1][2]
The vast source of genetic variation affecting an organism's fitness stems from the co-evolutionary arms race between hosts and parasites. There are two nonmutually exclusive hypotheses for explaining this. One is that there is selection for the maintenance of a highly diverse set of MHC genes if MHC heterozygotes are more resistant to parasites than homozygotes—this is called heterozygote advantage. The second is that there is selection that undergoes a frequency-dependent cycle—and is called the Red Queen hypothesis.
Hypotheses
In the first hypothesis, if individuals heterozygous at the MHC are more resistant to parasites than those that are homozygous, then it is beneficial for females to choose mates with MHC genes different from their own, and would result in MHC-heterozygous offspring—this is known as disassortative mating. The hypothesis states that individuals with a heterozygous MHC would be capable of recognizing a wider range of pathogens and therefore of inciting a specific immune response against a greater number of pathogens—thus having an immunity advantage. Unfortunately, the MHC-heterozygote advantage hypothesis has not been adequately tested.[2] A non-MHC immune genes across species exhibit heterozygote disadvantage, or no advantage.[3][4][5][6][7][8] In mice, increased MHC heterozygosity reduces fitness, challenging this hypothesis. MHC-heterozygous females had significantly reduced fitness compared to homozygotes.[9] This finding has been replicated in another study in mice and again in fish[10][11] In some cases, excess heterozygosity can lead to decreased fitness.[12]
The optimality hypothesis states too much variability in the MHC can result in a failure of T-cells to distinguish themselves non-selves, and thereby increase the risk of autoimmune disease. This would confer greater fitness to individuals without a large degree MHC diversity.[6][13] Autoimmune diseases are associated with MHC loci. In humans, those with greater MHC diversity have a greater risk for autoimmune disorders. MHC diversity may be low "because foreign peptides have to stand out against the self-background." On an individual level, MHC diversity tends to be low. Across many species, there is intermediate heterozygosity in the MHC. Overall evidence supports intermediate MHC heterozygosity is best.[14]
The Red Queen hypothesis asserts that MHC diversity is maintained by parasites. If individuals' MHC alleles render different resistances to a particular parasite, then the allele with the highest resistance is favored, selected for, and consequently spread throughout the population. Recombination and mutation cause generation of new variants among offspring, which may facilitate a quick response to rapidly evolving parasites or pathogens with much shorter generation times. However, if this particular allele becomes common, selection pressure on parasites to avoid recognition by this common allele increases. An advantageous characteristic that allows a parasite to escape recognition spreads, and causes selection against what was formerly a resistant allele. This enables the parasite to escape this cycle of frequency-dependent selection, and such a cycle eventually leads to a co-evolutionary arms race that may support the maintenance of MHC diversity. This hypothesis has empirical support.[15][2][16]
The inbreeding avoidance hypothesis has less to do with host-parasite relationships than does the heterozygote advantage hypothesis or the Red Queen hypothesis. The extreme diversity in the MHC would cause individuals sharing MHC alleles to be more likely to be related. As a result, one function of MHC-disassortative mating would be to avoid mating with family members and any harmful genetic consequences that could occur as a result. The hypothesis states that inbreeding increases the amount of overall homozygosity—not just locally in the MHC, so an increase in genetic homozygosity may be accompanied not only by the expression of recessive diseases and mutations, but by the loss of any potential heterozygote advantage as well.[17][2] Animals only rarely avoid inbreeding.[18] The inbreeding avoidance hypothesis has been "ruled out as an explanation for the observed pattern of MHC-dependent mate preference" because relatedness is not associated with mate choice.[19]
In the course of searching for potential mates, it would benefit females to be able to discriminate against "bad" genes in order to increase the health and viability of their offspring. If female mate choice occurs for "good" genes, then it is implied that genetic variation exists among males. Furthermore, one would presume that said difference in genes would impart a difference in fitness as well, which could potentially be chosen or selected for.
Generally, the extreme polymorphism of MHC genes is selected for by host-parasite arms races (the Red Queen hypothesis); however, disassortative mate choice may maintain genetic diversity in some species. Depending on how parasites alter selection on MHC alleles, MHC-dependent mate-choice may increase the fitness of the offspring by enhancing its immunity, as mentioned earlier. If this is the case, either through the heterozygote advantage hypothesis or the Red Queen hypothesis, then selection also favors mating practices that are MHC-dependent.
Therefore, mate choice—with respect to the MHC—has probably evolved so that females choose males either based on diverse genes (heterozygote advantage and inbreeding avoidance hypotheses) or "good" genes. The fact that females choose is naturally selected, as it would be an advantageous trait for females to be able to choose a male that provided either an indirect or direct benefit. As a result of female choice, sexual selection is imposed on males. This is evidenced by genetic "advertisement"—an example of this would be the existence of exaggerated traits, such as the elaborate tail-feathers of male peacocks. However, in humans, both sexes exert mate choice.
The relationship between olfaction and MHC
MHC-based sexual selection is known to involve olfactory mechanisms in such vertebrate taxa as fish, mice, humans, primates, birds, and reptiles.[1] At its simplest level, humans have long been acquainted with the sense of olfaction for its use in determining the pleasantness or the unpleasantness of one's resources, food, etc. At a deeper level, it has been predicted that olfaction serves to personally identify individuals based upon the genes of the MHC.[20]
Chemosensation, which is one of the most primitive senses, has evolved into a specialized sensory system. Humans can not only detect, but also assess, and respond to environmental (chemical) olfactory cues—especially those used to evoke behavioral and sexual responses from other individuals, also known as pheromones. Pheromones function to communicate one's species, sex, and perhaps most importantly one's genetic identity. The genes of the MHC provide the basis from which a set of unique olfactory coding develops.[20]
Although it is not known exactly how MHC-specific odors are recognized, it is currently believed that proteins bound to the peptide-binding groove of the MHC may produce the odorant. Each MHC protein binds to a specific peptide sequence, yielding a set of uniquely bound peptide-MHC complexes for each individual. During cellular turnover, the MHC-peptide complex is shed from the cell surface and the fragments are dispensed in bodily fluids such as blood serum, saliva, and urine. Scientists believe that commensal microflora, microorganisms that line epithelial surfaces open to the external environment such as the gastrointestinal tract and vagina, further degrade these fragments, which are made volatile by this process. Recently, it has been shown that receptors in the vomeronasal organ of mice are activated by peptides having similar characteristics to MHC proteins; further studies may hopefully soon clarify the exact transformation between MHC genotype and an olfactory mechanism.[1][20][21]
Empirical evidence
In humans
MHC similarity in humans has been studied in three broad ways: odor, facial attractiveness, and actual mate choice.[22] Studies of odor find MHC-dissimilarity preferences but vary in details, while facial attractiveness favors MHC-similarity and actual mating studies are varied.[22]
Specific studies
Several studies suggest that MHC-related odor preferences and mate choice are demonstrated by humans. However, the role of MHC in human mate choice has been relatively controversial. One study conducted by Ober et al. examined HLA types from 400 couples in the Hutterite community and found dramatically fewer HLA matches between husbands and wives than expected when considering the social structure of their community.[23] On the other hand, there was no evidence of MHC-based mate choice in the same study of 200 couples from South Amerindian tribes.[23]
Other studies have approached mate choice based on odor preference. In one study done by Wedekind et al., women were asked to smell male axillary odors collected on T-shirts worn by different males. Women that were ovulating rated the odors of MHC-dissimilar men as more pleasant than those of the MHC-similar men. Furthermore, odors of MHC-dissimilar men often reminded women of current or former partners, suggesting that odor—specifically odor for MHC-dissimilarity—plays a role in mate choice.[24]
In another study done by Wedekind et al., 121 women and men were asked to rank the pleasantness of the odors of sweaty T-shirts. Upon smelling the shirts, it was found that men and women who were reminded of their own mate or ex-mate had dramatically fewer MHC alleles in common with the wearer than would be expected by chance. If the selection for shirts was not random, and actually selected for MHC-dissimilar alleles, this suggests that MHC genetic composition does influence mate choice. Furthermore, when the degree of similarity between the wearer and the smeller was statistically accounted for, there was no longer a significant influence of MHC on odor preference. The results show that MHC similarity or dissimilarity certainly plays a role in mate choice. Specifically, MHC-disassortative mate choice and less similar MHC combinations are selected for.[25] One interesting aspect of the Wedekind's experiment was that in contrast to normally cycling women, women taking oral contraceptives preferred odors of MHC-similar men. This would suggest that the pill may interfere with the adaptive preference for dissimilarity.[24][25]
In primates
There is evidence of MHC-associated mate choice in other primates. In the grey mouse lemur Microcebus murinus, post-copulatory mate-choice is associated with genetic constitution. Fathers are more MHC-dissimilar from the mother than are randomly tested males. Fathers have more differences in amino acid and microsatellite diversity than did randomly tested males. It is hypothesized that this is caused by female cryptic choice.[26]
In other animals
In mice, both males and females choose MHC-dissimilar partners. Mice develop the ability to identify family members during early growth and are known to avoid inbreeding with kin, which would support the MHC-mediated mate choice hypothesis for inbreeding avoidance.[2]
Fish are another group of vertebrates shown to display MHC-associated mate choice. Scientists tested the Atlantic salmon, Salmo salar, by observing effects of MHC upon natural spawning salmon that resided in the river versus artificial crosses that were carried out in hatcheries. Logically, the artificial crosses would be bereft of the benefits of mate choice that would naturally be available. The results showed that the offspring of the artificially bred salmon were more infected with parasites: almost four times more than the naturally-spawned offspring were. In addition, wild offspring were more MHC-heterozygous than the artificially-bred offspring. These results support the Heterozygous Advantage hypothesis of sexual selection for MHC-dissimilar mate choice.[27] In another fish, the three-spined stickleback, it has been shown that females desire MHC diversity in their offspring, which affects their mate choice.[28]
Female Savannah sparrows, Passerculus sandwichensis, chose MHC-dissimilar males to mate with. Females are more likely to engage in extra-pair relationships if paired with MHC-similar mates and more dissimilar mates are available. Similarly, MHC diversity in house sparrows, Passer domesticus, suggests that MHC-disassortative mate choice occurs.[2]
MHC-mediated mate choice has been shown to exist in Swedish sand lizards, Lacerta agilis. Females preferred to associate with odor samples obtained from males more distantly related at the MHC I loci.[29]
Even though many species are socially monogamous, females can accept or actively seek mating outside of the relationship;[30] extra-pair paternity is a mating pattern known to be affiliated with MHC-associated mate choice. Birds are one of the more commonly studied groups of animals to exhibit this sexual behavior. In the scarlet rosefinch Carpocus erythrinus, females engaged in extra-pair paternity much less frequently when their mates were MHC-heterozygous.[31] In the Seychelles warbler Acrocephalus sechellensis, there was no evidence of MHC variation between social mates. However, when females' social mates were MHC-similar, they were more likely to participate in extra-pair paternity; in most cases, the extra-pair male was significantly more MHC-dissimilar than the social mate.[32]
MHC-mediated mate choice may occur after copulation, at the gametic level, through sperm competition or female cryptic choice. The Atlantic salmon, Salmo salar, is one species in which sperm competition is influenced by the variation in the major histocompatibility complex, specifically that of the Class I alleles. Atlantic salmon males have higher rates of successful fertilization when competing for eggs from females genetically similar at the class I genes of the MHC.[33]
Another species that exhibits MHC-associated cryptic choice is the Arctic charr Salvelinus alpinus. In this case, however, it seems that sperm selection is more dependent on the ovum. MHC-heterozygous males were found to have significantly more fertilization success than MHC-homozygous males; sperm count, motility, and swimming velocity were not shown to significantly co-vary with similarity or dissimilarity at the MHC. It is proposed that there is a chemo-attraction system responsible for the egg itself being able to discriminate and selectively choose between MHC-heterozygous and MHC-homozygous males.[34]
Contrary to the Atlantic salmon and the Arctic char, red junglefowl Gallus gallus males instead of females exert cryptic preference. Male junglefowl showed no preference when simultaneously presented with both an MHC-dissimilar and an MHC-similar female. However, they did show a cryptic preference by allocating more sperm to the more MHC-dissimilar of the two.[35]
Male sand lizards Lacerta agilis behave similarly to the male junglefowl. Initial copulation between a male and a female without any rivals was shown to be extended when the male sensed a higher female fecundity. However, second males adjusted the duration of their copulation depending on the relatedness between the female and the first male, believed to be determined by the MHC-odor of the copulatory plug. A closer genetic relatedness between a male and a female sand lizard increased the chances for a successful fertilization and rate of paternity for the second male.[36]
Abortional selection may be a form of cryptic female choice. Many studies on humans and rodents have found that females may spontaneously abort pregnancies in which the offspring is too MHC-similar.[citation needed] In addition, in vitro fertilizations are more likely to fail when couples have similar MHC genes.[citation needed]
MHC and sexual conflict
If males attempt to thwart female mate choice by mating with a female against her will, sexual conflict may interfere with the choice for compatibility at the MHC genes.
In Chinook salmon Oncorhyncus tshawytscha, females act more aggressively towards MHC-similar males than MHC-dissimilar males, suggesting the presence of female mate choice. Furthermore, males directed aggression at MHC-similar females. This was accompanied by male harassment of unreceptive females; however, there was a positive correlation between male aggression and reproductive success. The ability of the males to over-power the females' original mate choice resulted in the offspring of the targets of male aggression having low genetic diversity. Offspring with high genetic diversity seemed to happen only when the operational sex ratio was female-biased, when females were more likely to be able to exert mate choice, and males were less likely to harass females. These results suggest that sexual conflict may interfere with female mate choice for 'good' MHC genes.[37]
See also
- Body odor
- Body odor and subconscious human sexual attraction
- Pheromone
- The Compatibility Gene
References
- ↑ 1.0 1.1 1.2 "Mate choice decisions of stickleback females predictably modified by MHC peptide ligands". Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 102 (12): 4414–8. March 2005. doi:10.1073/pnas.0408264102. PMID 15755811. Bibcode: 2005PNAS..102.4414M.
- ↑ 2.0 2.1 2.2 2.3 2.4 2.5 "Individual odor recognition in procellariiform chicks: potential role for the major histocompatibility complex". Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1170: 442–6. July 2009. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.03887.x. PMID 19686174.
- ↑ Quéméré, Erwan; Rossi, Sophie; Petit, Elodie; Marchand, Pascal; Merlet, Joël; Game, Yvette; Galan, Maxime; Gilot-Fromont, Emmanuelle (2020-03-10). "Genetic epidemiology of the Alpine ibex reservoir of persistent and virulent brucellosis outbreak" (in en). Scientific Reports 10 (1): 4400. doi:10.1038/s41598-020-61299-2. ISSN 2045-2322. PMID 32157133. Bibcode: 2020NatSR..10.4400Q.
- ↑ Minias, Piotr; Vinkler, Michal (2022-05-01). "Selection Balancing at Innate Immune Genes: Adaptive Polymorphism Maintenance in Toll-Like Receptors". Molecular Biology and Evolution 39 (5): msac102. doi:10.1093/molbev/msac102. ISSN 1537-1719. PMID 35574644. PMC 9132207. https://doi.org/10.1093/molbev/msac102.
- ↑ Morger, Jennifer; Bajnok, Jaroslav; Boyce, Kellyanne; Craig, Philip S.; Rogan, Michael T.; Lun, Zhao-Rong; Hide, Geoff; Tschirren, Barbara (2014-08-01). "Naturally occurring Toll-like receptor 11 (TLR11) and Toll-like receptor 12 (TLR12) polymorphisms are not associated with Toxoplasma gondii infection in wild wood mice" (in en). Infection, Genetics and Evolution 26: 180–184. doi:10.1016/j.meegid.2014.05.032. ISSN 1567-1348. PMID 24910107. https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1567134814002019.
- ↑ 6.0 6.1 Antonides, Jennifer; Mathur, Samarth; Sundaram, Mekala; Ricklefs, Robert; DeWoody, J. Andrew (2019-05-22). "Immunogenetic response of the bananaquit in the face of malarial parasites". BMC Evolutionary Biology 19 (1): 107. doi:10.1186/s12862-019-1435-y. ISSN 1471-2148. PMID 31113360.
- ↑ Cornetti, Luca; Hilfiker, Daniela; Lemoine, Mélissa; Tschirren, Barbara (2018-08-06). "Small-scale spatial variation in infection risk shapes the evolution of aBorreliaresistance gene in wild rodents". Molecular Ecology 27 (17): 3515–3524. doi:10.1111/mec.14812. ISSN 0962-1083. PMID 30040159. https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/mec.14812.
- ↑ Nelson-Flower, Martha J; Germain, Ryan R; MacDougall-Shackleton, Elizabeth A; Taylor, Sabrina S; Arcese, Peter (2018-06-27). "Purifying Selection in the Toll-Like Receptors of Song Sparrows Melospiza melodia". Journal of Heredity 109 (5): 501–509. doi:10.1093/jhered/esy027. ISSN 0022-1503. PMID 29893971.
- ↑ Ilmonen, Petteri; Penn, Dustin J; Damjanovich, Kristy; Morrison, Linda; Ghotbi, Laleh; Potts, Wayne K (2007-08-01). "Major Histocompatibility Complex Heterozygosity Reduces Fitness in Experimentally Infected Mice". Genetics 176 (4): 2501–2508. doi:10.1534/genetics.107.074815. ISSN 1943-2631. PMID 17603099. PMC 1950649. https://doi.org/10.1534/genetics.107.074815.
- ↑ Joe., Demas, Gregory E. Nelson, Randy (2012). Ecoimmunology. Oxford University Press. pp. 238. ISBN 978-0-19-987624-2. OCLC 777401230. http://worldcat.org/oclc/777401230.
- ↑ McClelland, Erin E.; Granger, Donald L.; Potts, Wayne K. (August 2003). "Major Histocompatibility Complex-Dependent Susceptibility to Cryptococcus neoformans in Mice" (in en). Infection and Immunity 71 (8): 4815–4817. doi:10.1128/IAI.71.8.4815-4817.2003. ISSN 0019-9567. PMID 12874366.
- ↑ Takahata, Naoyuki (1994), Golding, Brian, ed., "Polymorphism at MHC Loci and Isolation by the Immune System in Vertebrates" (in en), Non-Neutral Evolution: Theories and Molecular Data (Boston, MA: Springer US): pp. 233–246, doi:10.1007/978-1-4615-2383-3_19, ISBN 978-1-4615-2383-3, https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4615-2383-3_19, retrieved 2022-07-28
- ↑ Nowak, M A; Tarczy-Hornoch, K; Austyn, J M (1992-11-15). "The optimal number of major histocompatibility complex molecules in an individual." (in en). Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 89 (22): 10896–10899. doi:10.1073/pnas.89.22.10896. ISSN 0027-8424. PMID 1438295. Bibcode: 1992PNAS...8910896N.
- ↑ Woelfing, Benno; Traulsen, Arne; Milinski, Manfred; Boehm, Thomas (2009-01-12). "Does intra-individual major histocompatibility complex diversity keep a golden mean?". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 364 (1513): 117–128. doi:10.1098/rstb.2008.0174. PMID 18926972.
- ↑ Lampert, K. P.; Fischer, P.; Schartl, M. (March 2009). "Major histocompatibility complex variability in the clonal Amazon molly, Poecilia formosa : is copy number less important than genotype?" (in en). Molecular Ecology 18 (6): 1124–1136. doi:10.1111/j.1365-294X.2009.04097.x. PMID 19226318. https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1365-294X.2009.04097.x.
- ↑ Šimková, Andrea; Košař, Martin; Vetešník, Lukáš; Vyskočilová, Martina (2013-06-14). "MHC genes and parasitism in Carassius gibelio, a diploid-triploid fish species with dual reproduction strategies". BMC Evolutionary Biology 13 (1): 122. doi:10.1186/1471-2148-13-122. ISSN 1471-2148. PMID 23768177.
- ↑ "Tracking the long-term decline and recovery of an isolated population". Science 282 (5394): 1695–8. November 1998. doi:10.1126/science.282.5394.1695. PMID 9831558. Bibcode: 1998Sci...282.1695W.
- ↑ Schlupp, Ingo; Berbel-Filho, Waldir (2021-06-03). Faculty Opinions recommendation of Meta-analytic evidence that animals rarely avoid inbreeding.. doi:10.3410/f.740048135.793586159.
- ↑ Bernatchez, L.; Landry, C. (May 2003). "MHC studies in nonmodel vertebrates: what have we learned about natural selection in 15 years?" (in en). Journal of Evolutionary Biology 16 (3): 363–377. doi:10.1046/j.1420-9101.2003.00531.x. ISSN 1010-061X. PMID 14635837.
- ↑ 20.0 20.1 20.2 "Odortypes: their origin and composition". Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 96 (4): 1522–5. February 1999. doi:10.1073/pnas.96.4.1522. PMID 9990056. Bibcode: 1999PNAS...96.1522Y.
- ↑ Bhutta MF (June 2007). "Sex and the nose: human pheromonal responses". J R Soc Med 100 (6): 268–74. doi:10.1177/014107680710000612. PMID 17541097. PMC 1885393. http://www.jrsm.org/cgi/pmidlookup?view=long&pmid=17541097.
- ↑ 22.0 22.1 "MHC-correlated mate choice in humans: A review". Psychoneuroendocrinology 34 (4): 497–512. May 2009. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.10.007. PMID 19054623.
- ↑ 23.0 23.1 "Is mate choice in humans MHC-dependent?". PLoS Genet. 4 (9): e1000184. 2008. doi:10.1371/journal.pgen.1000184. PMID 18787687.
- ↑ 24.0 24.1 "MHC-correlated odour preferences in humans and the use of oral contraceptives". Proc. Biol. Sci. 275 (1652): 2715–22. December 2008. doi:10.1098/rspb.2008.0825. PMID 18700206.
- ↑ 25.0 25.1 "Body odour preferences in men and women: do they aim for specific MHC combinations or simply heterozygosity?". Proc. Biol. Sci. 264 (1387): 1471–9. October 1997. doi:10.1098/rspb.1997.0204. PMID 9364787.
- ↑ "Compatibility counts: MHC-associated mate choice in a wild promiscuous primate". Proc. Biol. Sci. 275 (1634): 555–64. March 2008. doi:10.1098/rspb.2007.1433. PMID 18089539.
- ↑ "MHC-mediated mate choice increases parasite resistance in salmon". Proc. Biol. Sci. 275 (1641): 1397–403. June 2008. doi:10.1098/rspb.2008.0066. PMID 18364312.
- ↑ "Major histocompatibility complex diversity influences parasite resistance and innate immunity in sticklebacks". Proc. Biol. Sci. 271 (1535): 197–204. January 2004. doi:10.1098/rspb.2003.2567. PMID 15058398.
- ↑ "Major histocompatibility complex and mate choice in sand lizards". Proc. Biol. Sci. 270 (Suppl 2): S254–6. November 2003. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2003.0079. PMID 14667398.
- ↑ "Reed bunting females increase fitness through extra-pair mating with genetically dissimilar males". Proc. Biol. Sci. 274 (1627): 2865–71. November 2007. doi:10.1098/rspb.2007.0799. PMID 17785270.
- ↑ Promerová Vinkler. Occurrence of extra-pair paternity is connected to social male's MHC-variability in the scarlet rosefinch Carpodacus erythrinus. Journal of Avian Biology 42, 5-10(2011).
- ↑ "MHC-based patterns of social and extra-pair mate choice in the Seychelles warbler". Proc. Biol. Sci. 272 (1564): 759–67. April 2005. doi:10.1098/rspb.2004.3028. PMID 15870038.
- ↑ "Atlantic salmon eggs favour sperm in competition that have similar major histocompatibility alleles". Proc. Biol. Sci. 276 (1656): 559–66. February 2009. doi:10.1098/rspb.2008.1257. PMID 18854296.
- ↑ "MHC and fertilization success in the Arctic charr (Salvelinus alpinus)". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 57 (4): 374–380. 2005. doi:10.1007/s00265-004-0860-z.
- ↑ "Cryptic preference for MHC-dissimilar females in male red junglefowl, Gallus gallus". Proc. Biol. Sci. 276 (1659): 1083–92. March 2009. doi:10.1098/rspb.2008.1549. PMID 19129124.
- ↑ "Fecundity and MHC affects ejaculation tactics and paternity bias in sand lizards". Evolution 58 (4): 906–9. April 2004. doi:10.1554/03-610. PMID 15154566.
- ↑ "Sexual conflict inhibits female mate choice for major histocompatibility complex dissimilarity in Chinook salmon". Proc. Biol. Sci. 277 (1683): 885–94. March 2010. doi:10.1098/rspb.2009.1639. PMID 19864282.
Original source: https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Major histocompatibility complex and sexual selection.
Read more |