Gateway belief model
The gateway belief model (GBM) suggests that public perception of the degree of expert or scientific consensus on an issue functions as a so-called "gateway" cognition.[1][2] Perception of scientific agreement is suggested to be a key step towards acceptance of related beliefs.[3] Increasing the perception that there is normative agreement within the scientific community can increase individual support for an issue. A perception of disagreement may decrease support for an issue.[1][2]
Public opinion research has shown a "consensus gap" between the beliefs of the general public and the scientific community on a number of issues including climate change, vaccines, evolution, gun control, and GMO's.[4][5] The general public is assumed to underestimate the degree of agreement among scientists on established facts relating to these issues.[1]
According to the gateway belief model, views can be influenced by presenting information about the scientific consensus on a subject. Communicating accurate information about the scientific consensus on a topic reduces perceptions that there is disagreement within the scientific community. Some studies show a causal connection between changes in perceived consensus and subsequent attitudes on issues.[1][2] In the case of climate change, perceptions of expert agreement are considered a precursor to related beliefs about whether and why climate change is happening.[2]:130 In the case of COVID-19, perception of scientific consensus predicted personal attitudes and support for mitigation policies.[6]
The gateway belief model also implies that organized disinformation campaigns may be able to deliberately undermine public support for an issue by suggesting a lack of scientific consensus or amplifying opinions that disagree with the scientific consensus.[7] Undermining scientific consensus is therefore a frequent disinformation tactic.[8]
History
Theoretical background
The gateway belief model is a dual process theory in psychology and the communication sciences. Specifically, the GBM postulates a two-step process of opinion change, where (mis)perceptions of normative agreement influence "key" personal beliefs that people hold about an issue (step 1), which in turn, shape public attitudes and support (step 2).[9] Although the basic process of debiasing judgment can be viewed as a form of knowledge deficit,[10] development of the gateway belief model is based on research in cognitive and social psychology, mainly drawing on theories of heuristic information-processing, social norms, decision-making, and motivated cognition.[11][12]
Consensus-heuristic
In the face of uncertainty, people often look to others for guidance, including experts.[11][13] Prior research shows that people heuristically rely on consensus cues in the absence of motivation to cognitively elaborate,[14][15] because consensus typically implies correctness. Research also indicates that people desire to conform to the expert consensus[16] and generally prefer to rely on the combined judgment of multiple experts rather than on individual expert opinions.[17] Relying on consensus cues is often considered socially adaptive because it harnesses the wisdom of the crowd effect.[11][12] Consensus is therefore an example of a descriptive norm, i.e., the collective judgment of a group of individuals, such as experts.[18]
Public opinion research shows that the views of the general public often diverge sharply from experts on a number of important societal issues, especially in the United States.[5] This is known as the "consensus gap".[19] The main premise of the gateway belief model is that this gap can be reduced by highlighting or communicating the actual degree of social or scientific consensus on an issue.[20][21][12]
Norm perception as a vehicle for social change
The basic mechanism of the gateway belief model involves realigning people's (mis)perception of the degree of group consensus with the factual degree of consensus. This parallels research in social psychology on leveraging norm-perception as a vehicle for social change.[22]
For example, early research[23] showed that college students frequently misperceive the social consensus on campus binge drinking. Through a method known as "estimate and reveal", social psychologists have attempted to reveal the discrepancy between students' subjective perceptions of the drinking norm among their peers and the actual norm (which is typically much lower). Social norm communication campaigns indeed evidence that increasing awareness of the actual drinking norm has positive subsequent impacts on students' own attitudes and behavior towards binge drinking.[24]
While excessive binge drinking is often harmful to the individual, large-scale societal misperceptions of scientific agreement on social dilemmas such as climate change or vaccines can be collectively harmful. When the consensus intervention involves experts rather than peers, the social influence process is referred to as obedience.[16]
Role of misinformation
The "sticky" nature of myths and the spread of misinformation is often cited as a major cause of public confusion over the nature of scientific consensus.[25][26][27] Prominent examples include autism-vaccine controversies,[28] the causal link between smoking and lung cancer[29] and the role of carbon dioxide emissions in driving global warming.[30][31]
People's perception of expert consensus has generally shown to be sensitive to anecdotal evidence and misinformation.[7][25][32] Vested-interest groups, sometimes referred to as "merchants of doubt",[31] deliberately try to undermine public understanding of the scientific consensus on these topics through organized disinformation campaigns.[8][7][25][32]
Related concepts
Other related concepts include the false-consensus effect[33] and pluralistic ignorance.
Other theories
The "cultural cognition of scientific consensus" thesis[34] advocated by Dan Kahan stands in contrast to the gateway belief model (GBM)[35] but has not been supported by empirical results.[36] The cultural cognition thesis suggests that people will credit or dismiss empirical evidence based on whether it coheres or conflicts with their cultural or political values, a process known as "identity-protective cognition".[27][35] Because people are committed to the types of beliefs that define their everyday socio-political relations, the cultural cognition thesis predicts that exposing people to consensus information on contested issues will therefore increase attitude polarization.[34]
The empirical results of the gateway belief model contradict the prediction of the "cultural cognition of scientific consensus".[36][37][38] Notably, an emphasis on scientific consensus does not backfire, and can reduce or neutralize belief polarization between (political) groups.[9][39][37][40][25][32] Related research has also shown that conveying scientific agreement can reduce directional motivated reasoning,[36][41] although other research on this topic has revealed more mixed results.[42][43][38]
One explanation for these findings is that changing beliefs about what other groups think (so-called "meta-beliefs") does not require a full and immediate adjustment of one's own worldview. Perceived consensus can therefore be seen as a "non-identity threatening" cognition,[44] especially when a norm is described among a neutral out-group (scientists).[36] Kahan has a notable on-going scholarly debate in the literature with van der Linden and Lewandowsky on the role of perceived consensus and cultural cognition.[27][37][45][46][47]
References
- ↑ 1.0 1.1 1.2 1.3 Gundersen, Torbjørn; Alinejad, Donya; Branch, T.Y.; Duffy, Bobby; Hewlett, Kirstie; Holst, Cathrine; Owens, Susan; Panizza, Folco et al. (17 October 2022). "A New Dark Age? Truth, Trust, and Environmental Science" (in en). Annual Review of Environment and Resources 47 (1): 5–29. doi:10.1146/annurev-environ-120920-015909. ISSN 1543-5938.
- ↑ 2.0 2.1 2.2 2.3 Hulme, Mike (27 November 2019) (in en). Contemporary Climate Change Debates: A Student Primer. Routledge. p. 127-135. ISBN 978-0-429-82115-8. https://books.google.com/books?id=S8TADwAAQBAJ&pg=PA130.
- ↑ Hope, Debra A.; Bevins, Rick A. (26 September 2018) (in en). Change and Maintaining Change. Springer. ISBN 978-3-319-96920-6. https://books.google.com/books?id=ErNwDwAAQBAJ&pg=PA182.
- ↑ Funk, Cary (29 January 2015). "Public and Scientists' Views on Science and Society". https://www.pewresearch.org/science/2015/01/29/public-and-scientists-views-on-science-and-society/.
- ↑ 5.0 5.1 "Major Gaps Between the Public, Scientists on Key Issues". Pew Research Center: Internet, Science & Tech. July 1, 2015. https://www.pewresearch.org/internet/interactives/public-scientists-opinion-gap/.
- ↑ Kerr, John R.; van der Linden, Sander (2022). "Communicating expert consensus increases personal support for COVID‐19 mitigation policies". Journal of Applied Social Psychology 52 (1): 15–29. doi:10.1111/jasp.12827. ISSN 0021-9029. PMID 34511636.
- ↑ 7.0 7.1 7.2 Koehler, D (2016). "Can journalistic "false balance" distort public perception of consensus in expert opinion?". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied 22 (1): 24–38. doi:10.1037/xap0000073. PMID 26752513. https://zenodo.org/record/889605.
- ↑ 8.0 8.1 Lewandowsky, Stephan (1 April 2021). "Climate Change Disinformation and How to Combat It" (in en). Annual Review of Public Health 42 (1): 1–21. doi:10.1146/annurev-publhealth-090419-102409. ISSN 0163-7525. PMID 33355475.
- ↑ 9.0 9.1 van der Linden, Sander; Leiserowitz, Anthony; Feinberg, Geoffrey; Maibach, Edward (2015). "The Scientific Consensus on Human-Caused Climate Change as a Gateway Belief: Experimental Evidence". PLOS ONE 10 (2): e0118489. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0118489. PMID 25714347.
- ↑ Timpona, Joseph (2015). "Battling Misinformation: The Scientific Consensus as a Gateway Belief for Climate Change and GMOs". PLOS ONE. http://blogs.plos.org/thestudentblog/2015/05/15/battling-misinformation-the-scientific-consensus-as-a-gateway-belief-for-climate-change-and-gmos//.
- ↑ 11.0 11.1 11.2 van der Linden, Sander; Lewandowsky, Stephan (2015). "How to Combat Distrust of Science: The Surprising Power of the Psychology of Consensus". Scientific American Mind. http://www.scientificamerican.com/article/how-to-combat-distrust-of-science/.
- ↑ 12.0 12.1 12.2 Maibach, Ed; van der Linden, Sander (2016). "The importance of assessing and communicating scientific consensus". Environmental Research Letters 11 (9): 0913003. doi:10.1088/1748-9326/11/9/091003. Bibcode: 2016ERL....11i1003M.
- ↑ Kelman, H.C. (1961). "Processes of opinion change". Public Opinion Quarterly 25: 57–78. doi:10.1086/266996.
- ↑ Mutz, Diana (1998). Impersonal Influence: How Perceptions of Mass Collectives Affect Political Attitudes. Cambridge University Press. ISBN 9780521637268.
- ↑ Panagopolous, Costas; Harrison, Brian (2016). "Consensus Cues, Issue Salience and Policy Preferences: An Experimental Investigation". North American Journal of Psychology 18 (2): 405–418.
- ↑ 16.0 16.1 Tom, J (2017). "Social Origins of Scientific Deviance: Examining Creationism and Global Warming Skepticism". Sociological Perspectives 61 (3): 341–360. doi:10.1177/0731121417710459.
- ↑ Mannes, A.E.; Soll, J.B.; Larrick, R.P. (2014). "The Wisdom of Select Crowds". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 107 (2): 276–299. doi:10.1037/a0036677. PMID 25090129.
- ↑ Cialdini, Robert; Martin, Steve; Goldstein, Noah (2015). "Small behavioral science informed changes can produce large policy relevant effects". Behavioral Science & Policy 1: 21–27. doi:10.1353/bsp.2015.0008.
- ↑ Cook, John; Jacobs, Peter (2014). "Scientists are from Mars, Laypeople are from Venus: An Evidence-Based Rationale for Communicating the Consensus on Climate". Reports of the National Center for Science Education 34 (6): 3.1. http://reports.ncse.com/index.php/rncse/article/viewFile/347/599. Retrieved 10 July 2016.
- ↑ Hotchkiss, Michael (2015). "Emphasizing consensus about safety boosts support for vaccines". Princeton University News. https://www.princeton.edu/main/news/archive/S45/02/77M41/index.xml?section=topstories.
- ↑ van der Linden, Sander; Leiserowitz, Anthony; Maibach, Edward (2016). "Climate Change's Unseen Consensus". U.S. News & World Report. https://www.usnews.com/opinion/knowledge-bank/articles/2016-02-18/teachers-need-to-communicate-the-scientific-consensus-on-climate-change.
- ↑ Tankard, Margaret; Paluck, Betsy (2016). "Norm perception as a vehicle for social change". Social Issues and Policy Review 10 (1): 181–211. doi:10.1111/sipr.12022.
- ↑ Prentice, Deborah; Miller, Dale (1993). "Pluralistic ignorance and alcohol use on campus: some consequences of misperceiving the social norm". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 64 (2): 243–256. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.64.2.243. PMID 8433272.
- ↑ Haines, Michael; Spear, Sherilynn (1996). "Changing the Perception of the Norm: A Strategy to Decrease Binge Drinking among College Students". American Journal of College Health 45 (3): 134–140. doi:10.1080/07448481.1996.9936873. PMID 8952206.
- ↑ 25.0 25.1 25.2 25.3 van der Linden, Sander; Leiserowitz, Anthony; Rosenthal, Seth; Maibach, Ed (2017). "Inoculating the Public Against Misinformation About Climate Change". Global Challenges 1 (2): 1600008. doi:10.1002/gch2.201600008. PMID 31565263. Bibcode: 2017GloCh...100008V.
- ↑ Lewandowsky, Stephan; Ecker, Ullrich; Seifert, Colleen; Schwarz, Norbert; Cook, John (2012). "Misinformation and its Correction: Continued Influence and Successful Debiasing". Psychological Science in the Public Interest 13 (3): 106–131. doi:10.1177/1529100612451018. PMID 26173286.
- ↑ 27.0 27.1 27.2 Rosenberg, Paul (19 April 2014). "Why climate deniers are winning: The twisted psychology that overwhelms scientific consensus". Salon. http://www.salon.com/2014/04/19/why_climate_deniers_are_winning_the_twisted_psychology_that_overwhelms_scientific_consensus/.
- ↑ Clarke, Chris; Dixon, Graham (2013). "Heightening uncertainty around certain science media coverage, false balance, and the autism-vaccine controversy.". Science Communication 35 (3): 358–382. doi:10.1177/1075547012458290.
- ↑ Cappella, Joseph N.; Maloney, Erin; Ophir, Yotam; Brennan, Emily (2015). "Interventions to Correct Misinformation About Tobacco Products". Tobacco Regulatory Science 1 (2): 186–197. doi:10.18001/TRS.1.2.8. ISSN 2333-9748. PMID 27135046.
- ↑ Pierre, Jeffrey; Neuman, Scott (2021-10-27). "How decades of disinformation about fossil fuels halted U.S. climate policy". https://www.npr.org/2021/10/27/1047583610/once-again-the-u-s-has-failed-to-take-sweeping-climate-action-heres-why.
- ↑ 31.0 31.1 Oreskes, Naomi; Conway, Erik (2010). Merchants of Doubt. Bloomsbury Press. ISBN 978-1-59691-610-4.
- ↑ 32.0 32.1 32.2 Cook, J; Lewandowsky, S; Ecker, U (2017). "Neutralizing misinformation through inoculation: Exposing misleading argumentation techniques reduces their influence". PLOS ONE 12 (5): e0175799. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0175799. PMID 28475576. Bibcode: 2017PLoSO..1275799C.
- ↑ Wilson, Chris (2016). "Most Americans Think Their Opinion on Guns is Widely Shared". Time (magazine). http://time.com/4439610/gun-control-opinion/.
- ↑ 34.0 34.1 Kahan, Dan; Jenkins-Smith, H; Braman, D (2011). "Cultural cognition of scientific consensus". Journal of Risk Research 14 (2): 147–174. doi:10.1080/13669877.2010.511246. https://scholarship.law.gwu.edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=1269&context=faculty_publications.
- ↑ 35.0 35.1 Kahan, Dan (2015). "Climate‐science communication and the measurement problem". Political Psychology 36: 1–43. doi:10.1111/pops.12244.
- ↑ 36.0 36.1 36.2 36.3 van der Linden, S; Leiserowitz, A; Maibach, E (2017). "Scientific agreement can neutralize politicization of facts". Nature Human Behaviour 2 (1): 2–3. doi:10.1038/s41562-017-0259-2. PMID 30980051. https://www.repository.cam.ac.uk/handle/1810/278828.
- ↑ 37.0 37.1 37.2 Mooney, Chris. "How to Convince a Republican: Use a Pie Chart!". Mother Jones. http://www.motherjones.com/environment/2014/07/97-percent-climate-consensus-pie-charts.
- ↑ 38.0 38.1 Dixon, Graham (2016). "Applying the Gateway Belief Model to Genetically Modified Food Perceptions: New Insights and Additional Questions". Journal of Communication 66 (6): 888–908. doi:10.1111/jcom.12260.
- ↑ Lewandowsky, Stephan; Gilles, Gignac; Vaughan, Samuel (2013). "The Pivotal Role of Perceived Scientific Consensus in Acceptance of Science". Nature Climate Change 3 (4): 399–404. doi:10.1038/nclimate1720. Bibcode: 2013NatCC...3..399L.
- ↑ van der Linden, Sander; Leiserowitz, Anthony; Feinberg, Geoffrey; Maibach, Edward (2015). "How to communicate the scientific consensus on climate change: Plain facts, pie charts or metaphors?". Climatic Change 126 (1–2): 255–262. doi:10.1007/s10584-014-1190-4.
- ↑ Bolsen, Toby; Druckman, James (2015). "Counteracting the politicization of science". Journal of Communication 65 (5): 745–769. doi:10.1111/jcom.12171.
- ↑ Cook, John; Lewandowsky, Stephan (2016). "Rational Irrationality: Modeling Climate Change Belief Polarization Using Bayesian Networks". Topics in Cognitive Science 8 (1): 160–179. doi:10.1111/tops.12186. PMID 26749179.
- ↑ Deryugina, T; Shurchkov, O (2016). "The Effect of Information Provision on Public Consensus about Climate Change". PLOS ONE 11 (4): e0151469. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0151469. PMID 27064486. Bibcode: 2016PLoSO..1151469D.
- ↑ van der Linden, Sander (6 May 2016). "Why We Don't Worry More about Climate Change". The Huffington Post. http://www.huffingtonpost.com/researchgate/why-we-dont-worry-more-ab_b_9856402.html.
- ↑ van der Linden, Sander (2016). "A Conceptual Critique of the Cultural Cognition Thesis". Science Communication 38 (1): 128–138. doi:10.1177/1075547015614970.
- ↑ van der Linden, S; Leiserowitz, T; Maibach, E (2017). "Gateway Illusion or Cultural Cognition Confusion?". Journal of Science Communication 16 (5): A04. doi:10.22323/2.16050204. https://jcom.sissa.it/archive/16/05/JCOM_1605_2017_A04.
- ↑ Mooney, Chris. "How Do You Get People to Give a Damn about Climate Change". Mother Jones. http://www.motherjones.com/environment/2013/10/inquiring-minds-kahan-lewandowsky-communicate-climate.
Original source: https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Gateway belief model.
Read more |