Philosophy:Dysrationalia
Dysrationalia is defined as the inability to think and behave rationally despite adequate intelligence.[1] It is a concept in educational psychology and is not a clinical disorder such as a thought disorder. Dysrationalia can be a resource to help explain why smart people fall for Ponzi schemes and other fraudulent encounters.
History
The concept of dysrationalia was first proposed by psychologist Keith Stanovich in the early 1990s. Stanovich originally classified dysrationalia as a learning disability and characterized it as a difficulty in belief formation, in assessing belief consistency, or in the determination of action to achieve one's goals.[2] However, special education researcher Kenneth Kavale noted that dysrationalia may be more aptly categorized as a thinking disorder, rather than a learning disability, because it does not have a direct impact upon academic performance.[3]
Psychologist Robert Sternberg argued that the construct of dysrationalia needed to be better conceptualized since it lacked a theoretical framework (explaining why people are dysrational and how they become this way) and operationalization (how dysrationalia could be measured).[4][5] Sternberg also noted that the concept had the potential for misuse, as one may label another as dysrational simply because he or she does not agree with the other person's view: "I am afraid that Stanovich has fallen into a trap—that of labeling people as 'dysrational' who have beliefs that he does not accept. And therein lies frightening potential for misuse."[4]:23
Stanovich then replied to both Kavale[6] and Sternberg.[7] In response to Sternberg's concern about the construct's potential for misuse, Stanovich said that in that respect it is no different from other constructs such as intelligence, which is a construct that Sternberg himself uses.[7] Stanovich emphasized that use of the dysrationalia construct should be carefully based on rigorous standards of epistemic justification that do not depend solely on social agreement or disagreement and that refer to the process of justifying beliefs, not to the content of beliefs themselves.[7] Stanovich and his colleagues further developed the theoretical framework for, and operationalization of, dysrationalia in later books.
In 2002 Sternberg edited a book, Why Smart People Can Be So Stupid, in which the dysrationalia concept was extensively discussed.[8] In his 2009 book What Intelligence Tests Miss, Stanovich provided the detailed conceptualization that Sternberg called for in his earlier critique.[9] In that book, Stanovich showed that variation in rational thinking skills is surprisingly independent of intelligence. One implication of this finding is that dysrationalia should not be rare.
Mindware
Stanovich proposed two concepts related to dysrationalia: mindware gap and contaminated mindware.[10]
A mindware gap results from gaps in education and experience. This idea focuses on the lack or limitations within a person's knowledge in logic, probability theory, or scientific method when it comes to belief orientation or decision-making. Due to these gaps, intelligent people can make seemingly irrational decisions.
Contaminated mindware focuses on how intelligent people believe irrational ideologies, conspiracy theories, pseudosciences, and/or get-rich-quick schemes. A person can be led into such contaminated mindware through heuristic trust or fallacious reasoning.
Examples
One example that Stanovich related to dysrationalia centers on two former Illinois schoolteachers who pulled their children from the local public school in the area because discussions of the Holocaust are a part of the school's history curriculum.[1]:503 These parents, who are presumably competent due to their college education, believe that the Holocaust is a myth and should not be taught to their children. This is an example of a problem in belief formation regardless of intelligence.
A survey was given to Canadian Mensa club members on the topic of paranormal belief. Mensa members are provided membership strictly because of their high-IQ scores. The survey results showed that 44% of the members believed in astrology, 51% believed in biorhythms, and 56% believed in the existence of extraterrestrial visitors. Stanovich argued that these beliefs have no valid evidence and thus might have been an example of dysrationalia.[1]:503 Sternberg countered that "No one has yet conclusively proven any of these beliefs to be false", so endorsement of the beliefs should not be considered evidence of dysrationalia.[5] Stanovich's rebuttal to Sternberg explained that the purpose of the example was to question the epistemic rationality of the process by which people arrived at their unlikely conclusions, a process of evaluating the quality of arguments and evidence for and against each conclusion, not to assume irrationality based on the content of the conclusion alone.[7]
There are many examples of people who are famous because of their intelligence, but often display irrational behavior. Two examples cited by Stanovich were Martin Heidegger and William Crookes. Heidegger, a renowned philosopher, was also a Nazi apologist and "used the most specious of arguments to justify his beliefs".[1]:503 Crookes, a famous scientist who discovered the element thallium and was a Fellow of the Royal Society, "was repeatedly duped by spiritualist 'mediums' but never gave up his belief in spiritualism".[1]:503 Science journalist David Robson cited the example of Kary Mullis, an American biochemist and 1993 Nobel Prize winner who was also an astrology supporter and a climate change and HIV/AIDS denier.[11]
See also
- Philosophy:Argumentation theory – Study of how conclusions are reached through logical reasoning; one of four rhetorical modes
- Bounded rationality – Making of satisfactory, not optimal, decisions
- Philosophy:Cognitive development – Field of study in neuroscience and psychology
- Cognitive miser – Psychological tendency of people to think and solve problems in simple ways
- Physics:Concept inventory – Knowledge assessment tool
- Medicine:Dyscalculia – Difficulty in learning or comprehending arithmetic
- Medicine:Dyslexia – Specific learning disability characterized by troubles with reading
- Medicine:Dysthymia – Mental disorder characterized by chronic depression
- Educational assessment – Educational evaluation method
- Philosophy:Great Rationality Debate – Question of whether humans are rational or not
- Philosophy:Ignorance – Lack of knowledge and understanding
- Social:Instructional scaffolding – Support given to a student by an instructor throughout the learning process
- Irrationality – Thinking, talking, or acting without inclusion of rationality
- Philosophy:Neurathian bootstrap
- Predictably Irrational – 2008 book by Dan Ariely
- Reflective equilibrium – State of balance among a set of beliefs, arrived at by considering general principles
- Philosophy:Stupidity – Lack of intelligence
Notes
- ↑ 1.0 1.1 1.2 1.3 1.4 Stanovich, Keith E. (October 1993). "Dysrationalia: a new specific learning disability". Journal of Learning Disabilities 26 (8): 501–515. doi:10.1177/002221949302600803. PMID 8245696. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/14952987.
- ↑ Stanovich, Keith E. (May 1994). "An exchange: reconceptualizing intelligence: dysrationalia as an intuition pump". Educational Researcher 23 (4): 11–22. doi:10.3102/0013189X023004011. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/240801398.
- ↑ Kavale, Kenneth A. (October 1993). "How many learning disabilities are there? A commentary on Stanovich's 'Dysrationalia: a new specific learning disability'". Journal of Learning Disabilities 26 (8): 520–523, 567; discussion 524–532. doi:10.1177/002221949302600805. PMID 8245698.
- ↑ 4.0 4.1 Sternberg, Robert J. (May 1994). "What if the construct of dysrationalia were an example of itself?". Educational Researcher 23 (4): 22–27. doi:10.3102/0013189X023004022.
- ↑ 5.0 5.1 Sternberg, Robert J. (October 1993). "Would you rather take orders from Kirk or Spock? The relation between rational thinking and intelligence". Journal of Learning Disabilities 26 (8): 516–519; discussion 524–532. doi:10.1177/002221949302600804. PMID 8245697.
- ↑ Stanovich, Keith E. (October 1993). "It's practical to be rational". Journal of Learning Disabilities 26 (8): 524–532. doi:10.1177/002221949302600806.
- ↑ 7.0 7.1 7.2 7.3 Stanovich, Keith E. (October 1994). "The evolving concept of rationality: a rejoinder to Sternberg". Educational Researcher 23 (7): 33. doi:10.3102/0013189X023007033.
- ↑ Sternberg, Robert J., ed (2002). Why smart people can be so stupid. New Haven: Yale University Press. ISBN 0300090331. OCLC 48098337. https://archive.org/details/whysmartpeopleca0000ster_x0m1.
- ↑ Stanovich, Keith E. (2009). What intelligence tests miss: the psychology of rational thought. New Haven: Yale University Press. ISBN 9780300123852. OCLC 216936066. https://archive.org/details/whatintelligence00stan.
- ↑ Stanovich, Keith E.; Toplak, Maggie E.; West, Richard F. (2008). "The development of rational thought: a taxonomy of heuristics and biases". Advances in Child Development and Behavior 36: 251–285. doi:10.1016/S0065-2407(08)00006-2. ISBN 9780123743176. PMID 18808045.
- ↑ Robson, David G. (February 2019). "The stupidity trap". New Scientist 241 (3218): 30–33. doi:10.1016/S0262-4079(19)30332-X. Bibcode: 2019NewSc.241...30R.
Further reading
- Croskerry, Pat (2015). "Clinical decision making". in Barach, Paul R.; Jacobs, Jeffery P.; Lipshultz, Steven E. et al.. Pediatric and congenital cardiac care: quality improvement and patient safety. 2. London; New York: Springer Verlag. pp. 397–409. doi:10.1007/978-1-4471-6566-8_33. ISBN 9781447165651. OCLC 900507959. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/285937160.
- Facione, Peter A.; Gittens, Carol Ann (2016). Think critically (3rd ed.). Boston: Pearson Education. ISBN 9780133909661. OCLC 893099404.
- Forsythe, Chris; Liao, Huafei; Trumbo, Michael; Cardona-Rivera, Rogelio E. (2015). Cognitive neuroscience of human systems: work and everyday life. Advances in human factors and ergonomics series. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press/Taylor & Francis. ISBN 9781466570573. OCLC 796750072.
- Holyoak, Keith J.; Morrison, Robert G., eds (2012). The Oxford handbook of thinking and reasoning. Oxford library of psychology. Oxford; New York: Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199734689.001.0001. ISBN 9780199734689. OCLC 773023517.
- Kahneman, Daniel (2011). Thinking, fast and slow. New York: Farrar, Straus, and Giroux. ISBN 9780374275631. OCLC 706020998. https://archive.org/details/thinkingfastslow0000kahn.
- Moshman, David (2011). Adolescent rationality and development: cognition, morality, and identity (3rd ed.). New York: Psychology Press. ISBN 9781848728608. OCLC 644680695.
- Moshman, David (October 2000). "Diversity in reasoning and rationality: metacognitive and developmental considerations". Behavioral and Brain Sciences 23 (5): 689–690. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00483433. http://digitalcommons.unl.edu/edpsychpapers/46/.
- Moshman, David (2015). Epistemic cognition and development: the psychology of justification and truth. New York: Psychology Press. ISBN 9781848725133. OCLC 883648773.
- Nisbett, Richard E. (2015). Mindware: tools for smart thinking. New York: Farrar, Straus, and Giroux. ISBN 9780374112677. OCLC 889164994.
- Over, David (February 2010). "Dysrationalia: intelligence without rationality". Trends in Cognitive Sciences 14 (2): 55–56. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2009.11.006.
- Preiss, David; Sternberg, Robert J., eds (2010). Innovations in educational psychology: perspectives on learning, teaching, and human development. New York: Springer Publishing. ISBN 9780826121622. OCLC 316035759.
- Robson, David G. (2019). The intelligence trap: why smart people make dumb mistakes. New York: W. W. Norton & Company. ISBN 9780393651423. OCLC 1054001437.
- Stanovich, Keith E. (2010). "Metarationality: good decision-making strategies are self-correcting". Decision making and rationality in the modern world. Fundamentals of cognition series. Oxford; New York: Oxford University Press. pp. 143–162. ISBN 9780195328127. OCLC 318716093. https://archive.org/details/decisionmakingra00keit.
- Stanovich, Keith E. (November 2009). "Rational and irrational thought: the thinking that IQ tests miss". Scientific American Mind 20 (6): 34–39. doi:10.1038/scientificamericanmind1109-34. http://www.keithstanovich.com/Site/Research_on_Reasoning_files/Stanovich_IQ-Tests-Miss_SAM09.pdf.
- Stanovich, Keith E. (2011). Rationality and the reflective mind. New York: Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195341140.001.0001. ISBN 9780195341140. OCLC 648932780.
- Stanovich, Keith E. (2009). What intelligence tests miss: the psychology of rational thought. New Haven: Yale University Press. ISBN 9780300123852. OCLC 216936066. https://archive.org/details/whatintelligence00stan.
- Stanovich, Keith E. (January 2016). "The Comprehensive Assessment of Rational Thinking". Educational Psychologist 51 (1): 23–34. doi:10.1080/00461520.2015.1125787. http://www.keithstanovich.com/Site/Research_on_Reasoning_files/Stanovich_EdPsy_2016.pdf.
- Stanovich, Keith E.; West, Richard F.; Toplak, Maggie E. (2016). The rationality quotient: toward a test of rational thinking. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. ISBN 9780262034845. OCLC 946254542.
- Walton, Douglas N. (2010). "Why fallacies appear to be better arguments than they are". Informal Logic 30 (2): 159–184. doi:10.22329/il.v30i2.2868. https://informallogic.ca/index.php/informal_logic/article/view/2868.
External links
- Stanovich, Keith E.. "Publications on reasoning and rationality". http://www.keithstanovich.com/Site/Research_on_Reasoning.html.
Original source: https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Dysrationalia.
Read more |