Social:Theory of Regal and Kungic Societal Structures
The Theory of Regal and Kungic Societal Structures, alternatively known as Regality Theory,[1] is an evolutionarily based theory that attempts to explain the cultural dimensions for which members of a social group will select based on the psychological responses resulting from their surrounding environmental or ecological setting. The theory suggests that a harsh or hostile environment will lead to the regal societal structure or regal reaction, while a secure and safe environment is expected to lead to the kungic (kalyptic) societal structure or kungic reaction.[1][2][3] Both of these societal designs may be impacted by other groups designs, and the proximity of one group to another group.[1][2] In addressing these psychological response effects on cultural constructions, the theory applies at the individual-level and social group-level. Precursors to this regality theory are cultural r/k theory[4] and cultural selection theory.[2][5]
Theoretical foundations and conceptual connections
Evolutionary psychology is the major discipline that the regal and kungic society theory falls under. Evolutionary psychology focuses on the psychological adaptations thought to be specific to an entire species that are displayed and observed by individuals of the species of focus[6][7][8] These psychological adaptations are then suggested to explain elements of culture in that they are linked to cultural structure/patterns (social organization) based on the way that the individuals creating and perpetuating the culture are thinking of their surrounding world, which is considered a worldview. The regal and kungic society theory poses that the social group environment triggers psychological adaptations that are meant to address whatever collective dangers come from that environmental setting.[1] The modeling of this regality theory is also attributed to evolutionary game theory and life history theory (in a biological lens).[1][9] It incorporates evolutionary game theory in that the regal and kungic society theory have similar expectations of social behavior, specifically altruism and cooperation. Individuals fighting for the collective social group (altruistic behavior or self-sacrifice) directly refers to the regal dimension of the regal and kungic society theory. Life history theory's connection to this regality theory comes from the shared expectations of reproduction patterns based on ecological settings.[1][10] This also connects the theory of regal and kungic societal structures to the discipline of evolutionary ecology as both theories (regality theory and life history theory) are addressing events that are considered the universal stages/events of life across all organisms- birth, reproduction, death.[2][11]
Agner Fog
Agner Fog is considered the main writer and investigator of regality theory. Fog is attributed with coining the term "regality theory." He is currently an Associate Professor at the Technical University of Denmark (DTU), and has been present at DTU since 1995.[12] In this time, Fog has studied and published works of research that focus on cultural and evolutionary anthropology (includes regality theory), computer science (the field he predominantly lectures), and social systems.[12] Fog has also developed some forms of computer modeling that are meant to model some elements of cultural patterns.[13] He developed this regal and kungic societal patterning theory through the 1990s to the 2010s, but the term "regality theory" is directly used in his 2017 work, Warlike and Peaceful Societies: the Interaction of Genes and Culture.[1] However, first versions of the regal and kungic society theory by Fog are present in his book, Cultural Selection, which was published in 1999. In it, he uses the terms "regal" and "kungic" or "kalyptic" in the identical fashion as his 2017 book.[1][2]
Warlike and peaceful societies: the interaction of genes and culture
This is the book that breaks down the theory of regal and kungic soocietal structures in the greatest detail under the name of "regality theory".[1] This book, published in 2017, is meant to explore the regal and kungic conditions to understand how collective dangers (real or perceived) impact the psychological dispositions of social group members that lead to the preference for conditions of strong leadership and hierarchy or for egalitarian, peaceful social systems.[1][2][3]
Regal society
By this theory of regality, under harsh or hostile environmental conditions, the regal reaction is anticipated. It is the societal pattern that the term "warlike" is referring to. Individuals that live in these settings are likely to prefer an organization where there is a strong leader that is perceived as adequate enough to handle collective danger, whether it is real or perceived.[1][2][14] This leadership based condition would then address what is known as the collective action problem. This problem is understood as the need for all members of the collective to behave cooperatively or altruistically where they "fight" for the collective good no matter the individual level cost, which may be resource loss, injury, and/or death, but there is a failure to do this if group level interest are in conflict with the individual's interests.[15] These individual interests are thought to outweigh the cost and potential benefits of group/joint action. Should leadership employ their centralized power well enough across members of the social group, this collective action problem can be addressed and the potential of free-riding is lessened, which eradicates the occurrence of the Prisoner's Dilemma. The prisoner's dilemma, a concept from game theory (and subsequently evolutionary game theory), refers to the expected strategy choices of individuals in reference to obtaining some form of benefit/payoff(s). In this dilemma, there is the defector (cheater) and the cooperator.[1][2][16] The idea suggests that individuals will choose to behave (defect/cooperate) based on which choice will most likely incur the greatest payoff and/or lowest cost. The idea of the regal condition/response will have an authoritarian figure strong enough to ensure the cooperation of the social group members as it is perceived such cooperation will ensure the defeat of the collective danger, which is typically war or intergroup conflict.[1][17] It is mentioned as well that, in this condition, tyranny becomes possible as the collective is upholding a central power that could, by exploiting the fear of the group interference to the collective danger (real or not), monopolize resources and further their power. The characteristics expected in a society at this regal level would include authoritarianism, hierarchy, xenophobia (intolerance to minorities and out-groups), territoriality, and discipline of high degree.[1][2][3][18]
Authoritarian personality theory
Authoritarian personality theory is equated to regality theory, particularly at the regal level.[1] An authoritarian personality would entail an individual having attitudes depicted as belief of absolute obedience/submission to authority. This is submission is or belief in submission is accomplished via the oppression of those not in power (typical members of the social group) by the centralized authority.[19][17] It is currently understood in two sections based on two identified worldview. The worldview that the world is a dangerous place perpetuates the right-wing authoritarian personality type, while the psychological understanding of the world as a dog-eat-dog world will lead to social dominance orientation.[20] The perception of collective danger and the expected response to such danger described by authoritarian personality theory allow for the connection between this theory and regality theory. The difference between the two and the justification for the consideration of regality theory over authoritarian personality theory is that regality theory allows for greater empirical research via statistical analysis of causal relationships as well as regality theory's better avoidance of politics.[1][2][19][20]
Kungic society
A kungic society is then the resulting community formation from environments of collective safety and general peace,[1] and is considered the opposite condition to the regal condition, which suggests a dichotomous structure. This condition may also be known as the "kalyptic" level. There is little to no expected, perceived, or real collective danger that would trigger psychological adaptations specific to addressing dangers. The security of resources, territory, and within-group peace would result in the psychological dispositions towards an egalitarian system.[1][2] The theory poses that there would be greater equality/equity across the group members at this level and that leadership would be little or at least not centralized as in the regal counterpart. In the kungic society, it is also anticipated that tyranny would not be possible, as the collective would not tolerate an opportunity for a centralized power to exploit resources and members of the group for their own self-enrichment. The characteristics expected for this societal pattern would include an egalitarian system, tolerance of minorities and out-groups (opposite of xenophobia), peacefulness, little separation of "us" and "them", and less specialization.[1][18]
Applications
Methods of study
It is important to note that forms of study used to empirically observe regality of a culture are still developing. The most common form seen is to retroactively attempt to find evidence of the regal and kungic designs on studies already completed.[18] Thus, studies particularly designed with instruments to study regal and kungic are somewhat limited. A study was done by taking data of 186 non-industrial societies in the form of a standard cross-cultural sample and ethnographic records that created a sub-sample set. The study utilized various statistical methods and was retroactively performed on data from explorers of the 19th and 20th centuries.[18] Exploratory factor analysis, structural equation modeling, and multiple correlation analysis were all run on this data and found that cultural variables such as political integration, sex morals, trends in autonomy, class stratification, and high gods, are correlated with intergroup conflict in the directions that regality theory would predicts.[18] Studies that are considered related to this are studies that look that also attempt to isolate the relationship of cultural variables to collective dangers. Variations of factorial analyses appear to be most common statistical method that might be applied to regal and kungic society studies.[18]
There is suggestion that archaeological data may also prove regal and kungic societal structures of ancestral populations.[5][21] Artifacts, by regality theory, reveal in their design whether a researcher is looking at a piece from a kungic society or a regal society. This connection is considered exemplified by the organization of a design. If the design appears uniform and orderly, then is it likely from a regal society in that this condition would have higher value for these styles of detail- this is if one is to believe that identify may be derived form artifact. An artifact of a kungic society might then have design that is more abstract, disorderly, and lacking uniform.[5]
War and intergroup conflict response
Intergroup conflict and war are the major evolutionary factors thought to make up the collective dangers a social group might encounter.[1][2] This makes them two evolutionary factors very relevant to regality theory. War and intergroup conflict response is what the psychological dispositions that select for a regal society are theorized to be meant for.[1] A major objective of studying regal and kungic (kalyptic) responses is that one might be able to isolate universal conditions leading to war and intergroup conflict and the needed conditions for peace-building and peace-keeping.[22][23][24][25] This real-world application connects this regality theory to political psychology.
Regal/Kungic social patterning in non-human species
Considering the study of regality theory among real world human communities is relatively new, studies of regal and kungic level societies amongst non-humans remains basically non-existent and potentially more contentious than at the human level.[1][3] It would involve utilizing a definition of culture that is not human specific. Defining culture remains a contentious topic across the whole of social sciences. The empirical compromise has been studying patterns of social organization across non-human species that doesn't necessarily require definitive use of a culture definition that allows for the inclusion of non-human species.[26] Still, this is based on regal-like and kungic-like behaviors and this does not address the needed psychological flexibility for regality theory and culture as a whole.
Studies of the social structures of non-human primates, some social carnivores, and hymenoptera may show regal and kungic societal patterns. Non-human primates, as human's closest relative, are subject to the most studies to observe "human-unique" characteristics in non-human species as a whole. Chimpanzees and bonobos make the most popular example of non-human species potentially displaying regal and kungic societal patterns, though studies of their social organization were not necessarily targeting evidence of regality theory.[27][28] Chimpanzees typically exist in smaller temporary co-ed groups of males and females where males have rank of high or low, which impacts mating opportunity.[29] Aggressive behavior is observed amongst chimpanzees with some authoritative characteristics which allows for their consideration as regal society types.[1][2] Bonobos are observed to have a kungic society based on kungic-like societal expression. Like chimpanzees, they exist in co-ed groups and follow a fission-fusion pattern. In these groups, females are usually associated with rank and aggressive behavior between individuals is rare for this species. Unlike chimpanzees, the infants and young of the group are more so tolerated by the group males[30] These egalitarian patterns allow for their consideration as a kungic type.[2]
Social carnivores like wolves, lions, meerkats & dolphins have also been subject to lengthy recordings of their social organization.[31][26] Wolves and lions are considered to follow regal-like societal structures.[5] Meerkats, however, hold an odd placing. They are known to share burrowing space with other burrowing species harmoniously, though meerkat communities also have an alpha-pair that will scent-mark subordinate group members' space to assert dominance/authority. Also, there is killing of young in these communities, often by the subordinate members on the alpha pair's young.[26][32][33] This is meant to increase fitness and opportunity of the subordinate young. Their tolerance of outgroups hinders their direct fit into the regal or kungic society theory dynamics.[1][2]
Dolphins are also of interest in that they are one of the few non-human species that are considered to show signs of culture.[26][34][35] This is based on their adults teaching young to make and use tools, which is considered cultural transmission of learned behaviors. They vary in behaviors based on species and environment so deciding if they follow a kungic-like of regal-like societal pattern cannot be universally applied. However, they are all social, form strong social bonds and typically cooperative.[36]
Hymenoptera refers to insects such as bees, wasps and ants. They are considered to be the species that perfected the police state social system,[37] and thus they are prime examples of the regal societal structure.[26][37][19] In these groups, there is authoritarian structure- queens, workers, soldiers- and often aggressive behavior that is lethal to individuals that do not fit into the collective.[37]
Critique of regal and kungic societal structures theory
The shortcomings of regality theory include its relative newness and that the study of the theory seems highly specialized to Agner Fog, though there are kin theories of cultural dimensions and organization.[3][2][18][38] In several theories of cultural design there is some question as to this theory being potentially misused in negative explanations of some cultures/groups of people. The potential marginalization of people groups is compounded by the dichotomous design of the theory. Like the argument of personality as either states or traits, regality theory poses that cultures/societies are a collection of traits and that they create only two levels- regal/warlike or kungic/peaceful.[39][40] It suggests two general worldviews when there is no evidence found to suggest that all humans only think in these two ways.
See also
- Evolutionary psychology and culture
- Evolutionary ecology
- Evolutionary game theory
- Life history theory
- Cultural selection theory
- Authoritarian personality
References
- ↑ 1.00 1.01 1.02 1.03 1.04 1.05 1.06 1.07 1.08 1.09 1.10 1.11 1.12 1.13 1.14 1.15 1.16 1.17 1.18 1.19 1.20 1.21 1.22 1.23 Warlike and Peaceful Societies : the Interaction of Genes and Culture.. Fog, Agner. (1 ed.). UK. ISBN 9781783744039. OCLC 1012158524.
- ↑ 2.00 2.01 2.02 2.03 2.04 2.05 2.06 2.07 2.08 2.09 2.10 2.11 2.12 2.13 2.14 2.15 Fog, Agner. (1999). Cultural Selection. Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands. ISBN 9789401592512. OCLC 851368170.
- ↑ 3.0 3.1 3.2 3.3 3.4 "An Evolutionary Theory of Cultural Differentiation". https://www.researchgate.net/publication/245021446.
- ↑ "Memetics and Social Contagion: Two Sides of the Same Coin?". Journal of Memetics 2 (2): 171–185. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/279556688. Retrieved 2019-05-04.
- ↑ 5.0 5.1 5.2 5.3 "A Defense of Cultural Selection Theory". https://www.researchgate.net/publication/318743666.
- ↑ Tooby, John (1989). "ScienceDirect". Ethology and Sociobiology 10: 29–49. doi:10.1016/0162-3095(89)90012-5.
- ↑ Blonder, Lee (September 1993). "The Adapted Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and the Generation of Culture . Jerome H. Barkow, Leda Cosmides, John Tooby.". American Anthropologist 95 (3): 777–778. doi:10.1525/aa.1993.95.3.02a00690. ISSN 0002-7294.
- ↑ Barkow, Jerome H. (1992). The adapted mind : evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture. ISBN 9780195101072. OCLC 1040141738. https://archive.org/details/adaptedmindevolu0000unse.
- ↑ Kaplan, Hillard S.; Gangestad, Steven W. (2015-09-08), "Life History Theory and Evolutionary Psychology", The Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology (John Wiley & Sons, Inc.): pp. 68–95, doi:10.1002/9780470939376.ch2, ISBN 9780470939376
- ↑ Flatt, Thomas; Heyland, Andreas, eds (2011-05-12). Mechanisms of Life History Evolution. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199568765.001.0001. ISBN 9780199568765.
- ↑ Ahlström, T. (2010-01-13). "Life-history theory, past human populations and climatic perturbations". International Journal of Osteoarchaeology 21 (4): 407–419. doi:10.1002/oa.1147. ISSN 1047-482X.
- ↑ 12.0 12.1 "Agner Fog - DTU". https://www.dtu.dk/english/service/phonebook/person?id=78696&tab=2&qt=dtupublicationquery.
- ↑ Fog, Agner (November 2000). "Simulation of Evolution in Structured Populations: The Open Source Software Package Altruist". Biotech Software & Internet Report 1 (5): 226–229. doi:10.1089/152791600750034767. ISSN 1527-9162.
- ↑ "Perceived Danger Predicts Cultural Variables: A Meta-analysis of Culture Dimensions". https://www.researchgate.net/publication/330968146.
- ↑ Brown, Garrett Wallace; McLean, Iain; McMillan, Alistair (2018). The concise Oxford dictionary of politics and international relations.. Brown, Garrett Wallace,, McLean, Iain,, McMillan, Alistair (4th ed.). [Oxford]. ISBN 9780191749568. OCLC 1020773939.
- ↑ Stirling, Wynn C. (2003), "Game Theory Basics", Satisficing Games and Decision Making, Cambridge University Press, pp. 219–222, doi:10.1017/cbo9780511543456.013, ISBN 9780511543456
- ↑ 17.0 17.1 Spisak, Brian R.; O'Brien, Michael J.; Nicholson, Nigel; van Vugt, Mark (April 2015). "Niche Construction and the Evolution of Leadership". Academy of Management Review 40 (2): 291–306. doi:10.5465/amr.2013.0157. ISSN 0363-7425. https://digitalcommons.tamusa.edu/hist_faculty/26.
- ↑ 18.0 18.1 18.2 18.3 18.4 18.5 18.6 Fog, Agner (2014-02-17). Why are cultures warlike or peaceful? Test of regality theory on 186 non-industrial societies. MISC. OCLC 958611577.
- ↑ 19.0 19.1 19.2 Bunzel, Joseph H.; Adorno, T. W.; Frenkel-Brunswik, Else; Levinson, Daniel J.; Sanford, R. Nevitt; Bettelheim, Bruno; Janowitz, Morris; Ackerman, Nathan W. et al. (August 1950). "The Authoritarian Personality.". American Sociological Review 15 (4): 571. doi:10.2307/2087322. ISSN 0003-1224.
- ↑ 20.0 20.1 Duckitt, John; Sibley, Chris G. (2009-08-25). "A Dual-Process Motivational Model of Ideology, Politics, and Prejudice". Psychological Inquiry 20 (2–3): 98–109. doi:10.1080/10478400903028540. ISSN 1047-840X. PMID 21039534.
- ↑ Jacobs, Donald Trent, 1946- (2006). Unlearning the language of conquest : scholars expose anti-Indianism in America : deceptions that influence war and peace, civil liberties, public education, religion and spirituality, democratic ideals, the environment, law, literature, film, and happiness. University of Texas Press. ISBN 0292706545. OCLC 62525141.
- ↑ Fry, Douglas P., 1953- (2006). The human potential for peace : an anthropological challenge to assumptions about war and violence. Oxford University Press. ISBN 0195181778. OCLC 56921107. https://archive.org/details/humanpotentialfo00doug.
- ↑ Fry, Douglas P; Souillac, Geneviève (2017-03-02). "The Original Partnership Societies: Evolved Propensities for Equality, Prosociality, and Peace". Interdisciplinary Journal of Partnership Studies 4 (1). doi:10.24926/ijps.v4i1.150. ISSN 2380-8969.
- ↑ Kelly, Raymond (2000). Warless Societies and the Origin of War. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press. doi:10.3998/mpub.11589. ISBN 9780472097388.
- ↑ Harvey, Frank P. (March 2000). "Primordialism, Evolutionary Theory and Ethnic Violence in the Balkans: Opportunities and Constraints for Theory and Policy". Canadian Journal of Political Science 33 (1): 37–65. doi:10.1017/S0008423900000032. ISSN 1744-9324.
- ↑ 26.0 26.1 26.2 26.3 26.4 Laland, Kevin N.; Hoppitt, William (2003-05-19). "Do animals have culture?". Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews 12 (3): 150–159. doi:10.1002/evan.10111. ISSN 1060-1538.
- ↑ Van Lawick-Goodall, Jane (January 1968). "The Behaviour of Free-living Chimpanzees in the Gombe Stream Reserve". Animal Behaviour Monographs 1: 161–IN12. doi:10.1016/s0066-1856(68)80003-2. ISSN 0066-1856.
- ↑ Pepper, John W.; Mitani, John C.; Watts, David P. (1999). International Journal of Primatology 20 (5): 613–632. doi:10.1023/a:1020760616641. ISSN 0164-0291. https://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/2027.42/44563/1/10764_2004_Article_409685.pdf.
- ↑ Altmann, J. (1988-05-20). "Primate Societies. Barbara B. Smuts, Dorothy L. Cheney, Robert M. Seyfarth, Richard W. Wrangham, and Thomas T. Struhsaker, Eds. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, IL, 1987. xii, 578 pp., illus. $70; paper, $27.50". Science 240 (4855): 1076–1078. doi:10.1126/science.240.4855.1076-a. ISSN 0036-8075. PMID 17731746.
- ↑ Great ape societies. McGrew, W. C. (William Clement), 1944-, Marchant, Linda F. (Linda Frances), 1951-, Nishida, Toshisada, 1941-2011., 西田, 利貞(1941- ). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 1996. ISBN 0521554942. OCLC 33104578.
- ↑ Garfield, Zachary H.; von Rueden, Christopher; Hagen, Edward H. (2019). "ScienceDirect". The Leadership Quarterly 30: 59–80. doi:10.1016/j.leaqua.2018.09.001.
- ↑ Murray, Martin J (2013). "Development Dilemmas in Post-Apartheid South Africa ed. by Bill Freund and Harald Witt". Transformation: Critical Perspectives on Southern Africa 83 (1): 119–121. doi:10.1353/trn.2013.0021. ISSN 1726-1368. https://www.semanticscholar.org/paper/f1bb0f4303c92201e358a25a2dbde910bc6f9c2c.
- ↑ Moore, Heidi, 1976- (2004). A mob of meerkats. Chicago, Ill.: Heinemann Library. ISBN 1403446946. OCLC 52418369. https://archive.org/details/mobofmeerkats00moor.
- ↑ Hooper, Rowan (September 2011). "Dolphins appear to grieve in different ways". New Scientist 211 (2828): 10. doi:10.1016/s0262-4079(11)62128-3. ISSN 0262-4079. Bibcode: 2011NewSc.211R..10H.
- ↑ "Reason, John, (died 9 Feb. 2007), journalist and author; Rugby Union correspondent, Daily Telegraph and Sunday Telegraph, 1964–94; Director, Rugby Football Books Ltd, since 1971", Who Was Who, Oxford University Press, 2007-12-01, doi:10.1093/ww/9780199540884.013.u32041
- ↑ Díaz López, Bruno; Methion, Séverine (2017-03-18). "The impact of shellfish farming on common bottlenose dolphins' use of habitat". Marine Biology 164 (4). doi:10.1007/s00227-017-3125-x. ISSN 0025-3162. https://www.semanticscholar.org/paper/efd9b0ac16607c9d4f7d4bab331bcac8fbf19f91.
- ↑ 37.0 37.1 37.2 Whitfield, John (2002-04-01). "The police state". Nature 416 (6883): 782–784. doi:10.1038/416782a. ISSN 1476-4687. PMID 11976646. https://www.semanticscholar.org/paper/b5d0fc490b0c0a48615cd4244a6ca1ea8b7f647c.
- ↑ Nardon, Luciara; Steers, Richard M. (2009), "The culture theory jungle: divergence and convergence in models of national culture", Cambridge Handbook of Culture, Organizations, and Work, Cambridge University Press, pp. 3–22, doi:10.1017/cbo9780511581151.002, ISBN 9780511581151
- ↑ Kandler, Christian (2017). "Open Peer Commentary and Authors' Response: Personality Traits and States: Vague Dimensional Differentiation or Genuine Integration". European Journal of Personality 31 (5): 529–595 [549–550]. doi:10.1002/per.2128. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/320741020. Retrieved 2019-05-04.
- ↑ Chaplin, William F.; John, Oliver P.; Goldberg, Lewis R. (1988). "Conceptions of states and traits: Dimensional attributes with ideals as prototypes.". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 54 (4): 541–557. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.54.4.541. ISSN 0022-3514. PMID 3367279.
Original source: https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Theory of Regal and Kungic Societal Structures.
Read more |