Biology:Vervet monkey

From HandWiki
Revision as of 09:44, 13 February 2024 by LinuxGuru (talk | contribs) (update)
(diff) ← Older revision | Latest revision (diff) | Newer revision → (diff)
Short description: Species of Old World monkey

Vervet monkey[1]
Black faced vervet monkey.jpg
Adult male vervet monkey
File:Vervet Monkey (Chlorocebus pygerythrus) (W CERCOPITHECUS AETHIOPS R2 C2).ogg
Calls recorded at Lake Naivasha, Kenya
Scientific classification edit
Domain: Eukaryota
Kingdom: Animalia
Phylum: Chordata
Class: Mammalia
Order: Primates
Suborder: Haplorhini
Infraorder: Simiiformes
Family: Cercopithecidae
Genus: Chlorocebus
Species:
C. pygerythrus
Binomial name
Chlorocebus pygerythrus
F. Cuvier, 1821
Vervet Monkey area.png
     Native Range

The vervet monkey (Chlorocebus pygerythrus), or simply vervet, is an Old World monkey of the family Cercopithecidae native to Africa.[3] The term "vervet" is also used to refer to all the members of the genus Chlorocebus. The five distinct subspecies can be found mostly throughout Southern Africa, as well as some of the eastern countries. These mostly herbivorous monkeys have black faces and grey body hair color, ranging in body length from about 40 cm (16 in) for females, to about 50 cm (20 in) for males.

In addition to behavioral research on natural populations, vervet monkeys serve as a nonhuman primate model for understanding genetic and social behaviors of humans. They have been noted for having human-like characteristics, such as hypertension, anxiety, and social and dependent alcohol use.[4] Vervets live in social groups ranging from 10 to 70 individuals, with males moving to other groups at the time of sexual maturity. Studies done on vervet monkeys involve their communication and alarm calls, specifically in regard to kin and group recognition, and particular predator sightings.

Taxonomy

The vervet monkey was previously classified as Cercopithecus aethiops, now renamed 'grivet', and reclassified as Chlorocebus. The vervet and malbrouck have also been considered conspecific, or as subspecies of a widespread Ch. aethiops.[5] The different taxa are distinguished by coat colour and other morphological characteristics. The characteristics of Ch. aethiops graduate into Ch. pygerythrus where their ranges meet, and thus deciding if the vervets commonly known to occur in Kenya are actually Ch. aethiops is difficult; animals in the same pack may be classified as one species or the other, and Ch. pygerythrus may also interbreed with Ch. tantalus where their ranges meet.[6]

Colin Groves recognised the below five subspecies of vervet monkey in the third edition of Mammals of the World:[1]

  • Chlorocebus pygerythrus excubitor
  • Ch. p. hilgerti from southern Kenya[7]
  • Ch. p. nesiotes
  • Ch. p. pygerythrus from South Africa , Botswana,[7] Lesotho, and Eswatini.[8]
  • Ch. p. rufoviridis from Mozambique[7] and Uganda has a distinctly reddish-coloured back, which is darker towards the base of the tail.[9]

Groves used Ch. p. hilgerti for all East African vervets except the insular subspecies Ch. p. excubitor and Ch. p. nesiotes. The name Ch. p. centralis has been suggested to have precedence, and that Ch. p. hilgerti should be restricted to the population of southern Ethiopia.[10]

Synonymy

Ch. p. pygerythrus, as Cercopithecus aethiops, was also formerly divided into four subspecies:

  • C. a. pygerythrus, from South Africa (Western Cape, Eastern Cape, Northern Cape and southern KwaZulu-Natal), and Lesotho; is said to have pale-colored limbs and white hands and feet (though the hands are also said to be black with a scattering of greyish hairs),[9] and a greyish body colour with an olive sheen.[8]
  • C. a. cloetei, from northern KwaZulu-Natal, Eswatini, and northern South Africa; is darker, with greyish-brown speckles and dark feet.[8]
  • C. a. marjoriae, from southern Botswana and the North West Province of South Africa; is pale in colour (light ash-grey).[8]
  • C. a. ngamiensis,[11] from north-eastern Botswana and the Okavango; has pale feet and a yellowish back[8] and the tail is darker (especially towards the tip) than in other southern vervets.[9]

These subspecies are no longer recognised and are synonymous with Ch. p. pygerythrus.

Distribution and habitat

Natural habitat

The vervet monkey ranges throughout much of Southern and East Africa, being found from Ethiopia and extreme southern South Sudan all the way down to South Africa . It is not found west of the East African Rift or the Luangwa River,[1] where it is replaced by the closely related malbrouck (C. cynosuros) species. The vervet monkey inhabits savanna, riverine woodland, coastal forest, and mountains up to 4000 m (13,100 ft). They are adaptable and able to persist in secondary and/or highly fragmented vegetation, including cultivated areas, and sometimes are found living in both rural and urban environments.[2] Annual home range size has been observed to be as high as 176 ha, with an average population density of 54.68 animals/km².

Introduced

Vervet monkeys that are naturalised (introduced by humans) are found in Cape Verde, Ascension Island, Saint Helena, St Kitts and Nevis, and Barbados.[12] Dania Beach, Florida, is home to about 40 introduced vervets.[13][14]

Physical description

The vervet monkey very much resembles a gray langur, having a black face with a white fringe of hair, while its overall hair color is mostly grizzled-grey.[15][16] The species exhibits sexual dimorphism; the males are larger in weight and body length and may be recognized by a turquoise-blue scrotum. Adult males weigh between 3.9 and 8.0 kg (8.6 and 17.6 lb), averaging 5.5 kg (12 lb), and have a body length between 420 and 600 mm (17 and 24 in), averaging 490 mm (19 in) from the top of the head to the base of the tail. Adult females weigh between 3.4 and 5.3 kg (7.5 and 11.7 lb) and average 4.1 kg (9.0 lb), and measure between 300 and 495 mm (11.8 and 19.5 in), averaging 426 mm (16.8 in).[9][17]

Behaviour

Social behaviour

A vervet monkey grooms another in Gaborone, Botswana
Juvenile C. p. rufoviridis, Uganda

When males reach sexual maturity, they move to a neighboring group.[18] Often, males move with a brother or peer, presumably for protection against aggression by males and females of the resident group. Groups that had previously transferred males show significantly less aggression upon the arrival of another male. In almost every case, males migrate to adjacent groups. This obviously increases benefits in regard to distance traveled, but also reduces the amount of genetic variance, increasing the likelihood of inbreeding.[19]

Females remain in their groups throughout life. Separate dominance hierarchies are found for each sex. Male hierarchies are determined by age, tenure in the group, fighting abilities, and allies, while female hierarchies are dependent on maternal social status. A large proportion of interactions occurs between individuals that are similarly ranked and closely related. Between unrelated individuals, female competition exists for grooming members of high-ranking families, presumably to gain more access to resources. These observations suggest individual recognition is possible and enables discrimination of genetic relatedness and social status. Interactions between different groups are variable, ranging from highly aggressive to friendly. Furthermore, individuals seem to be able to recognise cross-group vocalisations, and identify from and to which monkey each call is intended, even if the call is made by a subadult male, which is likely to transfer groups. This suggests the members within a group are actively monitoring the activity of other groups, including the movement of individuals within a group.[20]

Within groups, aggression is directed primarily at individuals that are lower on the hierarchy. Once an individual is three years or older, it is considerably more likely to be involved in conflict. Conflict often arises when one group member shows aggression toward a close relative of another. Further, both males and females may redirect aggression towards individuals in which both had close relatives that were previously involved in a conflict. This suggests complex recognition not only of individuals, but also of associations between individuals. This does not suggest recognition of other's individual kinship bonds is possible, but rather that discrimination of social relationships does occur.[21]

Female and juvenile, Kruger Park, South Africa

Alarm calls and offspring recognition

Vervet monkeys have four confirmed predators: leopards, eagles, pythons, and baboons. The sighting of each predator elicits an acoustically distinct alarm call.[22] As infants, vervets learn to make the variety of calls from observation alone, without explicit tutelage. In experimentation with unreliable signalers, individuals became habituated to incorrect calls from a specific individual. Though the response was lessened for a specific predator, if an unreliable individual gives an alarm call for a different predator, group members respond as if the alarm caller is, in fact, reliable. This suggests vervet monkeys are able to recognize and to respond to not only the individual calling, but also to the semantics of what the individual is communicating.[23] Vervet monkeys are thought to have up to 30 different alarm calls. In the wild, they have been seen giving a different call when seeing a human being approaching, leading researchers to believe that vervet monkeys may have a way of distinguishing between different land and flight predators.[24]

Mothers can recognise their offspring by a scream alone. A juvenile scream elicits a reaction from all mothers, yet the juvenile's own mother has a shorter latency in looking in the direction of the scream, as well as an increased duration in her look. Further, mothers have been observed to help their offspring in conflict, yet rarely aid other juveniles. Other mothers evidently can determine to which mother the offspring belongs. Individuals have been observed to look towards the mother whose offspring is creating the scream.[25]

Kin relationships

Siblings likely provide the prevailing social relationships during development. Within social groups, mother-offspring and sibling interactive units are distinct groups. The sibling interactions are heavily supportive and friendly, but do have some competition. Contests primarily involve postweaning resource allocation by the common mother. For example, siblings have conflict over grooming time allocated by their mother. Offspring are usually not born in extremely close time proximity due to the interbirth period of the mother. This time can be reduced by use of an allomother. The clarity of the familial and sibships within a group may act as a form of alliance, which would come at relatively low cost in regards to grooming. Other alliances are shown through conflict with aggressive individuals that have acted against a closely related sibling.[26]

Vervet monkey female with a baby

Allomothering is the process when another individual besides the mother cares for an infant. In groups of vervet monkeys, infants are the target of a tremendous amount of attention. Days after an infant is born, every member of the group inspects the infant at least once by touching or sniffing. While all group members participate in infant caretaking, juvenile females that cannot yet menstruate are responsible for the majority of allomothering. The benefit is mutual for the mother and allomother. Mothers that use allomothers are able to shorten their interbirth periods, the time between successive births. At the same time, allomothers gain experience in rearing infants, and had more success in raising their own offspring. Juvenile females discriminate in preference for the infant they choose to allomother, and usually choose siblings or infants of high-ranking individuals. When a mother allows her juvenile daughter to become an allomother for a newborn sibling, the mother decreases her own investment in the infant, while increasing the chances of successful rearing of her immature daughter.[27]

Grandmothers and grandchildren share one-quarter of their genes, so they should be more likely to form affiliative relationships than unrelated members in a group. Not only do infants approach their grandmothers more often than unrelated members, but they also prefer their grandmothers compared to other adult female kin, not including their own mothers. Additional research has shown grandmothers show no preference over the sex of their grandchild. Interest in the grandchild spurred from the rank of the grandmother within a group. Higher-ranking grandmothers showed more interest in caring for their grandchildren when compared to low-ranking grandmothers. The presence of grandmothers has been associated with a decrease in mortality of infants.[28]

Spite

Spiteful actions are extremely rare in the animal kingdom. Often, an indirect benefit is gained by the individual acting 'spitefully', or by a close relative of that individual. Vervet monkeys have been observed to destroy a competitor's food source rather than consume or steal it themselves. While energy is being lost on destroying the food, an advantage is obtained by the individual due to an increase in competitive gain. This would be pertinent for a male that could be displaced within his group by immigrating males.[29]

Reproduction

Female vervets do not have external signs indicating estrus, thus elaborate social behaviors involving reproduction do not occur. Typically, a female gives birth once a year, between September and February, after a gestation period around 165 days. Usually, only one infant is born at a time, though twins can occur rarely. A normal infant weighs 300–400 grams (11–14 oz).[9]

Ecology

Diet

The vervet monkey eats a primarily herbivorous diet, living mostly on wild fruits, flowers, leaves, seeds, and seed pods. In agricultural areas, vervets become problem animals, as they raid bean, pea, young tobacco, vegetable, fruit, and grain crops. Animal foods of their diet include grasshoppers and termites. Raids of cattle egrets and weaver bird nests have been observed where the vervets eat the eggs and chicks.[9]

A list of some natural food plants and part of the plant eaten, in South Africa:[9][30]


Relationship with humans

An infant vervet monkey, South Africa

The monkeys are used for biomedical research.[31] Many people living in close proximity to vervet colonies see them as pests, as they steal their food. Heavy fines in some areas discourage the killing of vervet monkeys.[32]

Its status according to the IUCN is "least concern".

This species was known in ancient Egypt, including the Red Sea Mountains and the Nile Valley.[33] From fresco artworks found in Akrotiri on the Mediterranean island of Santorini there is evidence that the vervet monkey was known to the inhabitants of this settlement around 2000 BC; this fact is most noted for evidence of early contact between Egypt and Akrotiri.[34] Excavations dated to the end of the 1st century AD from Berenike, a Roman-Egyptian port-town on the Red Sea coast, demonstrate that vervet monkeys must have been kept as pets at that time.[35]

Gallery

References

  1. 1.0 1.1 1.2 Groves, C.P. (2005). "Chlorocebus pygerythrus". in Wilson, D.E.; Reeder, D.M.. Mammal Species of the World: A Taxonomic and Geographic Reference (3rd ed.). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press. p. 159. ISBN 0-801-88221-4. OCLC 62265494. http://www.departments.bucknell.edu/biology/resources/msw3/browse.asp?id=12100508. 
  2. 2.0 2.1 Butynski, T.M.; de Jong, Y.A. (2019). "Chlorocebus pygerythrus". IUCN Red List of Threatened Species 2019: e.T136271A17957823. doi:10.2305/IUCN.UK.2019-3.RLTS.T136271A17957823.en. https://www.iucnredlist.org/species/136271/17957823. Retrieved 19 November 2021. 
  3. "Vervet Monkey (Chlorocebus pygerythrus)" (in en). https://www.inaturalist.org/taxa/68137-Chlorocebus-pygerythrus. 
  4. Palmour, R.; Mulligan, J.; Howbert, J. J.; Ervin, F. (1997). "Of monkeys and men: vervets and the genetics of human-like behaviors". American Journal of Human Genetics 61 (3): 481–488. doi:10.1086/515526. PMID 9326311. 
  5. Kingdon, J. (1997). The Kingdom Guide to African Mammals. Academic Press Limited, London. ISBN 0-12-408355-2. https://archive.org/details/kingdonfieldguid00jona. 
  6. de Jong, Y.; Butynski, T. (14 January 2015). "A rainbow of savanna monkeys". National Geographic. https://blog.nationalgeographic.org/2015/01/14/primate-discoveries-in-northwest-kenya/. 
  7. 7.0 7.1 7.2 Biodiversity occurrence data provided by: Field Museum of Natural History, Museum of Vertebrate Zoology, University of Washington Burke Museum, and University of Turku (Accessed through GBIF Data Portal, www.gbif.net, 2010-06-18)
  8. 8.0 8.1 8.2 8.3 8.4 Cillié, B. (1992). Pocket guide to southern African mammals. Van Schaik Publishers. ISBN 0-627-01686-3. 
  9. 9.0 9.1 9.2 9.3 9.4 9.5 9.6 Skinner, J.D.; Smithers, R.H.N. (1990). The mammals of the southern African subregion (2nd ed.). Pretoria (South Africa): University of Pretoria. p. 771. ISBN 0-86979-802-2. 
  10. Gippoliti, S. (December 2018). "The name of the East African vervet monkey (genus Chlorocebus Gray, 1870) (Primates, Cercopithecidae)". Doriana (Genoa) 9 (404). ISSN 0417-9927. 
  11. Meester, J. A. J., I. L. Rautenbach, N. J. Dippenaar, and C. M. Baker. 1986. Classification of southern African mammals. Transvaal Museum Monographs 5:1–359
  12. McGuire, M. T. (June 1974). "The history of the St. Kitts vervet". Caribbean Quarterly 20 (2): 37–52. doi:10.1080/00086495.1974.11829225. ISSN 0008-6495. 
  13. "Scientist concerned about vervet monkeys missing from Dania Beach colony". 11 February 2020. https://wsvn.com/news/special-reports/scientist-concerned-about-vervet-monkeys-missing-from-dania-beach-colony/#:~:text=It%20is%20one%20of%20those,they%20don't%20bother%20anyone.. 
  14. Long, J. L. (2003). Introduced Mammals of the World: Their History, Distribution, and Influence. Csiro Publishing, Collingwood, Australia. ISBN 9780643099166
  15. Stuart, C.; Stuart, T. (1997). Field Guide to the Larger Mammals of Africa. Penguin Random House South Africa. ISBN 1-86825-757-6. 
  16. Fedigan, L.; Fedigan, L. M. (1988). Cercopithecus aethiops: a review of field studies.. Cambridge (UK): Cambridge University Press. pp. 389–411. 
  17. Napier, P.H., ed (1981). "Part II: Family Cercopithecidae, Subfamily Cercopithecinae". Catalogue of primates in the British Museum (Natural History) and elsewhere in the British Isles. London: British Museum (Natural History). p. 203. 
  18. Pasternak, G.; Brown, L.; Kienzle, S.; Fuller, A.; Barrett, L.; Henzi, P. (2013). "Population ecology of vervet monkey in a high latitude, semi-arid riparian woodland". Koedoe 55 (1). doi:10.4102/koedoe.v55i1.1078. 
  19. Cheney, D. L.; Seyfarth, R. M. (September 1983). "Nonrandom dispersal in free-ranging vervet monkeys: social and genetic consequences". The American Naturalist 122 (3): 392–412. doi:10.1086/284142. 
  20. Cheney, D. L.; Seyfarth, R. M. (1982). "Recognition of individuals within and between groups of free-ranging vervet monkeys". American Zoologist 22 (3): 519–529. doi:10.1093/icb/22.3.519. 
  21. Cheney, D. L.; Seyfarth, R. M. (1986). "The recognition of social alliances by vervet monkeys". Animal Behaviour 34 (6): 1722–1731. doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(86)80259-7. 
  22. Seyfarth, R. M.; Cheney, D. L; Marler, P. (1980). "Vervet monkey alarm calls: semantic communication in a free-ranging primate". Animal Behaviour 28 (4): 1070–1094. doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(80)80097-2. 
  23. Cheney, D. L.; Seyfarth, R.M. (1988). "Assessment of meaning and the detection of unreliable signals by vervet monkeys". Animal Behaviour 36 (2): 477–486. doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(88)80018-6. 
  24. Estes, Richard Despard (2012). The behaviour guide to African mammals (20th Anniversary ed.). London England: University of California press. 
  25. Cheney, D. L.; Seyfarth, R. M. (1980). "Vocal recognition in free-ranging vervet monkeys". Animal Behaviour 28 (2): 362–367. doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(80)80044-3. 
  26. Lee, P.C. (1987). "Sibships: cooperation and competition among immature vervet monkeys". Primates 28 (1): 47–59. doi:10.1007/BF02382182. 
  27. Fairbanks, L. A. (1990). "Reciprocal benefits of allomothering for female vervet monkeys". Animal Behaviour 40 (3): 553–562. doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80536-6. 
  28. Fairbanks, L. A. (1988). "Vervet monkey grandmothers: interactions with infant grandoffspring". International Journal of Primatology 9 (5): 426–441. doi:10.1007/bf02736218. 
  29. Horrocks, J.; Hunte, W. (1981). "'Spite'; a constraint on optimal foraging in the vervet monkey Cercopithecus aethiops sabaeus in Barbados". American Zoology 21: 939. 
  30. Pooley, E. (1993). The Complete Field Guide to Trees of Natal, Zululand and Transkei. ISBN:0-620-17697-0.
  31. Foggo, D. (6 July 2008). "Germ warfare fear over African monkeys taken to Iran". The Times (London). http://www.timesonline.co.uk/tol/news/world/africa/article4276460.ece. 
  32. Mngoma, N. (19 September 2014). "R10 000 reward for monkey killer". IOL. http://www.iol.co.za/news/crime-courts/r10-000-reward-for-monkey-killer-1.1753789#.VXt2t_mqrUc. 
  33. Moeyersons, J.; Vermeersch, P. M.; Beeckman, H.; Van Peer, P. (1999). "Holocene environmental changes in the Gebel Umm Hammad, Eastern Desert, Egypt: Dry cave deposits and their palaeoenvironmental significance during the last 115 ka, Sodmein Cave, Red Sea Mountains, Egypt". Geomorphology 26 (4): 297–312. doi:10.1016/S0169-555X(98)00067-1. 
  34. Michael Hogan, C. (2007-12-13). "Akrotiri". Modern Antiquarian. http://themodernantiquarian.com/site/10846/akrotiri.html#fieldnotes. 
  35. Osypińska, Marta (December 2016). "Pet cats at the Early Roman Red Sea port of Berenike, Egypt". Antiquity 90 (354). doi:10.15184/aqy.2016.181. 

External links

Wikidata ☰ Q8985570 entry