Biology:Helpers at the nest
Helpers at the nest is a term used in behavioural ecology and evolutionary biology to describe a social structure in which juveniles and sexually mature adolescents of either one or both sexes remain in association with their parents and help them raise subsequent broods or litters, instead of dispersing and beginning to reproduce themselves.[1] This phenomenon was first studied in birds where it occurs most frequently, but it is also known in animals from many different groups including mammals and insects. It is a simple form of co-operative breeding. The effects of helpers usually amount to a net benefit, however, benefits are not uniformly distributed by all helpers nor across all species that exhibit this behaviour.[2] There are multiple proposed explanations for the behaviour, but its variability and broad taxonomic occurrences result in simultaneously plausible theories.[3]
The term "helper" was coined by Alexander Skutch in 1935[1] and defined more carefully in 1961 in the avian context as "a bird which assists in the nesting of an individual other than its mate, or feeds or otherwise attends a bird of whatever age which is neither its mate nor its dependent offspring." The term has been criticised as being anthropomorphic, but it remains in use.[4] Other terms used especially in mammals, depending on the specific contexts, are non-maternal (care by other than the mother), alloparental (care by other than the parents), cooperative (care by non-breeding helpers) and communal (care by other breeding females) care.[5]
Examples
It occurs in between three and eight percent of bird species worldwide (estimates vary), but is much more common in Australia and Southern Africa.[6][7] Bird species in which this behaviour is found include the house sparrow, the common moorhen, the acorn woodpecker,[8] and the apostlebird. Humans, damarland mole rats and meerkats are examples of mammals that exhibit this behaviour.[5] It is also seen in a number of species of bee such as carpenter bees (note this is distinct from the behaviour of the European honey bee, where the worker bees are sterile and incapable of reproducing).
Evolution
Three explanations for the occurrence of helpers at the nest have been put forward; they are not mutually exclusive, and in any particular species an investigation of the exact benefits and costs will be needed to see what combination of these factors may have driven the evolution of helping.[3]
- Advantage to the helpers, who may be protected from predation, or gain skills that they will need when they subsequently reproduce, as a result of staying in the parental nest.
- Kin selection: since subsequent litters or broods from the same parents will be full siblings to the helpers, they are as closely related genetically as their own offspring would be. Helping their parents is therefore as productive for the juveniles as reproducing themselves would be, and if their parents are better able to reproduce, the balance of advantage may be greater.
- Delayed advantage to the helpers, in particular because they stand to inherit their parents' territory; this explanation is particularly compelling if suitable territories are in short supply, but requires specific quantitative conditions to be met, favouring a stable queue of potential heirs.[9]
Misconceptions
Although it is frequently assumed that helpers are non-breeders, molecular evidence suggests that this may happen, and the term "secondary helper" is sometimes used in this case to indicate helpers that mate with or are not related offspring of the pair being assisted. The term "primary helper" being used for the commoner case of the helper being offspring of the pair and not involved in mating.[10] Extrapair mates are chosen by the females and are these then contribute to the care of the young who may be sired by them.[11]
Juveniles living in association with their parents cannot automatically be regarded as helpers. In a number of species, such as the logrunners[12] and the Siberian jay, young remain in the parental territory, but never help feed nestlings. However the delayed advantage explanation for the juveniles' association with their parents can still work in the absence of effective helping, whereas the kin selection explanation cannot.
Associated effects
Reproductive success
Effects on reproductive success can be but are not always positive, and the strength of that positive correlation varies by species. Sometimes, the energy benefits to breeders, instead of being invested in improving reproductive performance, are used to offset reductions in reproductive performance.[13] However, in many species, nestling survival rates are increased from helper investments.[14] Further reproductive success benefits include better quality offspring,[14] and nestling immune response.[15]
Parental investment
Helpers at the nest can provide advantageous plasticity in the amount of investment parents need to give to their eggs and chicks. The presence of helpers can be associated with lower investments as the helpers can be depended on to provide certain elements of care and raising.[13] For example, in the superb fairy-wren, nests with helpers present exhibit different average egg sizes than those without helpers.[16] This plasticity can be seen in reference to change in climate conditions. In unfavourable conditions there is a need for female parents to maintain high egg investment, but in favourable conditions, egg investment by mothers declines as helpers can assist with some of the care-load. Superb fairy-wrens with helpers at the nest see average egg sizes increase in dry and hot conditions and decrease in colder conditions compared to nests without helpers.[16]
Helper variability
Generally, helpers provide a net benefit to parents raising offspring, but this benefit is not necessarily distributed uniformly. There can be an uneven distribution of benefits between species, or even between sexes within a species.[2] Benefits provided by one sex may more heavily fluctuate according to various factors such as food availability. In acorn woodpeckers, while helpers of both sexes provide relatively the same benefit to reproductive success, male helper behaviour fluctuates. This fluctuation is dependent upon the size of recent acorn crops. Large acorn crops translate to increased behavioural effects from male helpers such as higher rates of granary tending and time spent in the home territory.[2]
Benefits and consequences for helpers
Helpers often also benefit from joining a breeding couple. After unsuccessfully breeding, juveniles may join breeding couples to become helpers. By opting to become helpers while still juveniles and having poor breeding prospects, helpers can benefit from increased survival rates.[17] A trade-off of this increased survival over non-helper juveniles is that their successful breeding rate after becoming helpers decreases compared to non-helpers.[17]
See also
- Alloparenting
- Altruistic behaviour
- Ethology
References
- ↑ 1.0 1.1 Skutch, A. F. (1935). "Helpers at the nest". Auk 52 (3): 257–273. doi:10.2307/4077738. http://sora.unm.edu/sites/default/files/journals/auk/Auk/v052n03/p0257-p0273.pdf.
- ↑ 2.0 2.1 2.2 Koenig, Walter D.; Walters, Eric L.; Barve, Sahas (2019). "Does Helping-at-the-Nest Help? The Case of the Acorn Woodpecker" (in English). Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution 7. doi:10.3389/fevo.2019.00272. ISSN 2296-701X.
- ↑ 3.0 3.1 Dickinson, J. L.; Hatchwell, B. J. (2004) "Fitness consequences of helping" in Ecology and evolution of cooperative breeding in birds by Walter D. Koenig, Janis L. Dickinson. Cambridge University Press. ISBN:978-0-521-53099-6
- ↑ Brown, J. L. (1978). "Avian communal breeding systems". Annu. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 9: 123–155. doi:10.1146/annurev.es.09.110178.001011.
- ↑ 5.0 5.1 Rosenbaum, Stacy; Gettler, Lee T. (2018). "With a little help from her friends (and family) part I: the ecology and evolution of non-maternal care in mammals" (in en). Physiology & Behavior 193 (Pt A): 1–11. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2017.12.025. PMID 29933836.
- ↑ Jetz, Walter; Rubenstein, Dustin R. (2011). "Environmental Uncertainty and the Global Biogeography of Cooperative Breeding in Birds". Current Biology 21 (1): 72–78. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2010.11.075. PMID 21185192. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/49708773.
- ↑ McMahon T.A. and Finlayson, B. (1992) Global Runoff: Continental Comparisons of Annual Flows and Peak Discharges, Catena Verlag, ISBN:3-923381-27-1
- ↑ Koenig, Walter D.; Walters, Eric L. (2012-01-01). "Brooding, provisioning, and compensatory care in the cooperatively breeding acorn woodpecker" (in en). Behavioral Ecology 23 (1): 181–190. doi:10.1093/beheco/arr172. ISSN 1465-7279.
- ↑ Wiley, R. H.; Rabenold, K. N. (1984). "The evolution of cooperative breeding by delayed reciprocity and queuing for favorable social positions". Evolution 38 (3): 609–621. doi:10.2307/2408710. PMID 28555990. Archived from the original on 2010-06-17. https://web.archive.org/web/20100617053619/http://bilbo.bio.purdue.edu/~www_eco/faculty/rabenold/pdfs/EvoCooperativeBreeding%20by%20delayed%20reciprocity.pdf.
- ↑ Ligon, J. D.; Burt, D. B. (2004) "Evolutionary origins" in Ecology and evolution of cooperative breeding in birds by Walter D. Koenig, Janis L. Dickinson. Cambridge University Press. ISBN:978-0-521-53099-6
- ↑ Rubenstein, DR (2007). "Female extrapair mate choice in a cooperative breeder: trading sex for help and increasing offspring heterozygosity". Proceedings of the Royal Society B 274 (1620): 1895–1903. doi:10.1098/rspb.2007.0424. PMID 17526455.
- ↑ Frith, C.B., Frith, D.W. & Jansen, A. (1997). "The nesting biology of the Chowchilla Orthonyx spaldingii (Orthonychidae)". Emu 97: 18–30. doi:10.1071/MU97002.
- ↑ 13.0 13.1 Manica, Lilian Tonelli; Marini, Miguel Ângelo (2012-01-01). "Helpers at the nest of White-banded Tanager Neothraupis fasciata benefit male breeders but do not increase reproductive success" (in en). Journal of Ornithology 153 (1): 149–159. doi:10.1007/s10336-011-0718-x. ISSN 2193-7206.
- ↑ 14.0 14.1 Cusick, Jessica A.; de Villa, Miguel; DuVal, Emily H.; Cox, James A. (2018-02-23). "How do helpers help? Helper contributions throughout the nesting cycle in the cooperatively breeding brown-headed nuthatch" (in en). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 72 (3): 43. doi:10.1007/s00265-018-2470-1. ISSN 1432-0762.
- ↑ Valencia, Juliana; Solís, Elena; Sorci, Gabriele; de la Cruz, Carlos (2006-07-01). "Positive correlation between helpers at nest and nestling immune response in a cooperative breeding bird" (in en). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 60 (3): 399–404. doi:10.1007/s00265-006-0179-z. ISSN 1432-0762.
- ↑ 16.0 16.1 Langmore, N. E.; Bailey, L. D.; Heinsohn, R. G.; Russell, A. F.; Kilner, R. M. (2016-11-30). "Egg size investment in superb fairy-wrens: helper effects are modulated by climate". Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 283 (1843): 20161875. doi:10.1098/rspb.2016.1875. PMID 27903872.
- ↑ 17.0 17.1 McGowan, Andrew; Hatchwell, Ben J.; Woodburn, Richard J. W. (2003). "The effect of helping behaviour on the survival of juvenile and adult long-tailed tits Aegithalos caudatus" (in en). Journal of Animal Ecology 72 (3): 491–499. doi:10.1046/j.1365-2656.2003.00719.x. ISSN 1365-2656.
Original source: https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Helpers at the nest.
Read more |