Biology:Language processing in the brain

From HandWiki
Short description: "algorithmia-node.js"
Dual stream connectivity between the auditory cortex and frontal lobe of monkeys and humans. Top: The auditory cortex of the monkey (left) and human (right) is schematically depicted on the supratemporal plane and observed from above (with the parieto- frontal operculi removed). Bottom: The brain of the monkey (left) and human (right) is schematically depicted and displayed from the side. Orange frames mark the region of the auditory cortex, which is displayed in the top sub-figures. Top and Bottom: Blue colors mark regions affiliated with the ADS, and red colors mark regions affiliated with the AVS (dark red and blue regions mark the primary auditory fields). CC-BY icon.svg Material was copied from this source, which is available under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.

Language processing refers to the way humans use words to communicate ideas and feelings, and how such communications are processed and understood. Language processing is considered to be a uniquely human ability that is not produced with the same grammatical understanding or systematicity in even human's closest primate relatives.[1]

Throughout the 20th century the dominant model[2] for language processing in the brain was the Geschwind-Lichteim-Wernicke model, which is based primarily on the analysis of brain damaged patients. However, due to improvements in intra-cortical electrophysiological recordings of monkey and human brains, as well non-invasive techniques such as fMRI, PET, MEG and EEG, a dual auditory pathway[3][4] has been revealed and a two-streams model has been developed. In accordance with this model, there are two pathways that connect the auditory cortex to the frontal lobe, each pathway accounting for different linguistic roles. The auditory ventral stream pathway is responsible for sound recognition, and is accordingly known as the auditory 'what' pathway. The auditory dorsal stream in both humans and non-human primates is responsible for sound localization, and is accordingly known as the auditory 'where' pathway. In humans, this pathway (especially in the left hemisphere) is also responsible for speech production, speech repetition, lip-reading, and phonological working memory and long-term memory. In accordance with the 'from where to what' model of language evolution,[5][6] the reason the ADS is characterized with such a broad range of functions is that each indicates a different stage in language evolution.

The division of the two streams first occurs in the auditory nerve where the anterior branch enters the anterior cochlear nucleus in the brainstem which gives rise to the auditory ventral stream. The posterior branch enters the dorsal and posteroventral cochlear nucleus to give rise to the auditory dorsal stream.[7]:8

Language processing can also occur in relation to signed languages or written content.

Early neurolinguistics models

Language Areas of the human brain. The angular gyrus is represented in orange, supramarginal gyrus is represented in yellow, Broca's area is represented in blue, Wernicke's area is represented in green and the primary auditory cortex is represented in pink.

Throughout the 20th century, our knowledge of language processing in the brain was dominated by the Wernicke-Lichtheim-Geschwind model.[8][2][9] The Wernicke-Lichtheim-Geschwind model is primarily based on research conducted on brain-damaged individuals who were reported to possess a variety of language related disorders. In accordance with this model, words are perceived via a specialized word reception center (Wernicke's area) that is located in the left temporoparietal junction. This region then projects to a word production center (Broca's area) that is located in the left inferior frontal gyrus. Because almost all language input was thought to funnel via Wernicke's area and all language output to funnel via Broca's area, it became extremely difficult to identify the basic properties of each region. This lack of clear definition for the contribution of Wernicke's and Broca's regions to human language rendered it extremely difficult to identify their homologues in other primates.[10] With the advent of the fMRI and its application for lesion mappings, however, it was shown that this model is based on incorrect correlations between symptoms and lesions.[11][12][13][14][15][16][17] The refutation of such an influential and dominant model opened the door to new models of language processing in the brain.

Current neurolinguistics models

Anatomy

In the last two decades, significant advances occurred in our understanding of the neural processing of sounds in primates. Initially by recording of neural activity in the auditory cortices of monkeys[18][19] and later elaborated via histological staining[20][21][22] and fMRI scanning studies,[23] 3 auditory fields were identified in the primary auditory cortex, and 9 associative auditory fields were shown to surround them (Figure 1 top left). Anatomical tracing and lesion studies further indicated of a separation between the anterior and posterior auditory fields, with the anterior primary auditory fields (areas R-RT) projecting to the anterior associative auditory fields (areas AL-RTL), and the posterior primary auditory field (area A1) projecting to the posterior associative auditory fields (areas CL-CM).[20][24][25][26] Recently, evidence accumulated that indicates homology between the human and monkey auditory fields. In humans, histological staining studies revealed two separate auditory fields in the primary auditory region of Heschl's gyrus,[27][28] and by mapping the tonotopic organization of the human primary auditory fields with high resolution fMRI and comparing it to the tonotopic organization of the monkey primary auditory fields, homology was established between the human anterior primary auditory field and monkey area R (denoted in humans as area hR) and the human posterior primary auditory field and the monkey area A1 (denoted in humans as area hA1).[29][30][31][32][33] Intra-cortical recordings from the human auditory cortex further demonstrated similar patterns of connectivity to the auditory cortex of the monkey. Recording from the surface of the auditory cortex (supra-temporal plane) reported that the anterior Heschl's gyrus (area hR) projects primarily to the middle-anterior superior temporal gyrus (mSTG-aSTG) and the posterior Heschl's gyrus (area hA1) projects primarily to the posterior superior temporal gyrus (pSTG) and the planum temporale (area PT; Figure 1 top right).[34][35] Consistent with connections from area hR to the aSTG and hA1 to the pSTG is an fMRI study of a patient with impaired sound recognition (auditory agnosia), who was shown with reduced bilateral activation in areas hR and aSTG but with spared activation in the mSTG-pSTG.[36] This connectivity pattern is also corroborated by a study that recorded activation from the lateral surface of the auditory cortex and reported of simultaneous non-overlapping activation clusters in the pSTG and mSTG-aSTG while listening to sounds.[37]

Downstream to the auditory cortex, anatomical tracing studies in monkeys delineated projections from the anterior associative auditory fields (areas AL-RTL) to ventral prefrontal and premotor cortices in the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG)[38][39] and amygdala.[40] Cortical recording and functional imaging studies in macaque monkeys further elaborated on this processing stream by showing that acoustic information flows from the anterior auditory cortex to the temporal pole (TP) and then to the IFG.[41][42][43][44][45][46] This pathway is commonly referred to as the auditory ventral stream (AVS; Figure 1, bottom left-red arrows). In contrast to the anterior auditory fields, tracing studies reported that the posterior auditory fields (areas CL-CM) project primarily to dorsolateral prefrontal and premotor cortices (although some projections do terminate in the IFG.[47][39] Cortical recordings and anatomical tracing studies in monkeys further provided evidence that this processing stream flows from the posterior auditory fields to the frontal lobe via a relay station in the intra-parietal sulcus (IPS).[48][49][50][51][52][53] This pathway is commonly referred to as the auditory dorsal stream (ADS; Figure 1, bottom left-blue arrows). Comparing the white matter pathways involved in communication in humans and monkeys with diffusion tensor imaging techniques indicates of similar connections of the AVS and ADS in the two species (Monkey,[52] Human[54][55][56][57][58][59]). In humans, the pSTG was shown to project to the parietal lobe (sylvian parietal-temporal junction-inferior parietal lobule; Spt-IPL), and from there to dorsolateral prefrontal and premotor cortices (Figure 1, bottom right-blue arrows), and the aSTG was shown to project to the anterior temporal lobe (middle temporal gyrus-temporal pole; MTG-TP) and from there to the IFG (Figure 1 bottom right-red arrows).

Auditory ventral stream

The auditory ventral stream (AVS) connects the auditory cortex with the middle temporal gyrus and temporal pole, which in turn connects with the inferior frontal gyrus. This pathway is responsible for sound recognition, and is accordingly known as the auditory 'what' pathway. The functions of the AVS include the following.

Sound recognition

Accumulative converging evidence indicates that the AVS is involved in recognizing auditory objects. At the level of the primary auditory cortex, recordings from monkeys showed higher percentage of neurons selective for learned melodic sequences in area R than area A1,[60] and a study in humans demonstrated more selectivity for heard syllables in the anterior Heschl's gyrus (area hR) than posterior Heschl's gyrus (area hA1).[61] In downstream associative auditory fields, studies from both monkeys and humans reported that the border between the anterior and posterior auditory fields (Figure 1-area PC in the monkey and mSTG in the human) processes pitch attributes that are necessary for the recognition of auditory objects.[18] The anterior auditory fields of monkeys were also demonstrated with selectivity for con-specific vocalizations with intra-cortical recordings.[41][19][62] and functional imaging[63][42][43] One fMRI monkey study further demonstrated a role of the aSTG in the recognition of individual voices.[42] The role of the human mSTG-aSTG in sound recognition was demonstrated via functional imaging studies that correlated activity in this region with isolation of auditory objects from background noise,[64][65] and with the recognition of spoken words,[66][67][68][69][70][71][72] voices,[73] melodies,[74][75] environmental sounds,[76][77][78] and non-speech communicative sounds.[79] A meta-analysis of fMRI studies[80] further demonstrated functional dissociation between the left mSTG and aSTG, with the former processing short speech units (phonemes) and the latter processing longer units (e.g., words, environmental sounds). A study that recorded neural activity directly from the left pSTG and aSTG reported that the aSTG, but not pSTG, was more active when the patient listened to speech in her native language than unfamiliar foreign language.[81] Consistently, electro stimulation to the aSTG of this patient resulted in impaired speech perception[81] (see also[82][83] for similar results). Intra-cortical recordings from the right and left aSTG further demonstrated that speech is processed laterally to music.[81] An fMRI study of a patient with impaired sound recognition (auditory agnosia) due to brainstem damage was also shown with reduced activation in areas hR and aSTG of both hemispheres when hearing spoken words and environmental sounds.[36] Recordings from the anterior auditory cortex of monkeys while maintaining learned sounds in working memory,[46] and the debilitating effect of induced lesions to this region on working memory recall,[84][85][86] further implicate the AVS in maintaining the perceived auditory objects in working memory. In humans, area mSTG-aSTG was also reported active during rehearsal of heard syllables with MEG.[87] and fMRI[88] The latter study further demonstrated that working memory in the AVS is for the acoustic properties of spoken words and that it is independent to working memory in the ADS, which mediates inner speech. Working memory studies in monkeys also suggest that in monkeys, in contrast to humans, the AVS is the dominant working memory store.[89]

In humans, downstream to the aSTG, the MTG and TP are thought to constitute the semantic lexicon, which is a long-term memory repository of audio-visual representations that are interconnected on the basis of semantic relationships. (See also the reviews by[3][4] discussing this topic). The primary evidence for this role of the MTG-TP is that patients with damage to this region (e.g., patients with semantic dementia or herpes simplex virus encephalitis) are reported[90][91] with an impaired ability to describe visual and auditory objects and a tendency to commit semantic errors when naming objects (i.e., semantic paraphasia). Semantic paraphasias were also expressed by aphasic patients with left MTG-TP damage[14][92] and were shown to occur in non-aphasic patients after electro-stimulation to this region.[93][83] or the underlying white matter pathway[94] Two meta-analyses of the fMRI literature also reported that the anterior MTG and TP were consistently active during semantic analysis of speech and text;[66][95] and an intra-cortical recording study correlated neural discharge in the MTG with the comprehension of intelligible sentences.[96]

Sentence comprehension

In addition to extracting meaning from sounds, the MTG-TP region of the AVS appears to have a role in sentence comprehension, possibly by merging concepts together (e.g., merging the concept 'blue' and 'shirt' to create the concept of a 'blue shirt'). The role of the MTG in extracting meaning from sentences has been demonstrated in functional imaging studies reporting stronger activation in the anterior MTG when proper sentences are contrasted with lists of words, sentences in a foreign or nonsense language, scrambled sentences, sentences with semantic or syntactic violations and sentence-like sequences of environmental sounds.[97][98][99][100][101][102][103][104] One fMRI study[105] in which participants were instructed to read a story further correlated activity in the anterior MTG with the amount of semantic and syntactic content each sentence contained. An EEG study[106] that contrasted cortical activity while reading sentences with and without syntactic violations in healthy participants and patients with MTG-TP damage, concluded that the MTG-TP in both hemispheres participate in the automatic (rule based) stage of syntactic analysis (ELAN component), and that the left MTG-TP is also involved in a later controlled stage of syntax analysis (P600 component). Patients with damage to the MTG-TP region have also been reported with impaired sentence comprehension.[14][107][108] See review[109] for more information on this topic.

Bilaterality

In contradiction to the Wernicke-Lichtheim-Geschwind model that implicates sound recognition to occur solely in the left hemisphere, studies that examined the properties of the right or left hemisphere in isolation via unilateral hemispheric anesthesia (i.e., the WADA procedure[110]) or intra-cortical recordings from each hemisphere[96] provided evidence that sound recognition is processed bilaterally. Moreover, a study that instructed patients with disconnected hemispheres (i.e., split-brain patients) to match spoken words to written words presented to the right or left hemifields, reported vocabulary in the right hemisphere that almost matches in size with the left hemisphere[111] (The right hemisphere vocabulary was equivalent to the vocabulary of a healthy 11-years old child). This bilateral recognition of sounds is also consistent with the finding that unilateral lesion to the auditory cortex rarely results in deficit to auditory comprehension (i.e., auditory agnosia), whereas a second lesion to the remaining hemisphere (which could occur years later) does.[112][113] Finally, as mentioned earlier, an fMRI scan of an auditory agnosia patient demonstrated bilateral reduced activation in the anterior auditory cortices,[36] and bilateral electro-stimulation to these regions in both hemispheres resulted with impaired speech recognition.[81]

Auditory dorsal stream

The auditory dorsal stream connects the auditory cortex with the parietal lobe, which in turn connects with inferior frontal gyrus. In both humans and non-human primates, the auditory dorsal stream is responsible for sound localization, and is accordingly known as the auditory 'where' pathway. In humans, this pathway (especially in the left hemisphere) is also responsible for speech production, speech repetition, lip-reading, and phonological working memory and long-term memory.

Speech production

Studies of present-day humans have demonstrated a role for the ADS in speech production, particularly in the vocal expression of the names of objects. For instance, in a series of studies in which sub-cortical fibers were directly stimulated[94] interference in the left pSTG and IPL resulted in errors during object-naming tasks, and interference in the left IFG resulted in speech arrest. Magnetic interference in the pSTG and IFG of healthy participants also produced speech errors and speech arrest, respectively[114][115] One study has also reported that electrical stimulation of the left IPL caused patients to believe that they had spoken when they had not and that IFG stimulation caused patients to unconsciously move their lips.[116] The contribution of the ADS to the process of articulating the names of objects could be dependent on the reception of afferents from the semantic lexicon of the AVS, as an intra-cortical recording study reported of activation in the posterior MTG prior to activation in the Spt-IPL region when patients named objects in pictures[117] Intra-cortical electrical stimulation studies also reported that electrical interference to the posterior MTG was correlated with impaired object naming[118][82]

Vocal mimicry

Although sound perception is primarily ascribed with the AVS, the ADS appears associated with several aspects of speech perception. For instance, in a meta-analysis of fMRI studies[119] (Turkeltaub and Coslett, 2010), in which the auditory perception of phonemes was contrasted with closely matching sounds, and the studies were rated for the required level of attention, the authors concluded that attention to phonemes correlates with strong activation in the pSTG-pSTS region. An intra-cortical recording study in which participants were instructed to identify syllables also correlated the hearing of each syllable with its own activation pattern in the pSTG.[120] The involvement of the ADS in both speech perception and production has been further illuminated in several pioneering functional imaging studies that contrasted speech perception with overt or covert speech production.[121][122][123] These studies demonstrated that the pSTS is active only during the perception of speech, whereas area Spt is active during both the perception and production of speech. The authors concluded that the pSTS projects to area Spt, which converts the auditory input into articulatory movements.[124][125] Similar results have been obtained in a study in which participants' temporal and parietal lobes were electrically stimulated. This study reported that electrically stimulating the pSTG region interferes with sentence comprehension and that stimulation of the IPL interferes with the ability to vocalize the names of objects.[83] The authors also reported that stimulation in area Spt and the inferior IPL induced interference during both object-naming and speech-comprehension tasks. The role of the ADS in speech repetition is also congruent with the results of the other functional imaging studies that have localized activation during speech repetition tasks to ADS regions.[126][127][128] An intra-cortical recording study that recorded activity throughout most of the temporal, parietal and frontal lobes also reported activation in the pSTG, Spt, IPL and IFG when speech repetition is contrasted with speech perception.[129] Neuropsychological studies have also found that individuals with speech repetition deficits but preserved auditory comprehension (i.e., conduction aphasia) suffer from circumscribed damage to the Spt-IPL area[130][131][132][133][134][135][136] or damage to the projections that emanate from this area and target the frontal lobe[137][138][139][140] Studies have also reported a transient speech repetition deficit in patients after direct intra-cortical electrical stimulation to this same region.[11][141][142] Insight into the purpose of speech repetition in the ADS is provided by longitudinal studies of children that correlated the learning of foreign vocabulary with the ability to repeat nonsense words.[143][144]

Speech monitoring

In addition to repeating and producing speech, the ADS appears to have a role in monitoring the quality of the speech output. Neuroanatomical evidence suggests that the ADS is equipped with descending connections from the IFG to the pSTG that relay information about motor activity (i.e., corollary discharges) in the vocal apparatus (mouth, tongue, vocal folds). This feedback marks the sound perceived during speech production as self-produced and can be used to adjust the vocal apparatus to increase the similarity between the perceived and emitted calls. Evidence for descending connections from the IFG to the pSTG has been offered by a study that electrically stimulated the IFG during surgical operations and reported the spread of activation to the pSTG-pSTS-Spt region[145] A study[146] that compared the ability of aphasic patients with frontal, parietal or temporal lobe damage to quickly and repeatedly articulate a string of syllables reported that damage to the frontal lobe interfered with the articulation of both identical syllabic strings ("Bababa") and non-identical syllabic strings ("Badaga"), whereas patients with temporal or parietal lobe damage only exhibited impairment when articulating non-identical syllabic strings. Because the patients with temporal and parietal lobe damage were capable of repeating the syllabic string in the first task, their speech perception and production appears to be relatively preserved, and their deficit in the second task is therefore due to impaired monitoring. Demonstrating the role of the descending ADS connections in monitoring emitted calls, an fMRI study instructed participants to speak under normal conditions or when hearing a modified version of their own voice (delayed first formant) and reported that hearing a distorted version of one's own voice results in increased activation in the pSTG.[147] Further demonstrating that the ADS facilitates motor feedback during mimicry is an intra-cortical recording study that contrasted speech perception and repetition.[129] The authors reported that, in addition to activation in the IPL and IFG, speech repetition is characterized by stronger activation in the pSTG than during speech perception.

Integration of phonemes with lip-movements

Although sound perception is primarily ascribed with the AVS, the ADS appears associated with several aspects of speech perception. For instance, in a meta-analysis of fMRI studies[119] in which the auditory perception of phonemes was contrasted with closely matching sounds, and the studies were rated for the required level of attention, the authors concluded that attention to phonemes correlates with strong activation in the pSTG-pSTS region. An intra-cortical recording study in which participants were instructed to identify syllables also correlated the hearing of each syllable with its own activation pattern in the pSTG.[148] Consistent with the role of the ADS in discriminating phonemes,[119] studies have ascribed the integration of phonemes and their corresponding lip movements (i.e., visemes) to the pSTS of the ADS. For example, an fMRI study[149] has correlated activation in the pSTS with the McGurk illusion (in which hearing the syllable "ba" while seeing the viseme "ga" results in the perception of the syllable "da"). Another study has found that using magnetic stimulation to interfere with processing in this area further disrupts the McGurk illusion.[150] The association of the pSTS with the audio-visual integration of speech has also been demonstrated in a study that presented participants with pictures of faces and spoken words of varying quality. The study reported that the pSTS selects for the combined increase of the clarity of faces and spoken words.[151] Corroborating evidence has been provided by an fMRI study[152] that contrasted the perception of audio-visual speech with audio-visual non-speech (pictures and sounds of tools). This study reported the detection of speech-selective compartments in the pSTS. In addition, an fMRI study[153] that contrasted congruent audio-visual speech with incongruent speech (pictures of still faces) reported pSTS activation. For a review presenting additional converging evidence regarding the role of the pSTS and ADS in phoneme-viseme integration see.[154]

Phonological long-term memory

A growing body of evidence indicates that humans, in addition to having a long-term store for word meanings located in the MTG-TP of the AVS (i.e., the semantic lexicon), also have a long-term store for the names of objects located in the Spt-IPL region of the ADS (i.e., the phonological lexicon). For example, a study[155][156] examining patients with damage to the AVS (MTG damage) or damage to the ADS (IPL damage) reported that MTG damage results in individuals incorrectly identifying objects (e.g., calling a "goat" a "sheep," an example of semantic paraphasia). Conversely, IPL damage results in individuals correctly identifying the object but incorrectly pronouncing its name (e.g., saying "gof" instead of "goat," an example of phonemic paraphasia). Semantic paraphasia errors have also been reported in patients receiving intra-cortical electrical stimulation of the AVS (MTG), and phonemic paraphasia errors have been reported in patients whose ADS (pSTG, Spt, and IPL) received intra-cortical electrical stimulation.[83][157][94] Further supporting the role of the ADS in object naming is an MEG study that localized activity in the IPL during the learning and during the recall of object names.[158] A study that induced magnetic interference in participants' IPL while they answered questions about an object reported that the participants were capable of answering questions regarding the object's characteristics or perceptual attributes but were impaired when asked whether the word contained two or three syllables.[159] An MEG study has also correlated recovery from anomia (a disorder characterized by an impaired ability to name objects) with changes in IPL activation.[160] Further supporting the role of the IPL in encoding the sounds of words are studies reporting that, compared to monolinguals, bilinguals have greater cortical density in the IPL but not the MTG.[161][162] Because evidence shows that, in bilinguals, different phonological representations of the same word share the same semantic representation,[163] this increase in density in the IPL verifies the existence of the phonological lexicon: the semantic lexicon of bilinguals is expected to be similar in size to the semantic lexicon of monolinguals, whereas their phonological lexicon should be twice the size. Consistent with this finding, cortical density in the IPL of monolinguals also correlates with vocabulary size.[164][165] Notably, the functional dissociation of the AVS and ADS in object-naming tasks is supported by cumulative evidence from reading research showing that semantic errors are correlated with MTG impairment and phonemic errors with IPL impairment. Based on these associations, the semantic analysis of text has been linked to the inferior-temporal gyrus and MTG, and the phonological analysis of text has been linked to the pSTG-Spt- IPL[166][167][168]

Phonological working memory

Working memory is often treated as the temporary activation of the representations stored in long-term memory that are used for speech (phonological representations). This sharing of resources between working memory and speech is evident by the finding[169][170] that speaking during rehearsal results in a significant reduction in the number of items that can be recalled from working memory (articulatory suppression). The involvement of the phonological lexicon in working memory is also evidenced by the tendency of individuals to make more errors when recalling words from a recently learned list of phonologically similar words than from a list of phonologically dissimilar words (the phonological similarity effect).[169] Studies have also found that speech errors committed during reading are remarkably similar to speech errors made during the recall of recently learned, phonologically similar words from working memory.[171] Patients with IPL damage have also been observed to exhibit both speech production errors and impaired working memory[172][173][174][175] Finally, the view that verbal working memory is the result of temporarily activating phonological representations in the ADS is compatible with recent models describing working memory as the combination of maintaining representations in the mechanism of attention in parallel to temporarily activating representations in long-term memory.[170][176][177][178] It has been argued that the role of the ADS in the rehearsal of lists of words is the reason this pathway is active during sentence comprehension[179] For a review of the role of the ADS in working memory, see.[180]

The 'from where to what' model of language evolution hypotheses 7 stages of language evolution.

The evolution of language

The auditory dorsal stream also has non-language related functions, such as sound localization[181][182][183][184][185] and guidance of eye movements.[186][187] Recent studies also indicate a role of the ADS in localization of family/tribe members, as a study[188] that recorded from the cortex of an epileptic patient reported that the pSTG, but not aSTG, is selective for the presence of new speakers. An fMRI[189] study of fetuses at their third trimester also demonstrated that area Spt is more selective to female speech than pure tones, and a sub-section of Spt is selective to the speech of their mother in contrast to unfamiliar female voices.

It is presently unknown why so many functions are ascribed to the human ADS. An attempt to unify these functions under a single framework was conducted in the 'From where to what' model of language evolution[190][191] In accordance with this model, each function of the ADS indicates of a different intermediate phase in the evolution of language. The roles of sound localization and integration of sound location with voices and auditory objects is interpreted as evidence that the origin of speech is the exchange of contact calls (calls used to report location in cases of separation) between mothers and offspring. The role of the ADS in the perception and production of intonations is interpreted as evidence that speech began by modifying the contact calls with intonations, possibly for distinguishing alarm contact calls from safe contact calls. The role of the ADS in encoding the names of objects (phonological long-term memory) is interpreted as evidence of gradual transition from modifying calls with intonations to complete vocal control. The role of the ADS in the integration of lip movements with phonemes and in speech repetition is interpreted as evidence that spoken words were learned by infants mimicking their parents' vocalizations, initially by imitating their lip movements. The role of the ADS in phonological working memory is interpreted as evidence that the words learned through mimicry remained active in the ADS even when not spoken. This resulted with individuals capable of rehearsing a list of vocalizations, which enabled the production of words with several syllables. Further developments in the ADS enabled the rehearsal of lists of words, which provided the infra-structure for communicating with sentences.

Sign language in the brain

Neuroscientific research has provided a scientific understanding of how sign language is processed in the brain. There are over 135 discrete sign languages around the world- making use of different accents formed by separate areas of a country.[192]

By resorting to lesion analyses and neuroimaging, neuroscientists have discovered that whether it be spoken or sign language, human brains process language in general, in a similar manner regarding which area of the brain is being used. [192]Lesion analyses are used to examine the consequences of damage to specific brain regions involved in language while neuroimaging explore regions that are engaged in the processing of language.[192]

Previous hypotheses have been made that damage to Broca's area or Wernicke’s area does not affect sign language being perceived; however, it is not the case. Studies have shown that damage to these areas are similar in results in spoken language where sign errors are present and/or repeated. [192]In both types of languages, they are affected by damage to the left hemisphere of the brain rather than the right -usually dealing with the arts.

There are obvious patterns for utilizing and processing language. In sign language, Broca’s area is activated while processing sign language employs Wernicke’s area similar to that of spoken language [192]

There have been other hypotheses about the lateralization of the two hemispheres. Specifically, the right hemisphere was thought to contribute to the overall communication of a language globally whereas the left hemisphere would be dominant in generating the language locally.[193] Through research in aphasias, RHD signers were found to have a problem maintaining the spatial portion of their signs, confusing similar signs at different locations necessary to communicate with another properly.[193] LHD signers, on the other hand, had similar results to those of hearing patients. Furthermore, other studies have emphasized that sign language is present bilaterally but will need to continue researching to reach a conclusion.[193]

Writing in the brain

There is a comparatively small body of research on the neurology of reading and writing.[194] Most of the studies performed deal with reading rather than writing or spelling, and the majority of both kinds focus solely on the English language.[195] English orthography is less transparent than that of other languages using a Latin script.[194] Another difficulty is that some studies focus on spelling words of English and omit the few logographic characters found in the script.[194]

In terms of spelling, English words can be divided into three categories – regular, irregular, and “novel words” or “nonwords.” Regular words are those in which there is a regular, one-to-one correspondence between grapheme and phoneme in spelling. Irregular words are those in which no such correspondence exists. Nonwords are those that exhibit the expected orthography of regular words but do not carry meaning, such as nonce words and onomatopoeia.[194]

An issue in the cognitive and neurological study of reading and spelling in English is whether a single-route or dual-route model best describes how literate speakers are able to read and write all three categories of English words according to accepted standards of orthographic correctness. Single-route models posit that lexical memory is used to store all spellings of words for retrieval in a single process. Dual-route models posit that lexical memory is employed to process irregular and high-frequency regular words, while low-frequency regular words and nonwords are processed using a sub-lexical set of phonological rules.[194]

The single-route model for reading has found support in computer modelling studies, which suggest that readers identify words by their orthographic similarities to phonologically alike words.[194] However, cognitive and lesion studies lean towards the dual-route model. Cognitive spelling studies on children and adults suggest that spellers employ phonological rules in spelling regular words and nonwords, while lexical memory is accessed to spell irregular words and high-frequency words of all types.[194] Similarly, lesion studies indicate that lexical memory is used to store irregular words and certain regular words, while phonological rules are used to spell nonwords.[194]

More recently, neuroimaging studies using positron emission tomography and fMRI have suggested a balanced model in which the reading of all word types begins in the visual word form area, but subsequently branches off into different routes depending upon whether or not access to lexical memory or semantic information is needed (which would be expected with irregular words under a dual-route model).[194] A 2007 fMRI study found that subjects asked to produce regular words in a spelling task exhibited greater activation in the left posterior STG, an area used for phonological processing, while the spelling of irregular words produced greater activation of areas used for lexical memory and semantic processing, such as the left IFG and left SMG and both hemispheres of the MTG.[194] Spelling nonwords was found to access members of both pathways, such as the left STG and bilateral MTG and ITG.[194] Significantly, it was found that spelling induces activation in areas such as the left fusiform gyrus and left SMG that are also important in reading, suggesting that a similar pathway is used for both reading and writing.[194]

Far less information exists on the cognition and neurology of non-alphabetic and non-English scripts. Every language has a morphological and a phonological component, either of which can be recorded by a writing system. Scripts recording words and morphemes are considered logographic, while those recording phonological segments, such as syllabaries and alphabets, are phonographic.[195] Most systems combine the two and have both logographic and phonographic characters.[195]

In terms of complexity, writing systems can be characterized as “transparent” or “opaque” and as “shallow” or “deep.” A “transparent” system exhibits an obvious correspondence between grapheme and sound, while in an “opaque” system this relationship is less obvious. The terms “shallow” and “deep” refer to the extent that a system’s orthography represents morphemes as opposed to phonological segments.[195] Systems that record larger morphosyntactic or phonological segments, such as logographic systems and syllabaries put greater demand on the memory of users.[195] It would thus be expected that an opaque or deep writing system would put greater demand on areas of the brain used for lexical memory than would a system with transparent or shallow orthography.

See also


References

  1. Seidenberg, Mark S.; Petitto, Laura A. (1987). "Communication, symbolic communication, and language: Comment on Savage-Rumbaugh, McDonald, Sevcik, Hopkins, and Rupert (1986)". Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 116 (3): 279–287. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.116.3.279. 
  2. 2.0 2.1 "Disconnexion syndromes in animals and man. I". Brain 88 (2): 237–94. June 1965. doi:10.1093/brain/88.2.237. PMID 5318481. 
  3. 3.0 3.1 "The cortical organization of speech processing". Nature Reviews. Neuroscience 8 (5): 393–402. May 2007. doi:10.1038/nrn2113. PMID 17431404. 
  4. 4.0 4.1 "The cortical organization of lexical knowledge: a dual lexicon model of spoken language processing". Brain and Language 121 (3): 273–88. June 2012. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2012.03.005. PMID 22498237. 
  5. "From where to what: a neuroanatomically based evolutionary model of the emergence of speech in humans". F1000Research 4: 67. 2017-09-20. doi:10.12688/f1000research.6175.3. PMID 28928931.  CC-BY icon.svg Material was copied from this source, which is available under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.
  6. "From Mimicry to Language: A Neuroanatomically Based Evolutionary Model of the Emergence of Vocal Language". Frontiers in Neuroscience 10: 307. 2016. doi:10.3389/fnins.2016.00307. PMID 27445676.  CC-BY icon.svg Material was copied from this source, which is available under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.
  7. "Chapter 1: Auditory pathways: anatomy and physiology". Handbook of Clinical Neurology. 129. 2015. pp. 3–25. doi:10.1016/B978-0-444-62630-1.00001-9. ISBN 978-0-444-62630-1. 
  8. "On Aphasia". Brain 7 (4): 433–484. 1885-01-01. doi:10.1093/brain/7.4.433. 
  9. Wernicke, Carl (1974). Der aphasische Symptomenkomplex. Springer Berlin Heidelberg. pp. 1–70. ISBN 978-3-540-06905-8. 
  10. "The evolutionary origin of the language areas in the human brain. A neuroanatomical perspective". Brain Research. Brain Research Reviews 25 (3): 381–96. December 1997. doi:10.1016/s0165-0173(97)00053-2. PMID 9495565. 
  11. 11.0 11.1 "Conduction aphasia and the arcuate fasciculus: A reexamination of the Wernicke-Geschwind model". Brain and Language 70 (1): 1–12. October 1999. doi:10.1006/brln.1999.2135. PMID 10534369. 
  12. "Wernicke's area revisited: parallel streams and word processing". Brain and Language 127 (2): 181–91. November 2013. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2013.09.014. PMID 24404576. 
  13. "The pursuit of brain-language relationships". Brain and Language 71 (1): 59–61. January 2000. doi:10.1006/brln.1999.2212. PMID 10716807. 
  14. 14.0 14.1 14.2 "Lesion analysis of the brain areas involved in language comprehension". Cognition 92 (1–2): 145–77. May 2004. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2003.11.002. PMID 15037129. 
  15. "The Wernicke conundrum and the anatomy of language comprehension in primary progressive aphasia". Brain 138 (Pt 8): 2423–37. August 2015. doi:10.1093/brain/awv154. PMID 26112340. 
  16. "Towards a new neurobiology of language". The Journal of Neuroscience 32 (41): 14125–31. October 2012. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.3244-12.2012. PMID 23055482. 
  17. "Unexpected CT-scan findings in global aphasia". Cortex; A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior 22 (1): 55–69. March 1986. doi:10.1016/s0010-9452(86)80032-6. PMID 2423296. 
  18. 18.0 18.1 "Cortical representations of pitch in monkeys and humans". Current Opinion in Neurobiology 16 (4): 391–9. August 2006. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2006.07.001. PMID 16842992. 
  19. 19.0 19.1 "Processing of complex sounds in the macaque nonprimary auditory cortex". Science 268 (5207): 111–4. April 1995. doi:10.1126/science.7701330. PMID 7701330. Bibcode1995Sci...268..111R. 
  20. 20.0 20.1 "Cortical connections of the auditory cortex in marmoset monkeys: core and medial belt regions". The Journal of Comparative Neurology 496 (1): 27–71. May 2006. doi:10.1002/cne.20923. PMID 16528722. 
  21. "Cortical connections of auditory cortex in marmoset monkeys: lateral belt and parabelt regions". Anatomical Record 295 (5): 800–21. May 2012. doi:10.1002/ar.22451. PMID 22461313. 
  22. "Subdivisions of auditory cortex and processing streams in primates". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 97 (22): 11793–9. October 2000. doi:10.1073/pnas.97.22.11793. PMID 11050211. Bibcode2000PNAS...9711793K. 
  23. "Functional imaging reveals numerous fields in the monkey auditory cortex". PLOS Biology 4 (7): e215. July 2006. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.0040215. PMID 16774452. 
  24. "Tonotopic organization, architectonic fields, and connections of auditory cortex in macaque monkeys". The Journal of Comparative Neurology 335 (3): 437–59. September 1993. doi:10.1002/cne.903350312. PMID 7693772. 
  25. "Mechanisms and streams for processing of "what" and "where" in auditory cortex". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 97 (22): 11800–6. October 2000. doi:10.1073/pnas.97.22.11800. PMID 11050212. Bibcode2000PNAS...9711800R. 
  26. "Serial and parallel processing in rhesus monkey auditory cortex". The Journal of Comparative Neurology 382 (1): 89–103. May 1997. doi:10.1002/(sici)1096-9861(19970526)382:1<89::aid-cne6>3.3.co;2-y. PMID 9136813. 
  27. "Mapping auditory core, lateral belt, and parabelt cortices in the human superior temporal gyrus". The Journal of Comparative Neurology 491 (3): 270–89. October 2005. doi:10.1002/cne.20702. PMID 16134138. 
  28. "Histochemical identification of cortical areas in the auditory region of the human brain". Experimental Brain Research 143 (4): 499–508. April 2002. doi:10.1007/s00221-002-1014-z. PMID 11914796. 
  29. "Human primary auditory cortex follows the shape of Heschl's gyrus". The Journal of Neuroscience 31 (40): 14067–75. October 2011. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.2000-11.2011. PMID 21976491. 
  30. "Tonotopic organization of human auditory cortex". NeuroImage 50 (3): 1202–11. April 2010. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.01.046. PMID 20096790. 
  31. "Mapping the tonotopic organization in human auditory cortex with minimally salient acoustic stimulation". Cerebral Cortex 22 (9): 2024–38. September 2012. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhr282. PMID 21980020. 
  32. "Extensive cochleotopic mapping of human auditory cortical fields obtained with phase-encoding fMRI". PLOS ONE 6 (3): e17832. March 2011. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0017832. PMID 21448274. Bibcode2011PLoSO...617832S. 
  33. "Functional properties of human auditory cortical fields". Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience 4: 155. 2010. doi:10.3389/fnsys.2010.00155. PMID 21160558. 
  34. "Temporal envelope processing in the human auditory cortex: response and interconnections of auditory cortical areas". Hearing Research 237 (1–2): 1–18. March 2008. doi:10.1016/j.heares.2007.12.003. PMID 18255243. https://www.hal.inserm.fr/inserm-00254870/file/Temporal_envelope_processing_in_the_human_auditory_cortex.pdf. 
  35. "Evidence of functional connectivity between auditory cortical areas revealed by amplitude modulation sound processing". Cerebral Cortex 17 (2): 304–13. February 2007. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhj148. PMID 16514106. 
  36. 36.0 36.1 36.2 "Functional Mapping of the Human Auditory Cortex: fMRI Investigation of a Patient with Auditory Agnosia from Trauma to the Inferior Colliculus". Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology 28 (3): 160–80. September 2015. doi:10.1097/wnn.0000000000000072. PMID 26413744. http://opus.bath.ac.uk/48370/1/Poliva_Rafal_Auditory_Agnosia_CBN_v28n3_Sept2015.pdf. 
  37. "Cortical spatio-temporal dynamics underlying phonological target detection in humans". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 23 (6): 1437–46. June 2011. doi:10.1162/jocn.2010.21466. PMID 20465359. 
  38. "Resection of the medial temporal lobe disconnects the rostral superior temporal gyrus from some of its projection targets in the frontal lobe and thalamus". Cerebral Cortex 19 (9): 2114–30. September 2009. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhn236. PMID 19150921. 
  39. 39.0 39.1 "Auditory belt and parabelt projections to the prefrontal cortex in the rhesus monkey". The Journal of Comparative Neurology 403 (2): 141–57. January 1999. doi:10.1002/(sici)1096-9861(19990111)403:2<141::aid-cne1>3.0.co;2-v. PMID 9886040. 
  40. "Thalamic projections of the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex in the rhesus monkey (Macaca mulatta)". Brain Research 110 (1): 21–38. June 1976. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(76)90206-7. PMID 819108. 
  41. 41.0 41.1 "Voice cells in the primate temporal lobe". Current Biology 21 (16): 1408–15. August 2011. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2011.07.028. PMID 21835625. 
  42. 42.0 42.1 42.2 "A voice region in the monkey brain". Nature Neuroscience 11 (3): 367–74. March 2008. doi:10.1038/nn2043. PMID 18264095. 
  43. 43.0 43.1 "Species-specific calls evoke asymmetric activity in the monkey's temporal poles". Nature 427 (6973): 448–51. January 2004. doi:10.1038/nature02268. PMID 14749833. Bibcode2004Natur.427..448P. 
  44. "Neural representation of vocalizations in the primate ventrolateral prefrontal cortex". Journal of Neurophysiology 93 (2): 734–47. February 2005. doi:10.1152/jn.00675.2004. PMID 15371495. 
  45. "Coding of auditory-stimulus identity in the auditory non-spatial processing stream". Journal of Neurophysiology 99 (1): 87–95. January 2008. doi:10.1152/jn.01069.2007. PMID 18003874. 
  46. 46.0 46.1 "Representation of speech categories in the primate auditory cortex". Journal of Neurophysiology 105 (6): 2634–46. June 2011. doi:10.1152/jn.00037.2011. PMID 21346209. 
  47. "Chemoarchitectonics and corticocortical terminations within the superior temporal sulcus of the rhesus monkey: evidence for subdivisions of superior temporal polysensory cortex". The Journal of Comparative Neurology 360 (3): 513–35. September 1995. doi:10.1002/cne.903600312. PMID 8543656. 
  48. "Selectivity for the spatial and nonspatial attributes of auditory stimuli in the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex". The Journal of Neuroscience 24 (50): 11307–16. December 2004. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.3935-04.2004. PMID 15601937. 
  49. Deacon, Terrence W (February 1992). "Cortical connections of the inferior arcuate sulcus cortex in the macaque brain". Brain Research 573 (1): 8–26. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(92)90109-m. ISSN 0006-8993. PMID 1374284. 
  50. "Corticocortical connections of visual, sensorimotor, and multimodal processing areas in the parietal lobe of the macaque monkey". The Journal of Comparative Neurology 428 (1): 112–37. December 2000. doi:10.1002/1096-9861(20001204)428:1<112::aid-cne8>3.0.co;2-9. PMID 11058227. 
  51. "Forebrain connectivity of the prefrontal cortex in the marmoset monkey (Callithrix jacchus): an anterograde and retrograde tract-tracing study". The Journal of Comparative Neurology 502 (1): 86–112. May 2007. doi:10.1002/cne.21300. PMID 17335041. 
  52. 52.0 52.1 "Association fibre pathways of the brain: parallel observations from diffusion spectrum imaging and autoradiography". Brain 130 (Pt 3): 630–53. March 2007. doi:10.1093/brain/awl359. PMID 17293361. 
  53. "Further observations on parieto-temporal connections in the rhesus monkey". Experimental Brain Research 55 (2): 301–12. July 1984. doi:10.1007/bf00237280. PMID 6745368. 
  54. "Perisylvian language networks of the human brain". Annals of Neurology 57 (1): 8–16. January 2005. doi:10.1002/ana.20319. PMID 15597383. 
  55. "Dissociating the human language pathways with high angular resolution diffusion fiber tractography". The Journal of Neuroscience 28 (45): 11435–44. November 2008. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.2388-08.2008. PMID 18987180. 
  56. "Delineation of the middle longitudinal fascicle in humans: a quantitative, in vivo, DT-MRI study". Cerebral Cortex 19 (4): 777–85. April 2009. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhn124. PMID 18669591. 
  57. "Middle longitudinal fasciculus delineation within language pathways: a diffusion tensor imaging study in human". European Journal of Radiology 82 (1): 151–7. January 2013. doi:10.1016/j.ejrad.2012.05.034. PMID 23084876. 
  58. "The neural architecture of the language comprehension network: converging evidence from lesion and connectivity analyses". Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience 5: 1. 2011. doi:10.3389/fnsys.2011.00001. PMID 21347218. 
  59. "Ventral and dorsal pathways for language". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 105 (46): 18035–40. November 2008. doi:10.1073/pnas.0805234105. PMID 19004769. Bibcode2008PNAS..10518035S. 
  60. "Early stages of melody processing: stimulus-sequence and task-dependent neuronal activity in monkey auditory cortical fields A1 and R". Journal of Neurophysiology 100 (6): 3009–29. December 2008. doi:10.1152/jn.00828.2007. PMID 18842950. 
  61. "Intracortical responses in human and monkey primary auditory cortex support a temporal processing mechanism for encoding of the voice onset time phonetic parameter". Cerebral Cortex 15 (2): 170–86. February 2005. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhh120. PMID 15238437. 
  62. "Coding of auditory-stimulus identity in the auditory non-spatial processing stream". Journal of Neurophysiology 99 (1): 87–95. January 2008. doi:10.1152/jn.01069.2007. PMID 18003874. 
  63. "Processing of vocalizations in humans and monkeys: a comparative fMRI study". NeuroImage 62 (3): 1376–89. September 2012. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.05.070. PMID 22659478. https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-02327524/file/ProcessingVocalizations.pdf. 
  64. "Functional magnetic resonance imaging of a human auditory cortex area involved in foreground-background decomposition". The European Journal of Neuroscience 10 (2): 803–9. February 1998. doi:10.1046/j.1460-9568.1998.00086.x. PMID 9749748. 
  65. "Sensitivity to auditory object features in human temporal neocortex". The Journal of Neuroscience 24 (14): 3637–42. April 2004. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.5458-03.2004. PMID 15071112. 
  66. 66.0 66.1 "Where is the semantic system? A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies". Cerebral Cortex 19 (12): 2767–96. December 2009. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhp055. PMID 19329570. 
  67. "Hierarchical processing in spoken language comprehension". The Journal of Neuroscience 23 (8): 3423–31. April 2003. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.23-08-03423.2003. PMID 12716950. 
  68. "Neural substrates of phonemic perception". Cerebral Cortex 15 (10): 1621–31. October 2005. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhi040. PMID 15703256. 
  69. "Defining a left-lateralized response specific to intelligible speech using fMRI". Cerebral Cortex 13 (12): 1362–8. December 2003. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhg083. PMID 14615301. 
  70. "Vowel sound extraction in anterior superior temporal cortex". Human Brain Mapping 27 (7): 562–71. July 2006. doi:10.1002/hbm.20201. PMID 16281283. 
  71. "Multiple stages of auditory speech perception reflected in event-related FMRI". Cerebral Cortex 17 (10): 2251–7. October 2007. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhl133. PMID 17150986. 
  72. "Identification of a pathway for intelligible speech in the left temporal lobe". Brain 123 (12): 2400–6. December 2000. doi:10.1093/brain/123.12.2400. PMID 11099443. 
  73. "Adaptation to speaker's voice in right anterior temporal lobe". NeuroReport 14 (16): 2105–2109. November 2003. doi:10.1097/00001756-200311140-00019. PMID 14600506. 
  74. "Parametrically dissociating speech and nonspeech perception in the brain using fMRI". Brain and Language 78 (3): 364–96. September 2001. doi:10.1006/brln.2001.2484. PMID 11703063. 
  75. "Cortical representation of natural complex sounds: effects of acoustic features and auditory object category". The Journal of Neuroscience 30 (22): 7604–12. June 2010. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.0296-10.2010. PMID 20519535. 
  76. "Lefties get it "right" when hearing tool sounds". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 18 (8): 1314–30. August 2006. doi:10.1162/jocn.2006.18.8.1314. PMID 16859417. 
  77. "Distinct pathways involved in sound recognition and localization: a human fMRI study". NeuroImage 14 (4): 802–16. October 2001. doi:10.1006/nimg.2001.0888. PMID 11554799. http://infoscience.epfl.ch/record/86832/files/Maeder2003_155.pdf. 
  78. "Human auditory belt areas specialized in sound recognition: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study". NeuroReport 17 (16): 1659–62. November 2006. doi:10.1097/01.wnr.0000239962.75943.dd. PMID 17047449. http://infoscience.epfl.ch/record/91044/files/00001756-200611060-00001.pdf. 
  79. "The superior temporal sulcus differentiates communicative and noncommunicative auditory signals". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 24 (5): 1224–32. May 2012. doi:10.1162/jocn_a_00208. PMID 22360624. 
  80. "Phoneme and word recognition in the auditory ventral stream". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 109 (8): E505-14. February 2012. doi:10.1073/pnas.1113427109. PMID 22308358. 
  81. 81.0 81.1 81.2 81.3 "A blueprint for real-time functional mapping via human intracranial recordings". PLOS ONE 2 (10): e1094. October 2007. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0001094. PMID 17971857. Bibcode2007PLoSO...2.1094L. 
  82. 82.0 82.1 "Left anterior temporal cortex actively engages in speech perception: A direct cortical stimulation study". Neuropsychologia 49 (5): 1350–1354. April 2011. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.01.023. PMID 21251921. 
  83. 83.0 83.1 83.2 83.3 "Electrostimulation mapping of comprehension of auditory and visual words". Cortex; A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior 71: 398–408. October 2015. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2015.07.001. PMID 26332785. 
  84. "In search of an auditory engram". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 102 (26): 9359–64. June 2005. doi:10.1073/pnas.0503998102. PMID 15967995. Bibcode2005PNAS..102.9359F. 
  85. "The effect of temporal lobe and hippocampal lesions on auditory and visual recent memory in monkeys". Brain 83 (3): 470–489. 1960. doi:10.1093/brain/83.3.470. ISSN 0006-8950. 
  86. "Sequential auditory and visual discriminations after temporal lobe ablation in monkeys". Physiology & Behavior 24 (6): 1149–56. June 1980. doi:10.1016/0031-9384(80)90062-1. PMID 6774349. 
  87. "Dynamics of gamma-band activity in human magnetoencephalogram during auditory pattern working memory". NeuroImage 20 (2): 816–27. October 2003. doi:10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00350-1. PMID 14568454. 
  88. "Human dorsal and ventral auditory streams subserve rehearsal-based and echoic processes during verbal working memory". Neuron 48 (4): 687–97. November 2005. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2005.09.029. PMID 16301183. 
  89. "Monkeys have a limited form of short-term memory in audition". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 109 (30): 12237–41. July 2012. doi:10.1073/pnas.1209685109. PMID 22778411. Bibcode2012PNAS..10912237S. 
  90. "Temporal lobe lesions and semantic impairment: a comparison of herpes simplex virus encephalitis and semantic dementia". Brain 130 (Pt 4): 1138–47. April 2007. doi:10.1093/brain/awl344. PMID 17251241. 
  91. "Where do you know what you know? The representation of semantic knowledge in the human brain". Nature Reviews. Neuroscience 8 (12): 976–87. December 2007. doi:10.1038/nrn2277. PMID 18026167. 
  92. "Anterior temporal involvement in semantic word retrieval: voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping evidence from aphasia". Brain 132 (Pt 12): 3411–27. December 2009. doi:10.1093/brain/awp284. PMID 19942676. 
  93. "Distribution of auditory and visual naming sites in nonlesional temporal lobe epilepsy patients and patients with space-occupying temporal lobe lesions". Epilepsia 48 (3): 531–8. March 2007. doi:10.1111/j.1528-1167.2006.00955.x. PMID 17326797. 
  94. 94.0 94.1 94.2 "The anatomo-functional connectivity of language revisited. New insights provided by electrostimulation and tractography". Neuropsychologia 46 (4): 927–34. March 2008. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.10.025. PMID 18093622. 
  95. "Meta-analyzing left hemisphere language areas: phonology, semantics, and sentence processing". NeuroImage 30 (4): 1414–32. May 2006. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.11.002. PMID 16413796. 
  96. 96.0 96.1 "Neuronal activity in the human lateral temporal lobe. I. Responses to speech". Experimental Brain Research 77 (3): 451–75. October 1989. doi:10.1007/bf00249600. PMID 2806441. 
  97. "The cortical representation of speech". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 5 (4): 467–79. October 1993. doi:10.1162/jocn.1993.5.4.467. PMID 23964919. http://www.archipel.uqam.ca/2942/1/The_cortical_representation_of_speech.pdf. 
  98. "Response of anterior temporal cortex to syntactic and prosodic manipulations during sentence processing". Human Brain Mapping 26 (2): 128–38. October 2005. doi:10.1002/hbm.20148. PMID 15895428. 
  99. "Role of anterior temporal cortex in auditory sentence comprehension: an fMRI study". NeuroReport 12 (8): 1749–52. June 2001. doi:10.1097/00001756-200106130-00046. PMID 11409752. 
  100. "The response of left temporal cortex to sentences". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 14 (4): 550–60. May 2002. doi:10.1162/08989290260045800. PMID 12126497. 
  101. "The role of left inferior frontal and superior temporal cortex in sentence comprehension: localizing syntactic and semantic processes". Cerebral Cortex 13 (2): 170–7. February 2003. doi:10.1093/cercor/13.2.170. PMID 12507948. 
  102. "Language in context: emergent features of word, sentence, and narrative comprehension". NeuroImage 25 (3): 1002–15. 2005. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.013. PMID 15809000. 
  103. "Selective attention to semantic and syntactic features modulates sentence processing networks in anterior temporal cortex". Cerebral Cortex 19 (4): 786–96. April 2009. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhn126. PMID 18669589. 
  104. "Cortical representation of the constituent structure of sentences". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 108 (6): 2522–7. February 2011. doi:10.1073/pnas.1018711108. PMID 21224415. 
  105. "Syntactic structure building in the anterior temporal lobe during natural story listening". Brain and Language 120 (2): 163–73. February 2012. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2010.04.002. PMID 20472279. 
  106. "Differentiation of syntactic processes in the left and right anterior temporal lobe: Event-related brain potential evidence from lesion patients". Brain and Language 87 (1): 135–136. October 2003. doi:10.1016/s0093-934x(03)00236-0. 
  107. "Language processing and working memory: Neuropsychological evidence for separate phonological and semantic capacities.". Journal of Memory and Language 33 (1): 83–111. February 1994. doi:10.1006/jmla.1994.1005. 
  108. "Damage to left anterior temporal cortex predicts impairment of complex syntactic processing: a lesion-symptom mapping study". Human Brain Mapping 34 (10): 2715–23. October 2013. doi:10.1002/hbm.22096. PMID 22522937. 
  109. "Neurobiological roots of language in primate audition: common computational properties". Trends in Cognitive Sciences 19 (3): 142–50. March 2015. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2014.12.008. PMID 25600585. 
  110. "Bilateral capacity for speech sound processing in auditory comprehension: evidence from Wada procedures". Brain and Language 107 (3): 179–84. December 2008. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2008.09.006. PMID 18976806. 
  111. Zaidel, Eran (September 1976). "Auditory Vocabulary of the Right Hemisphere Following Brain Bisection or Hemidecortication". Cortex 12 (3): 191–211. doi:10.1016/s0010-9452(76)80001-9. ISSN 0010-9452. PMID 1000988. 
  112. "Pure word deafness and the bilateral processing of the speech code". Cognitive Science 25 (5): 679–693. October 2001. doi:10.1016/s0364-0213(01)00050-7. 
  113. "Interhemispheric functional relationships in auditory agnosia. An analysis of the preconditions and a conceptual model". Brain and Language 5 (3): 286–300. May 1978. doi:10.1016/0093-934x(78)90027-5. PMID 656899. 
  114. "TMS produces two dissociable types of speech disruption". NeuroImage 13 (3): 472–8. March 2001. doi:10.1006/nimg.2000.0701. PMID 11170812. https://research.gold.ac.uk/60/1/PSY-Stewart2001h_GRO.pdf. 
  115. "A common neural substrate for language production and verbal working memory". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 23 (6): 1358–67. June 2011. doi:10.1162/jocn.2010.21519. PMID 20617889. 
  116. "Movement intention after parietal cortex stimulation in humans". Science 324 (5928): 811–3. May 2009. doi:10.1126/science.1169896. PMID 19423830. Bibcode2009Sci...324..811D. 
  117. "Spatiotemporal imaging of cortical activation during verb generation and picture naming". NeuroImage 50 (1): 291–301. March 2010. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.035. PMID 20026224. 
  118. "Transcortical sensory aphasia: revisited and revised". Brain 123 (8): 1634–42. August 2000. doi:10.1093/brain/123.8.1634. PMID 10908193. 
  119. 119.0 119.1 119.2 "Localization of sublexical speech perception components". Brain and Language 114 (1): 1–15. July 2010. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2010.03.008. PMID 20413149. 
  120. "Categorical speech representation in human superior temporal gyrus" (in En). Nature Neuroscience 13 (11): 1428–32. November 2010. doi:10.1038/nn.2641. PMID 20890293. 
  121. "Role of left posterior superior temporal gyrus in phonological processing for speech perception and production". Cognitive Science 25 (5): 663–678. September 2001. doi:10.1207/s15516709cog2505_2. ISSN 0364-0213. 
  122. "Separate neural subsystems within 'Wernicke's area'". Brain 124 (Pt 1): 83–95. January 2001. doi:10.1093/brain/124.1.83. PMID 11133789. 
  123. "Auditory-motor interaction revealed by fMRI: speech, music, and working memory in area Spt". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 15 (5): 673–82. July 2003. doi:10.1162/089892903322307393. PMID 12965041. 
  124. "Sounds do-able: auditory-motor transformations and the posterior temporal plane". Trends in Neurosciences 28 (12): 636–43. December 2005. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2005.09.010. PMID 16216346. 
  125. "The cortical organization of speech processing". Nature Reviews. Neuroscience 8 (5): 393–402. May 2007. doi:10.1038/nrn2113. PMID 17431404. 
  126. "Cerebral networks and functional brain asymmetry: evidence from regional metabolic changes during word repetition". Brain and Language 63 (1): 108–21. June 1998. doi:10.1006/brln.1997.1937. PMID 9642023. 
  127. "The constraints functional neuroimaging places on classical models of auditory word processing". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 13 (6): 754–65. August 2001. doi:10.1162/08989290152541421. PMID 11564320. 
  128. "The left posterior superior temporal gyrus participates specifically in accessing lexical phonology". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 20 (9): 1698–710. September 2008. doi:10.1162/jocn.2008.20113. PMID 18345989. 
  129. 129.0 129.1 "ECoG gamma activity during a language task: differentiating expressive and receptive speech areas". Brain 131 (Pt 8): 2013–27. August 2008. doi:10.1093/brain/awn147. PMID 18669510. 
  130. "The critical role of Wernicke's area in sentence repetition". Annals of Neurology 17 (6): 549–57. June 1985. doi:10.1002/ana.410170604. PMID 4026225. 
  131. "Supra- and infrasylvian conduction aphasia". Brain and Language 76 (3): 317–31. March 2001. doi:10.1006/brln.2000.2425. PMID 11247647. 
  132. "Acute conduction aphasia: an analysis of 20 cases". Brain and Language 85 (1): 93–108. April 2003. doi:10.1016/s0093-934x(02)00502-3. PMID 12681350. 
  133. "Brain Regions Underlying Repetition and Auditory-Verbal Short-term Memory Deficits in Aphasia: Evidence from Voxel-based Lesion Symptom Mapping". Aphasiology 26 (3–4): 338–354. March 2012. doi:10.1080/02687038.2011.602391. PMID 24976669. 
  134. "It's either a cook or a baker: patients with conduction aphasia get the gist but lose the trace". Brain and Language 105 (2): 134–40. May 2008. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2007.12.007. PMID 18243294. 
  135. "Impaired speech repetition and left parietal lobe damage". The Journal of Neuroscience 30 (33): 11057–61. August 2010. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.1120-10.2010. PMID 20720112. 
  136. "Conduction aphasia, sensory-motor integration, and phonological short-term memory - an aggregate analysis of lesion and fMRI data". Brain and Language 119 (3): 119–28. December 2011. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2010.12.001. PMID 21256582. 
  137. "MR tractography depicting damage to the arcuate fasciculus in a patient with conduction aphasia". Neurology 68 (10): 789. March 2007. doi:10.1212/01.wnl.0000256348.65744.b2. PMID 17339591. 
  138. "Language dysfunction after stroke and damage to white matter tracts evaluated using diffusion tensor imaging". AJNR. American Journal of Neuroradiology 29 (3): 483–7. March 2008. doi:10.3174/ajnr.A0846. PMID 18039757. 
  139. "Diffusion tensor imaging depicting damage to the arcuate fasciculus in patients with conduction aphasia: a study of the Wernicke-Geschwind model". Neurological Research 32 (7): 775–8. September 2010. doi:10.1179/016164109x12478302362653. PMID 19825277. 
  140. "Sensory-to-motor integration during auditory repetition: a combined fMRI and lesion study". Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 8: 24. 2014. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2014.00024. PMID 24550807. 
  141. "Conduction aphasia elicited by stimulation of the left posterior superior temporal gyrus". Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 66 (3): 393–6. March 1999. doi:10.1136/jnnp.66.3.393. PMID 10084542. 
  142. "Conduction aphasia as a function of the dominant posterior perisylvian cortex. Report of two cases" (in en-US). Journal of Neurosurgery 104 (5): 845–8. May 2006. doi:10.3171/jns.2006.104.5.845. PMID 16703895. 
  143. Service, Elisabet; Kohonen, Viljo (April 1995). "Is the relation between phonological memory and foreign language learning accounted for by vocabulary acquisition?". Applied Psycholinguistics 16 (2): 155–172. doi:10.1017/S0142716400007062. 
  144. "Phonology, working memory, and foreign-language learning". The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. A, Human Experimental Psychology 45 (1): 21–50. July 1992. doi:10.1080/14640749208401314. PMID 1636010. 
  145. "Functional connectivity in the human language system: a cortico-cortical evoked potential study". Brain 127 (Pt 10): 2316–30. October 2004. doi:10.1093/brain/awh246. PMID 15269116. 
  146. "The relation between oral movement control and speech". Brain and Language 37 (4): 565–90. November 1989. doi:10.1016/0093-934x(89)90112-0. PMID 2479446. 
  147. "Neural mechanisms underlying auditory feedback control of speech". NeuroImage 39 (3): 1429–43. February 2008. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.09.054. PMID 18035557. 
  148. "Categorical speech representation in human superior temporal gyrus". Nature Neuroscience 13 (11): 1428–32. November 2010. doi:10.1038/nn.2641. PMID 20890293. 
  149. "A neural basis for interindividual differences in the McGurk effect, a multisensory speech illusion". NeuroImage 59 (1): 781–7. January 2012. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.07.024. PMID 21787869. 
  150. "fMRI-Guided transcranial magnetic stimulation reveals that the superior temporal sulcus is a cortical locus of the McGurk effect". The Journal of Neuroscience 30 (7): 2414–7. February 2010. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4865-09.2010. PMID 20164324. 
  151. "Speech comprehension aided by multiple modalities: behavioural and neural interactions". Neuropsychologia 50 (5): 762–76. April 2012. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.01.010. PMID 22266262. 
  152. "Audiovisual integration in human superior temporal sulcus: Inverse effectiveness and the neural processing of speech and object recognition". NeuroImage 44 (3): 1210–23. February 2009. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.09.034. PMID 18973818. 
  153. "Visual phonetic processing localized using speech and nonspeech face gestures in video and point-light displays". Human Brain Mapping 32 (10): 1660–76. October 2011. doi:10.1002/hbm.21139. PMID 20853377. 
  154. "The processing of audio-visual speech: empirical and neural bases". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences 363 (1493): 1001–10. March 2008. doi:10.1098/rstb.2007.2155. PMID 17827105. 
  155. "The dorsal stream contribution to phonological retrieval in object naming". Brain 135 (Pt 12): 3799–814. December 2012. doi:10.1093/brain/aws300. PMID 23171662. 
  156. "Anterior temporal involvement in semantic word retrieval: voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping evidence from aphasia". Brain 132 (Pt 12): 3411–27. December 2009. doi:10.1093/brain/awp284. PMID 19942676. 
  157. Ojemann, George A. (June 1983). "Brain organization for language from the perspective of electrical stimulation mapping". Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 (2): 189–206. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00015491. ISSN 1469-1825. 
  158. "Learning new names for new objects: cortical effects as measured by magnetoencephalography". Brain and Language 89 (3): 617–22. June 2004. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2003.12.007. PMID 15120553. 
  159. "Phonological decisions require both the left and right supramarginal gyri". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 107 (38): 16494–9. September 2010. doi:10.1073/pnas.1008121107. PMID 20807747. Bibcode2010PNAS..10716494H. 
  160. "Adult brain plasticity elicited by anomia treatment". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 15 (3): 444–61. April 2003. doi:10.1162/089892903321593153. PMID 12729495. https://aaltodoc.aalto.fi/handle/123456789/30995. 
  161. "Neurolinguistics: structural plasticity in the bilingual brain". Nature 431 (7010): 757. October 2004. doi:10.1038/431757a. PMID 15483594. Bibcode2004Natur.431..757M. 
  162. "Exploring cross-linguistic vocabulary effects on brain structures using voxel-based morphometry". Bilingualism 10 (2): 189–199. July 2007. doi:10.1017/S1366728907002933. PMID 18418473. 
  163. Willness, Chelsea (2016-01-08). "The Oxford handbook of organizational climate and culture By Benjamin Schneider & Karen M. Barbera (Eds.) New York, NY: Oxford University Press, 2014. ISBN 978-0-19-986071-5". British Journal of Psychology 107 (1): 201–202. doi:10.1111/bjop.12170. 
  164. "Anatomical traces of vocabulary acquisition in the adolescent brain". The Journal of Neuroscience 27 (5): 1184–9. January 2007. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4442-06.2007. PMID 17267574. 
  165. "Contrasting effects of vocabulary knowledge on temporal and parietal brain structure across lifespan". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 22 (5): 943–54. May 2010. doi:10.1162/jocn.2009.21238. PMID 19366285. 
  166. "Evaluation of the dual route theory of reading: a metanalysis of 35 neuroimaging studies". NeuroImage 20 (2): 693–712. October 2003. doi:10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00343-4. PMID 14568445. 
  167. "Cross-cultural effect on the brain revisited: universal structures plus writing system variation" (in fr). Human Brain Mapping 25 (1): 92–104. May 2005. doi:10.1002/hbm.20124. PMID 15846818. 
  168. "Reading disorders in primary progressive aphasia: a behavioral and neuroimaging study". Neuropsychologia 47 (8–9): 1893–900. July 2009. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.02.033. PMID 19428421. 
  169. 169.0 169.1 Baddeley, Alan; Lewis, Vivien; Vallar, Giuseppe (May 1984). "Exploring the Articulatory Loop". The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A 36 (2): 233–252. doi:10.1080/14640748408402157. 
  170. 170.0 170.1 "The magical number 4 in short-term memory: a reconsideration of mental storage capacity". The Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24 (1): 87–114; discussion 114–85. February 2001. doi:10.1017/S0140525X01003922. PMID 11515286. 
  171. "Articulatory and phonological determinants of word length effects in span tasks". The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. A, Human Experimental Psychology 45 (2): 177–92. August 1992. doi:10.1080/14640749208401323. PMID 1410554. 
  172. "The role of high-level speech planning in rehearsal: Evidence from patients with apraxia of speech". Journal of Memory and Language 31 (1): 54–73. February 1992. doi:10.1016/0749-596x(92)90005-i. 
  173. "The role of the output phonological buffer in the control of speech timing: a single case study". Cortex; A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior 31 (3): 469–86. September 1995. doi:10.1016/s0010-9452(13)80060-3. PMID 8536476. 
  174. "The selective impairment of the phonological output buffer". Cognitive Neuropsychology 17 (6): 517–46. September 2000. doi:10.1080/02643290050110638. PMID 20945193. 
  175. "The selective impairment of the phonological output buffer: evidence from a Chinese patient". Behavioural Neurology 16 (2–3): 179–89. 2005. doi:10.1155/2005/647871. PMID 16410633. 
  176. Oberauer, Klaus (2002). "Access to information in working memory: Exploring the focus of attention.". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 28 (3): 411–421. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.28.3.411. PMID 12018494. 
  177. "The nature of individual differences in working memory capacity: active maintenance in primary memory and controlled search from secondary memory". Psychological Review 114 (1): 104–32. January 2007. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.114.1.104. PMID 17227183. 
  178. "As Time Goes By". Current Directions in Psychological Science 21 (6): 413–419. December 2012. doi:10.1177/0963721412459513. https://archive-ouverte.unige.ch/unige:88269. 
  179. "Neurobiological roots of language in primate audition: common computational properties". Trends in Cognitive Sciences 19 (3): 142–50. March 2015. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2014.12.008. PMID 25600585. 
  180. "The search for the phonological store: from loop to convolution". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 20 (5): 762–78. May 2008. doi:10.1162/jocn.2008.20501. PMID 18201133. 
  181. "Populations of auditory cortical neurons can accurately encode acoustic space across stimulus intensity". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 106 (14): 5931–5. April 2009. doi:10.1073/pnas.0901023106. PMID 19321750. Bibcode2009PNAS..106.5931M. 
  182. "Functional specialization in rhesus monkey auditory cortex". Science 292 (5515): 290–3. April 2001. doi:10.1126/science.1058911. PMID 11303104. Bibcode2001Sci...292..290T. 
  183. ""What" and "where" in the human auditory system". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 98 (21): 12301–6. October 2001. doi:10.1073/pnas.211209098. PMID 11572938. Bibcode2001PNAS...9812301A. 
  184. "Automatic and intrinsic auditory "what" and "where" processing in humans revealed by electrical neuroimaging". Cerebral Cortex 17 (1): 9–17. January 2007. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhj119. PMID 16421326. 
  185. "Response preferences for "what" and "where" in human non-primary auditory cortex". NeuroImage 32 (2): 968–77. August 2006. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.03.050. PMID 16733092. 
  186. "Responses to auditory stimuli in macaque lateral intraparietal area. II. Behavioral modulation". Journal of Neurophysiology 82 (1): 343–58. July 1999. doi:10.1152/jn.1999.82.1.343. PMID 10400963. 
  187. "Spatially tuned auditory responses in area LIP of macaques performing delayed memory saccades to acoustic targets". Journal of Neurophysiology 75 (3): 1233–41. March 1996. doi:10.1152/jn.1996.75.3.1233. PMID 8867131. 
  188. "A blueprint for real-time functional mapping via human intracranial recordings". PLOS ONE 2 (10): e1094. October 2007. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0001094. PMID 17971857. Bibcode2007PLoSO...2.1094L. 
  189. "Assessing fetal response to maternal speech using a noninvasive functional brain imaging technique". International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience 30 (2): 159–61. April 2012. doi:10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2011.11.002. PMID 22123457. 
  190. "From where to what: a neuroanatomically based evolutionary model of the emergence of speech in humans". F1000Research 4: 67. 2017-09-20. doi:10.12688/f1000research.6175.3. PMID 28928931. 
  191. "From Mimicry to Language: A Neuroanatomically Based Evolutionary Model of the Emergence of Vocal Language". Frontiers in Neuroscience 10: 307. 2016-06-30. doi:10.3389/fnins.2016.00307. PMID 27445676. 
  192. 192.0 192.1 192.2 192.3 192.4 Suri, Sana. "What sign language teaches us about the brain" (in en). http://theconversation.com/what-sign-language-teaches-us-about-the-brain-29628. 
  193. 193.0 193.1 193.2 Scientific American. (2002). Sign language in the brain. [Brochure]. Retrieved from http://lcn.salk.edu/Brochure/SciAM%20ASL.pdf
  194. 194.00 194.01 194.02 194.03 194.04 194.05 194.06 194.07 194.08 194.09 194.10 194.11 "Are There Separate Neural Systems for Spelling? New Insights into the Role of Rules and Memory in Spelling from Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging". Mind, Brain and Education 1 (1): 48–59. March 2007. doi:10.1111/j.1751-228X.2007.00005.x. PMID 20011680. 
  195. 195.0 195.1 195.2 195.3 195.4 Treiman, Rebecca; Kessler, Brett (2007). Writing Systems and Spelling Development. Blackwell Publishing Ltd. pp. 120–134. doi:10.1002/9780470757642.ch7. ISBN 978-0-470-75764-2.