Working memory

From HandWiki
Short description: Cognitive system for temporarily holding information

Working memory is a cognitive system with a limited capacity that can hold information temporarily.[1] It is important for reasoning and the guidance of decision-making and behavior.[2][3] Working memory is often used synonymously with short-term memory, but some theorists consider the two forms of memory distinct, assuming that working memory allows for the manipulation of stored information, whereas short-term memory only refers to the short-term storage of information.[2][4] Working memory is a theoretical concept central to cognitive psychology, neuropsychology, and neuroscience.

History

The term "working memory" was coined by Miller, Galanter, and Pribram,[5][6] and was used in the 1960s in the context of theories that likened the mind to a computer. In 1968, Atkinson and Shiffrin[7] used the term to describe their "short-term store". The term short-term store was the name previously used for working memory. Other suggested names were short-term memory, primary memory, immediate memory, operant memory, and provisional memory.[8] Short-term memory is the ability to remember information over a brief period (in the order of seconds). Most theorists today use the concept of working memory to replace or include the older concept of short-term memory, marking a stronger emphasis on the notion of manipulating information rather than mere maintenance.[citation needed]

The earliest mention of experiments on the neural basis of working memory can be traced back to more than 100 years ago, when Hitzig and Ferrier described ablation experiments of the prefrontal cortex (PFC); they concluded that the frontal cortex was important for cognitive rather than sensory processes.[9] In 1935 and 1936, Carlyle Jacobsen and colleagues were the first to show the deleterious effect of prefrontal ablation on delayed response.[9][10]

Theories

Numerous models have been proposed for how working memory functions, both anatomically and cognitively. Of those, the two that have been most influential are summarized below.

The multicomponent model

Main page: Biology:Baddeley's model of working memory
Baddeley and Hitch's model of working memory

In 1974 Baddeley and Hitch[11] introduced the multicomponent model of working memory. The theory proposed a model containing three components: the central executive, the phonological loop, and the visuospatial sketchpad with the central executive functioning as a control center of sorts, directing info between the phonological and visuospatial components.[12] The central executive is responsible for, among other things, directing attention to relevant information, suppressing irrelevant information and inappropriate actions, and coordinating cognitive processes when more than one task is simultaneously performed. A "central executive" is responsible for supervising the integration of information and for coordinating subordinate systems responsible for the short-term maintenance of information. One subordinate system, the phonological loop (PL), stores phonological information (that is, the sound of language) and prevents its decay by continuously refreshing it in a rehearsal loop. It can, for example, maintain a seven-digit telephone number for as long as one repeats the number to oneself repeatedly.[13] The other subordinate system, the visuospatial sketchpad, stores visual and spatial information. It can be used, for example, for constructing and manipulating visual images and for representing mental maps. The sketchpad can be further broken down into a visual subsystem (dealing with such phenomena as shape, colour, and texture), and a spatial subsystem (dealing with location).[citation needed]

In 2000 Baddeley extended the model by adding a fourth component, the episodic buffer, which holds representations that integrate phonological, visual, and spatial information, and possibly information not covered by the subordinate systems (e.g., semantic information, musical information). The episodic buffer is also the link between working memory and long-term memory.[14] The component is episodic because it is assumed to bind information into a unitary episodic representation. The episodic buffer resembles Tulving's concept of episodic memory, but it differs in that the episodic buffer is a temporary store.[15]

Working memory as part of long-term memory

Central Executive
Long-term Memory
The central executive of working memory is retrieving memory from long-term memory.

Anders Ericsson and Walter Kintsch[16] have introduced the notion of "long-term working memory", which they define as a set of "retrieval structures" in long-term memory that enable seamless access to the information relevant for everyday tasks. In this way, parts of long-term memory effectively function as working memory. In a similar vein, Cowan does not regard working memory as a separate system from long-term memory. Representations in working memory are a subset of representations in long-term memory. Working memory is organized into two embedded levels. The first consists of long-term memory representations that are activated. There can be many of these—there is theoretically no limit to the activation of representations in long-term memory. The second level is called the focus of attention. The focus is regarded as having a limited capacity and holds up to four of the activated representations.[17]

Oberauer has extended Cowan's model by adding a third component—a more narrow focus of attention that holds only one chunk at a time. The one-element focus is embedded in the four-element focus and serves to select a single chunk for processing. For example, four digits can be held in mind at the same time in Cowan's "focus of attention". When the individual wishes to perform a process on each of these digits—for example, adding the number two to each digit—separate processing is required for each digit since most individuals cannot perform several mathematical processes in parallel.[18] Oberauer's attentional component selects one of the digits for processing and then shifts the attentional focus to the next digit, continuing until all digits have been processed.[19]

Capacity

Working memory is widely acknowledged as having limited capacity. An early quantification of the capacity limit associated with short-term memory was the "magical number seven" suggested by Miller in 1956.[20] Miller claimed that the information-processing capacity of young adults is around seven elements, referred to as "chunks", regardless of whether the elements are digits, letters, words, or other units. Later research revealed this number depends on the category of chunks used (e.g., span may be around seven for digits, six for letters, and five for words), and even on features of the chunks within a category. For instance, attention span is lower for longer words than short words. In general, memory span for verbal contents (digits, letters, words, etc.) depends on the phonological complexity of the content (i.e., the number of phonemes, the number of syllables),[21] and on the lexical status of the contents (whether the contents are words known to the person or not).[22] Several other factors affect a person's measured span, and therefore it is difficult to pin down the capacity of short-term or working memory to a number of chunks. Nonetheless, Cowan proposed that working memory has a capacity of about four chunks in young adults (and fewer in children and old adults).[23]

In the visual domain, some investigations report no fixed capacity limit with respect to the total number of items that can be held in working memory. Instead, the results argue for a limited resource that can be flexibly shared between items retained in memory (see below in Resource theories), with some items in the focus of attention being allocated more resource and recalled with greater precision.[24][25][26][27]

Whereas most adults can repeat about seven digits in correct order, some individuals have shown impressive enlargements of their digit span—up to 80 digits. This feat is possible by extensive training on an encoding strategy by which the digits in a list are grouped (usually in groups of three to five) and these groups are encoded as a single unit (a chunk). For this to succeed, participants must be able to recognize the groups as some known string of digits. One person studied by Ericsson and his colleagues, for example, used an extensive knowledge of racing times from the history of sports in the process of coding chunks: several such chunks could then be combined into a higher-order chunk, forming a hierarchy of chunks. In this way, only some chunks at the highest level of the hierarchy must be retained in working memory, and for retrieval the chunks are unpacked. That is, the chunks in working memory act as retrieval cues that point to the digits they contain. Practicing memory skills such as these does not expand working memory capacity proper: it is the capacity to transfer (and retrieve) information from long-term memory that is improved, according to Ericsson and Kintsch (1995; see also Gobet & Simon, 2000[28]).

Measures and correlates

Working memory capacity can be tested by a variety of tasks. A commonly used measure is a dual-task paradigm, combining a memory span measure with a concurrent processing task, sometimes referred to as "complex span". Daneman and Carpenter invented the first version of this kind of task, the "reading span", in 1980.[29] Subjects read a number of sentences (usually between two and six) and tried to remember the last word of each sentence. At the end of the list of sentences, they repeated back the words in their correct order. Other tasks that do not have this dual-task nature have also been shown to be good measures of working memory capacity.[30] Whereas Daneman and Carpenter believed that the combination of "storage" (maintenance) and processing is needed to measure working memory capacity, we know now that the capacity of working memory can be measured with short-term memory tasks that have no additional processing component.[31][32] Conversely, working memory capacity can also be measured with certain processing tasks that don't involve maintenance of information.[33][34] The question of what features a task must have to qualify as a good measure of working memory capacity is a topic of ongoing research.

Recently, several studies of visual working memory have used delayed response tasks. These use analogue responses in a continuous space, rather than a binary (correct/incorrect) recall method, as often used in visual change detection tasks. Instead of asking participants to report whether a change occurred between the memory and probe array, delayed reproduction tasks require them to reproduce the precise quality of a visual feature, e.g. an object's location, orientation or colour.[24][25][26][27] In addition, the combination of visual perception such as within objects and colors can be used to improve memory strategy through elaboration, thus creating reinforcement within the capacity of working memory.[35]

Measures of working-memory capacity are strongly related to performance in other complex cognitive tasks, such as reading comprehension, problem solving, and with measures of intelligence quotient.[36]

Some researchers have argued[37] that working-memory capacity reflects the efficiency of executive functions, most notably the ability to maintain multiple task-relevant representations in the face of distracting irrelevant information; and that such tasks seem to reflect individual differences in the ability to focus and maintain attention, particularly when other events are serving to capture attention. Both working memory and executive functions rely strongly, though not exclusively, on frontal brain areas.[38]

Other researchers have argued that the capacity of working memory is better characterized as the ability to mentally form relations between elements, or to grasp relations in given information. This idea has been advanced, among others, by Graeme Halford, who illustrated it by our limited ability to understand statistical interactions between variables.[39] These authors asked people to compare written statements about the relations between several variables to graphs illustrating the same or a different relation, as in the following sentence: "If the cake is from France, then it has more sugar if it is made with chocolate than if it is made with cream, but if the cake is from Italy, then it has more sugar if it is made with cream than if it is made of chocolate". This statement describes a relation between three variables (country, ingredient, and amount of sugar), which is the maximum most individuals can understand. The capacity limit apparent here is obviously not a memory limit (all relevant information can be seen continuously) but a limit to how many relationships are discerned simultaneously.[citation needed]

Experimental studies of working-memory capacity

There are several hypotheses about the nature of the capacity limit. One is that a limited pool of cognitive resources is needed to keep representations active and thereby available for processing, and for carrying out processes.[40] Another hypothesis is that memory traces in working memory decay within a few seconds, unless refreshed through rehearsal, and because the speed of rehearsal is limited, we can maintain only a limited amount of information.[41] Yet another idea is that representations held in working memory interfere with each other.[42]

Decay theories

The assumption that the contents of short-term or working memory decay over time, unless decay is prevented by rehearsal, goes back to the early days of experimental research on short-term memory.[43][44] It is also an important assumption in the multi-component theory of working memory.[45] The most elaborate decay-based theory of working memory to date is the "time-based resource sharing model".[46] This theory assumes that representations in working memory decay unless they are refreshed. Refreshing them requires an attentional mechanism that is also needed for any concurrent processing task. When there are small time intervals in which the processing task does not require attention, this time can be used to refresh memory traces. The theory therefore predicts that the amount of forgetting depends on the temporal density of attentional demands of the processing task—this density is called "cognitive load". The cognitive load depends on two variables, the rate at which the processing task requires individual steps to be carried out, and the duration of each step. For example, if the processing task consists of adding digits, then having to add another digit every half-second places a higher cognitive load on the system than having to add another digit every two seconds. In a series of experiments, Barrouillet and colleagues have shown that memory for lists of letters depends neither on the number of processing steps nor the total time of processing but on cognitive load.[47]

Resource theories

Resource theories assume that the capacity of working memory is a limited resource that must be shared between all representations that need to be maintained in working memory simultaneously.[24] Some resource theorists also assume that maintenance and concurrent processing share the same resource;[40] this can explain why maintenance is typically impaired by a concurrent processing demand. Resource theories have been very successful in explaining data from tests of working memory for simple visual features, such as colors or orientations of bars. An ongoing debate is whether the resource is a continuous quantity that can be subdivided among any number of items in working memory, or whether it consists of a small number of discrete "slots", each of which can be assigned to one memory item, so that only a limited number of about 3 items can be maintained in working memory at all.[48]

Interference theories

Several forms of interference have been discussed by theorists. One of the oldest ideas is that new items simply replace older ones in working memory. Another form of interference is retrieval competition. For example, when the task is to remember a list of 7 words in their order, we need to start recall with the first word. While trying to retrieve the first word, the second word, which is represented in proximity, is accidentally retrieved as well, and the two compete for being recalled. Errors in serial recall tasks are often confusions of neighboring items on a memory list (so-called transpositions), showing that retrieval competition plays a role in limiting our ability to recall lists in order, and probably also in other working memory tasks. A third form of interference is the distortion of representations by superposition: When multiple representations are added on top of each other, each of them is blurred by the presence of all the others.[49] A fourth form of interference assumed by some authors is feature overwriting.[50][51] The idea is that each word, digit, or other item in working memory is represented as a bundle of features, and when two items share some features, one of them steals the features from the other. The more items are held in working memory, and the more their features overlap, the more each of them will be degraded by the loss of some features.[citation needed]

Limitations

None of these hypotheses can explain the experimental data entirely. The resource hypothesis, for example, was meant to explain the trade-off between maintenance and processing: The more information must be maintained in working memory, the slower and more error prone concurrent processes become, and with a higher demand on concurrent processing memory suffers. This trade-off has been investigated by tasks like the reading-span task described above. It has been found that the amount of trade-off depends on the similarity of the information to be remembered and the information to be processed. For example, remembering numbers while processing spatial information, or remembering spatial information while processing numbers, impair each other much less than when material of the same kind must be remembered and processed.[52] Also, remembering words and processing digits, or remembering digits and processing words, is easier than remembering and processing materials of the same category.[53] These findings are also difficult to explain for the decay hypothesis, because decay of memory representations should depend only on how long the processing task delays rehearsal or recall, not on the content of the processing task. A further problem for the decay hypothesis comes from experiments in which the recall of a list of letters was delayed, either by instructing participants to recall at a slower pace, or by instructing them to say an irrelevant word once or three times in between recall of each letter. Delaying recall had virtually no effect on recall accuracy.[54][55] The interference theory seems to fare best with explaining why the similarity between memory contents and the contents of concurrent processing tasks affects how much they impair each other. More similar materials are more likely to be confused, leading to retrieval competition.

Development

The capacity of working memory increases gradually over childhood[56] and declines gradually in old age.[57]

Childhood

Main page: Philosophy:Neo-Piagetian theories of cognitive development

Measures of performance on tests of working memory increase continuously between early childhood and adolescence, while the structure of correlations between different tests remains largely constant.[56] Starting with work in the Neo-Piagetian tradition,[58][59] theorists have argued that the growth of working-memory capacity is a major driving force of cognitive development. This hypothesis has received substantial empirical support from studies showing that the capacity of working memory is a strong predictor of cognitive abilities in childhood.[60] Particularly strong evidence for a role of working memory for development comes from a longitudinal study showing that working-memory capacity at one age predicts reasoning ability at a later age.[61] Studies in the Neo-Piagetian tradition have added to this picture by analyzing the complexity of cognitive tasks in terms of the number of items or relations that have to be considered simultaneously for a solution. Across a broad range of tasks, children manage task versions of the same level of complexity at about the same age, consistent with the view that working memory capacity limits the complexity they can handle at a given age.[62] An experiment has correlated that a decrease of complexity regarding capacity limits are articulated from the implications that research concerning language processes, have an effect on the capacity that the children who have developed language disorders, have performed lower then their age-matched peers. A correlation between memory storage deficits can be viewed as a contribution due to these language disorders or causing the language disorder, but hasn't fully suggested a deficit in being able to rehearse information.[63]

Although neuroscience studies support the notion that children rely on prefrontal cortex for performing various working memory tasks, an fMRI meta-analysis on children compared to adults performing the n back task revealed lack of consistent prefrontal cortex activation in children, while posterior regions including the insular cortex and cerebellum remain intact.[64]

Aging

Working memory is among the cognitive functions most sensitive to decline in old age.[65][66] Several explanations for this decline have been offered. One is the processing speed theory of cognitive aging by Tim Salthouse.[67] Drawing on the finding that cognitive processes generally slow as people grow older, Salthouse argues that slower processing leaves more time for working memory content to decay, thus reducing effective capacity. However, the decline of working memory capacity cannot be entirely attributed to slowing because capacity declines more in old age than speed.[66][68] Another proposal is the inhibition hypothesis advanced by Lynn Hasher and Rose Zacks.[69] This theory assumes a general deficit in old age in the ability to inhibit irrelevant information. Thus, working memory should tend to be cluttered with irrelevant content that reduces effective capacity for relevant content. The assumption of an inhibition deficit in old age has received much empirical support[70] but, so far, it is not clear whether the decline in inhibitory ability fully explains the decline of working memory capacity. An explanation on the neural level of the decline of working memory and other cognitive functions in old age has been proposed by West.[71] She argues that working memory depends to a large degree on the prefrontal cortex, which deteriorates more than other brain regions as we grow old. The prefrontal cortex hemodynamics also play an important role in the impairment of working memory through a prevalence of sleeping disorders that many older adults face but it is not the only region that is influenced since other brain regions have demonstrated an output of influence within Neuroimaging studies.[72][73] Within the studies of fMRI, a connection between sleep deprivation was observed through a reduction of performance on the prefrontal cortex and a overall decrease in working memory performance.[74] Age-related decline in working memory can be briefly reversed using low intensity transcranial stimulation to synchronize rhythms in prefrontal and temporal areas.[75]

Training

Some studies in the effects of training on working memory, including the first by Torkel Klingberg, suggest that working memory in those with ADHD can improve by training.[76] This study found that a period of working memory training increases a range of cognitive abilities and increases IQ test scores. Another study by the same group[77] has shown that, after training, measured brain activity related to working memory increased in the prefrontal cortex, an area that many researchers have associated with working memory functions. One study has shown that working memory training increases the density of prefrontal and parietal dopamine receptors (specifically, DRD1) in test subjects.[78] However, subsequent experiments with the same training program have shown mixed results, with some successfully replicating, and others failing to replicate the beneficial effects of training on cognitive performance.[79]

In another influential study, training with a working memory task (the dual n-back task) improved performance on a fluid intelligence test in healthy young adults.[80] The improvement of fluid intelligence by training with the n-back task was replicated in 2010,[81] but two studies published in 2012 failed to reproduce the effect.[82][83] The combined evidence from about 30 experimental studies on the effectiveness of working-memory training has been evaluated by several meta-analyses.[84] tr[85] The authors of these meta-analyses disagree in their conclusions as to whether or not working-memory training improves intelligence. Yet these meta-analyses agree that, the more distant the outcome measure, the weaker is the causal link–training working memory almost always yields increases in working memory, often in attention, and sometimes in academic performance, but it is still an outstanding question what exact circumstances differs between cases of successful and unsuccessful transfer of effects.[86][79]

In the brain

Neural mechanisms of maintaining information

The first insights into the neuronal and neurotransmitter basis of working memory came from animal research. The work of Jacobsen[87] and Fulton in the 1930s first showed that lesions to the PFC impaired spatial working memory performance in monkeys. The later work of Joaquin Fuster[88] recorded the electrical activity of neurons in the PFC of monkeys while they were doing a delayed matching task. In that task, the monkey sees how the experimenter places a bit of food under one of two identical-looking cups. A shutter is then lowered for a variable delay period, screening off the cups from the monkey's view. After the delay, the shutter opens and the monkey is allowed to retrieve the food from under the cups. Successful retrieval in the first attempt – something the animal can achieve after some training on the task – requires holding the location of the food in memory over the delay period. Fuster found neurons in the PFC that fired mostly during the delay period, suggesting that they were involved in representing the food location while it was invisible. Later research has shown similar delay-active neurons also in the posterior parietal cortex, the thalamus, the caudate, and the globus pallidus.[89] The work of Goldman-Rakic and others showed that principal sulcal, dorsolateral PFC interconnects with all of these brain regions, and that neuronal microcircuits within PFC are able to maintain information in working memory through recurrent excitatory glutamate networks of pyramidal cells that continue to fire throughout the delay period.[90] These circuits are tuned by lateral inhibition from GABAergic interneurons.[91] The neuromodulatory arousal systems markedly alter PFC working memory function; for example, either too little or too much dopamine or norepinephrine impairs PFC network firing[92] and working memory performance.[93]

The research described above on persistent firing of certain neurons in the delay period of working memory tasks shows that the brain has a mechanism of keeping representations active without external input. Keeping representations active, however, is not enough if the task demands maintaining more than one chunk of information. In addition, the components and features of each chunk must be bound together to prevent them from being mixed up. For example, if a red triangle and a green square must be remembered at the same time, one must make sure that "red" is bound to "triangle" and "green" is bound to "square". One way of establishing such bindings is by having the neurons that represent features of the same chunk fire in synchrony, and those that represent features belonging to different chunks fire out of sync.[94] In the example, neurons representing redness would fire in synchrony with neurons representing the triangular shape, but out of sync with those representing the square shape. So far, there is no direct evidence that working memory uses this binding mechanism, and other mechanisms have been proposed as well.[95] It has been speculated that synchronous firing of neurons involved in working memory oscillate with frequencies in the theta band (4 to 8 Hz). Indeed, the power of theta frequency in the EEG increases with working memory load,[96] and oscillations in the theta band measured over different parts of the skull become more coordinated when the person tries to remember the binding between two components of information.[97]

Localization in the brain

Localization of brain functions in humans has become much easier with the advent of brain imaging methods (PET and fMRI). This research has confirmed that areas in the PFC are involved in working memory functions. During the 1990s much debate has centered on the different functions of the ventrolateral (i.e., lower areas) and the dorsolateral (higher) areas of the PFC. A human lesion study provides additional evidence for the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in working memory.[98] One view was that the dorsolateral areas are responsible for spatial working memory and the ventrolateral areas for non-spatial working memory. Another view proposed a functional distinction, arguing that ventrolateral areas are mostly involved in pure maintenance of information, whereas dorsolateral areas are more involved in tasks requiring some processing of the memorized material. The debate is not entirely resolved but most of the evidence supports the functional distinction.[99]

Brain imaging has revealed that working memory functions are not limited to the PFC. A review of numerous studies[100] shows areas of activation during working memory tasks scattered over a large part of the cortex. There is a tendency for spatial tasks to recruit more right-hemisphere areas, and for verbal and object working memory to recruit more left-hemisphere areas. The activation during verbal working memory tasks can be broken down into one component reflecting maintenance, in the left posterior parietal cortex, and a component reflecting subvocal rehearsal, in the left frontal cortex (Broca's area, known to be involved in speech production).[101]

There is an emerging consensus that most working memory tasks recruit a network of PFC and parietal areas. A study has shown that during a working memory task the connectivity between these areas increases.[102] Another study has demonstrated that these areas are necessary for working memory, and not simply activated accidentally during working memory tasks, by temporarily blocking them through transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS), thereby producing an impairment in task performance.[103]

A current debate concerns the function of these brain areas. The PFC has been found to be active in a variety of tasks that require executive functions.[38] This has led some researchers to argue that the role of PFC in working memory is in controlling attention, selecting strategies, and manipulating information in working memory, but not in maintenance of information. The maintenance function is attributed to more posterior areas of the brain, including the parietal cortex.[104][105] Other authors interpret the activity in parietal cortex as reflecting executive functions, because the same area is also activated in other tasks requiring attention but not memory.[106] Evidence from decoding studying employing multi-voxel-pattern-analysis of fMRI data showed the content of visual working memory can be decoded from activity patterns in visual cortex, but not prefrontal cortex.[107] This led to the suggestion that the maintenance function of visual working memory is performed by visual cortex while the role of the prefrontal cortex is in executive control over working memory[107] though it has been pointed out that such comparisons do not take into account the base rate of decoding across different regions.[108]

A 2003 meta-analysis of 60 neuroimaging studies found left frontal cortex was involved in low-task demand verbal working memory and right frontal cortex for spatial working memory. Brodmann's areas (BAs) 6, 8, and 9, in the superior frontal cortex was involved when working memory must be continuously updated and when memory for temporal order had to be maintained. Right Brodmann 10 and 47 in the ventral frontal cortex were involved more frequently with demand for manipulation such as dual-task requirements or mental operations, and Brodmann 7 in the posterior parietal cortex was also involved in all types of executive function.[109]

Working memory has been suggested to involve two processes with different neuroanatomical locations in the frontal and parietal lobes.[110] First, a selection operation that retrieves the most relevant item, and second an updating operation that changes the focus of attention made upon it. Updating the attentional focus has been found to involve the transient activation in the caudal superior frontal sulcus and posterior parietal cortex, while increasing demands on selection selectively changes activation in the rostral superior frontal sulcus and posterior cingulate/precuneus.[110]

Articulating the differential function of brain regions involved in working memory is dependent on tasks able to distinguish these functions.[111] Most brain imaging studies of working memory have used recognition tasks such as delayed recognition of one or several stimuli, or the n-back task, in which each new stimulus in a long series must be compared to the one presented n steps back in the series. The advantage of recognition tasks is that they require minimal movement (just pressing one of two keys), making fixation of the head in the scanner easier. Experimental research and research on individual differences in working memory, however, has used largely recall tasks (e.g., the reading span task, see below). It is not clear to what degree recognition and recall tasks reflect the same processes and the same capacity limitations.

Brain imaging studies have been conducted with the reading span task or related tasks. Increased activation during these tasks was found in the PFC and, in several studies, also in the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC). People performing better on the task showed larger increase of activation in these areas, and their activation was correlated more over time, suggesting that their neural activity in these two areas was better coordinated, possibly due to stronger connectivity.[112][113]

Neural models

One approach to modeling the neurophysiology and the functioning of working memory is prefrontal cortex basal ganglia working memory (PBWM). In this model, the prefrontal cortex works hand-in-hand with the basal ganglia to accomplish the tasks of working memory. Many studies have shown this to be the case.[114] One used ablation techniques in patients who had had seizures and had damage to the prefrontal cortex and basal ganglia.[115] Researchers found that such damage resulted in decreased capacity to carry out the executive function of working memory.[115] Additional research conducted on patients with brain alterations due to methamphetamine use found that training working memory increases volume in the basal ganglia.[116]

Effects of stress on neurophysiology

Working memory is impaired by acute and chronic psychological stress. This phenomenon was first discovered in animal studies by Arnsten and colleagues,[117] who have shown that stress-induced catecholamine release in PFC rapidly decreases PFC neuronal firing and impairs working memory performance through feedforward, intracellular signaling pathways.[118] Exposure to chronic stress leads to more profound working memory deficits and additional architectural changes in PFC, including dendritic atrophy and spine loss,[119] which can be prevented by inhibition of protein kinase C signaling.[120] fMRI research has extended this research to humans, and confirms that reduced working memory caused by acute stress links to reduced activation of the PFC, and stress increased levels of catecholamines.[121] Imaging studies of medical students undergoing stressful exams have also shown weakened PFC functional connectivity, consistent with the animal studies.[122] The marked effects of stress on PFC structure and function may help to explain how stress can cause or exacerbate mental illness. The more stress in one's life, the lower the efficiency of working memory in performing simple cognitive tasks. Students who performed exercises that reduced the intrusion of negative thoughts showed an increase in their working memory capacity. Mood states (positive or negative) can have an influence on the neurotransmitter dopamine, which in turn can affect problem solving.[123]

Effects of alcohol on neurophysiology

Excessive alcohol use can result in brain damage which impairs working memory.[124] Alcohol has an effect on the blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) response. The BOLD response correlates increased blood oxygenation with brain activity, which makes this response a useful tool for measuring neuronal activity.[125] The BOLD response affects regions of the brain such as the basal ganglia and thalamus when performing a working memory task. Adolescents who start drinking at a young age show a decreased BOLD response in these brain regions.[126] Alcohol dependent young women in particular exhibit less of a BOLD response in parietal and frontal cortices when performing a spatial working memory task.[127] Binge drinking, specifically, can also affect one's performance on working memory tasks, particularly visual working memory.[128][129] Additionally, there seems to be a gender difference in regards to how alcohol affects working memory. While women perform better on verbal working memory tasks after consuming alcohol compared to men, they appear to perform worse on spatial working memory tasks as indicated by less brain activity.[130][131] Finally, age seems to be an additional factor. Older adults are more susceptible than others to the effects of alcohol on working memory.[132]

Genetics

Behavioral genetics

Individual differences in working-memory capacity are to some extent heritable; that is, about half of the variation between individuals is related to differences in their genes.[133][134][135] The genetic component of variability of working-memory capacity is largely shared with that of fluid intelligence.[134][133]

Attempts to identify individual genes

Little is known about which genes are related to the functioning of working memory. Within the theoretical framework of the multi-component model, one candidate gene has been proposed, namely ROBO1 for the hypothetical phonological loop component of working memory.[136]

More recently another gene was found regarding working memory. Looking at genetically diverse mice, GPR12 was found in promoting a protein necessary for working memory. When they took mice that were performing worse on memory tests than their control mouse counterparts and increased their GPR12 proteins, those mice improved from 50% to 80%. That brought the low performance mice up to level similar to their control counterparts.[137]

With the build up of prior work on mice such as testing the Formimidoyltransferase Cyclodeaminase (FTCD) gene in regards to the Morris water maze performance, testing out if there was a potential variation of genetic coding within the FTCD gene within humans was soon tested out. Results showed that a variation was found but varied depending on the age of the individual. In regards to the FTCD gene, it appeared that only children were affected by it. Working memory seemed to have a higher performance when the FTCD gene was present but had no similar affect to adults.[138]

Role in academic achievement

Working memory capacity is correlated with learning outcomes in literacy and numeracy. Initial evidence for this relation comes from the correlation between working-memory capacity and reading comprehension, as first observed by Daneman and Carpenter (1980)[139] and confirmed in a later meta-analytic review of several studies.[140] Subsequent work found that working memory performance in primary school children accurately predicted performance in mathematical problem solving.[141] One longitudinal study showed that a child's working memory at 5 years old is a better predictor of academic success than IQ.[142]

A randomized controlled study of 580 children in Germany indicated that working memory training at age six had a significant positive effect in spatial working memory immediately after training, and that the effect gradually transferred to other areas, with significant and meaningful increases in reading comprehension, mathematics (geometry), and IQ (measured by Raven matrices). Additionally, a marked increase in ability to inhibit impulses was detected in the follow-up after one year, measured as a higher score in the Go-No Go task. Four years after the treatment, the effects persisted and was captured as a 16 percentage point higher acceptance rate to the academic track (German Gymnasium), as compared to the control group.[86]

In a large-scale screening study, one in ten children in mainstream classrooms were identified with working memory deficits. The majority of them performed very poorly in academic achievements, independent of their IQ.[143] Similarly, working memory deficits have been identified in national curriculum low-achievers as young as seven years of age.[144] Without appropriate intervention, these children lag behind their peers. A recent study of 37 school-age children with significant learning disabilities has shown that working memory capacity at baseline measurement, but not IQ, predicts learning outcomes two years later.[145] This suggests that working memory impairments are associated with low learning outcomes and constitute a high risk factor for educational underachievement for children. In children with learning disabilities such as dyslexia, ADHD, and developmental coordination disorder, a similar pattern is evident.[146][147][148][149]

Relation to attention

There is some evidence that optimal working memory performance links to the neural ability to focus attention on task-relevant information and to ignore distractions,[150] and that practice-related improvement in working memory is due to increasing these abilities.[151] One line of research suggests a link between the working memory capacities of a person and their ability to control the orientation of attention to stimuli in the environment.[152] Such control enables people to attend to information important for their current goals, and to ignore goal-irrelevant stimuli that tend to capture their attention due to their sensory saliency (such as an ambulance siren). The direction of attention according to one's goals is assumed to rely on "top-down" signals from the pre-frontal cortex (PFC) that biases processing in posterior cortical areas.[153] Capture of attention by salient stimuli is assumed to be driven by "bottom-up" signals from subcortical structures and the primary sensory cortices.[154] The ability to override "bottom-up" capture of attention differs between individuals, and this difference has been found to correlate with their performance in a working-memory test for visual information.[152] Another study, however, found no correlation between the ability to override attentional capture and measures of more general working-memory capacity.[155]

Relationship with neural disorders

An impairment of working memory functioning is normally seen in several neural disorders:

ADHD: Several authors[156] have proposed that symptoms of ADHD arise from a primary deficit in a specific executive function (EF) domain such as working memory, response inhibition or a more general weakness in executive control.[157] A meta-analytical review cites several studies that found significant lower group results for ADHD in spatial and verbal working memory tasks, and in several other EF tasks. However, the authors concluded that EF weaknesses neither are necessary nor sufficient to cause all cases of ADHD.[157]

Several neurotransmitters, such as dopamine and glutamate may be involved in both ADHD and working memory. Both are associated with the frontal brain, self-direction and self-regulation, but cause–effect have not been confirmed, so it is unclear whether working memory dysfunction leads to ADHD, or ADHD distractibility leads to poor functionality of working memory, or if there is some other connection.[158][159][160]

Parkinson's disease: Patients with Parkinson's show signs of a reduced verbal function of working memory. They wanted to find if the reduction is due to a lack of ability to focus on relevant tasks, or a low amount of memory capacity. Twenty-one patients with Parkinson's were tested in comparison to the control group of 28 participants of the same age. The researchers found that both hypotheses were the reason working memory function is reduced which did not fully agree with their hypothesis that it is either one or the other.[161]

Alzheimer's disease: As Alzheimer's disease becomes more serious, less working memory functions. In addition to deficits in episodic memory, Alzheimer's disease is associated with impairments in visual short-term memory, assessed using delayed reproduction tasks.[162][163][164] These investigations point to a deficit in visual feature binding as an important component of the deficit in Alzheimer's disease. There is one study that focuses on the neural connections and fluidity of working memory in mice brains. Half of the mice were given an injection that mimicked the effects of Alzheimer's, and the other half were not. Then the mice were expected to go through a maze that is a task to test working memory. The study helps answer questions about how Alzheimer's can deteriorate the working memory and ultimately obliterate memory functions.[165]

Huntington's disease: A group of researchers hosted a study that researched the function and connectivity of working memory over a 30-month longitudinal experiment. It found that there were certain places in the brain where most connectivity was decreased in pre-Huntington diseased patients, in comparison to the control group that remained consistently functional.[166]

Relationship with uncertainty

A recent study by Li and colleagues showed evidence that the same brain regions responsible for working memory are also responsible for how much humans trust those memories. In the past, studies have shown that individuals can evaluate how much they trust their own memories, but how humans can do this was largely unknown. Using spatial memory tests and fMRI scans, they processed where and when the information was being stored and used this data to determine memory errors. They also asked the participants to express how uncertain they were about their memories. With both sets of information, the researchers could conclude that memory and the trust in that memory are stored within the same brain region.[167]

See also

References

  1. Models of working memory: Mechanisms of Active Maintenance and Executive Control. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 1999. ISBN 978-0-521-58325-1. http://psych.colorado.edu/~miyake/MWM%20Chapter%201.pdf. 
  2. 2.0 2.1 "Executive functions". Annual Review of Psychology 64: 135–168. 2013. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750. PMID 23020641. "WM (holding information in mind and manipulating it) is distinct from short-term memory (just holding information in mind). They cluster onto separate factors in factor analyses of children, adolescents, and adults (Alloway et al. 2004, Gathercole et al. 2004). They are linked to different neural subsystems. WM relies more on dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, whereas maintaining information in mind but not manipulating it [as long as the number of items is not huge (suprathreshold)] does not need involvement of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (D’Esposito et al. 1999, Eldreth et al. 2006, Smith & Jonides 1999). Imaging studies show frontal activation only in ventrolateral prefrontal cortex for memory maintenance that is not suprathreshold.

    WM and short-term memory also show different developmental progressions; the latter develops earlier and faster.".
     
  3. "Chapter 13: Higher Cognitive Function and Behavioral Control". Molecular Neuropharmacology: A Foundation for Clinical Neuroscience (2nd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill Medical. 2009. pp. 313–321. ISBN 978-0-07-148127-4. " • Executive function, the cognitive control of behavior, depends on the prefrontal cortex, which is highly developed in higher primates and especially humans.
     • Working memory is a short-term, capacity-limited cognitive buffer that stores information and permits its manipulation to guide decision-making and behavior. ...
    working memory may be impaired in ADHD, the most common childhood psychiatric disorder seen in clinical settings ... ADHD can be conceptualized as a disorder of executive function; specifically, ADHD is characterized by reduced ability to exert and maintain cognitive control of behavior. Compared with healthy individuals, those with ADHD have diminished ability to suppress inappropriate prepotent responses to stimuli (impaired response inhibition) and diminished ability to inhibit responses to irrelevant stimuli (impaired interference suppression). ... Early results with structural MRI show thinning of the cerebral cortex in ADHD subjects compared with age-matched controls in prefrontal cortex and posterior parietal cortex, areas involved in working memory and attention."
     
  4. "Chapter 20 What are the differences between long-term, short-term, and working memory?". Essence of Memory. Progress in Brain Research. 169. 2008. pp. 323–338. doi:10.1016/S0079-6123(07)00020-9. ISBN 978-0-444-53164-3. 
  5. Plans and the structure of behavior. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. 1960. pp. 65. ISBN 978-0-03-010075-8. OCLC 190675. https://archive.org/details/plansstructureo00mill/page/65. 
  6. "Working memory: looking back and looking forward". Nature Reviews. Neuroscience 4 (10): 829–839. October 2003. doi:10.1038/nrn1201. PMID 14523382. 
  7. Human Memory: A Proposed System and its Control Processes. Psychology of Learning and Motivation. 8. Academic Press. 1968. pp. 89–195. doi:10.1016/S0079-7421(08)60422-3. ISBN 978-0-12-543302-0. OCLC 185468704. 
  8. The prefrontal cortex: anatomy, physiology, and neuropsychology of the frontal lobe. Philadelphia: Lippincott-Raven. 1997. ISBN 978-0-397-51849-4. OCLC 807338522. [page needed]
  9. 9.0 9.1 The prefrontal cortex (4th ed.). Oxford, UK: Elsevier. 2008. p. 126. ISBN 978-0-12-373644-4. https://books.google.com/books?id=zuZlvNICdhUC&pg=PT140. 
  10. "The prefrontal region:Its early history". Frontal lobe function and dysfunction. New York: Oxford University Press. 1991. p. 19. ISBN 978-0-19-506284-7. https://books.google.com/books?id=9b1htO0V0rwC&q=Jacobsen%20%20prefrontal%20ablation&pg=PA19. 
  11. Working Memory. Psychology of Learning and Motivation. 2. Academic Press. 1974. pp. 47–89. doi:10.1016/S0079-7421(08)60452-1. ISBN 978-0-12-543308-2. OCLC 777285348. 
  12. Working Memory : Capacity, Developments and Improvement Techniques. New York: Nova Science Publishers, Inc.. 2011. 
  13. Variations in psychology (9th ed.). New York: Wadsworth. 2013. pp. 281–282. 
  14. Variations in psychology (9th ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth. 2013. pp. 281–282. 
  15. "The episodic buffer: a new component of working memory?". Trends in Cognitive Sciences 4 (11): 417–423. November 2000. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01538-2. PMID 11058819. 
  16. "Long-term working memory". Psychological Review 102 (2): 211–245. April 1995. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.102.2.211. PMID 7740089. 
  17. Attention and memory: an integrated framework. Oxford [Oxfordshire]: Oxford University Press. 1995. ISBN 978-0-19-506760-6. OCLC 30475237. 
  18. "Attention, working memory, and long-term memory in multimedia learning: A integrated perspective based on process models of working memory". Educational Psychology Review 26 (2): 289. 2014. doi:10.1007/s10648-013-9242-2. 
  19. "Access to information in working memory: exploring the focus of attention". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 28 (3): 411–421. May 2002. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.28.3.411. PMID 12018494. 
  20. "The magical number seven plus or minus two: some limits on our capacity for processing information". Psychological Review 63 (2): 81–97. March 1956. doi:10.1037/h0043158. PMID 13310704.  Republished: "The magical number seven, plus or minus two: some limits on our capacity for processing information. 1956". Psychological Review 101 (2): 343–352. April 1994. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.101.2.343. PMID 8022966. 
  21. "The Effect of Word Length on Immediate Serial Recall Depends on Phonological Complexity, Not Articulatory Duration". The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A 51 (2): 283–304. 1998-05-01. doi:10.1080/713755759. ISSN 0272-4987. 
  22. "The role of long-term memory mechanisms in memory span". British Journal of Psychology 86 (4): 527–36. November 1995. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1995.tb02570.x. 
  23. "The magical number 4 in short-term memory: a reconsideration of mental storage capacity". The Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24 (1): 87–185. February 2001. doi:10.1017/S0140525X01003922. PMID 11515286. 
  24. 24.0 24.1 24.2 "Changing concepts of working memory". Nature Neuroscience 17 (3): 347–356. March 2014. doi:10.1038/nn.3655. PMID 24569831. 
  25. 25.0 25.1 "The precision of visual working memory is set by allocation of a shared resource". Journal of Vision 9 (10): 7.1–711. September 2009. doi:10.1167/9.10.7. PMID 19810788. 
  26. 26.0 26.1 "Temporal dynamics of encoding, storage, and reallocation of visual working memory". Journal of Vision 11 (10): 6. September 2011. doi:10.1167/11.10.6. PMID 21911739. 
  27. 27.0 27.1 "A review of visual memory capacity: Beyond individual items and toward structured representations". Journal of Vision 11 (5): 4. May 2011. doi:10.1167/11.5.4. PMID 21617025. 
  28. "Some shortcomings of long-term working memory". British Journal of Psychology 91 (Pt 4): 551–570. November 2000. doi:10.1348/000712600161989. PMID 11104178. http://bura.brunel.ac.uk/handle/2438/807. 
  29. "Individual differences in working memory and reading". Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior 19 (4): 450–66. August 1980. doi:10.1016/S0022-5371(80)90312-6. 
  30. "Working memory capacity—facets of a cognitive ability construct". Personality and Individual Differences 29 (6): 1017–45. December 2000. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(99)00251-2. 
  31. "On the division of short-term and working memory: an examination of simple and complex span and their relation to higher order abilities". Psychological Bulletin 133 (6): 1038–1066. November 2007. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.133.6.1038. PMID 17967093. 
  32. "Working memory and intelligence are highly related constructs, but why?". Intelligence 36 (6): 584–606. 2008. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2008.01.002. 
  33. "The multiple faces of working memory – storage, processing, supervision, and coordination". Intelligence 31 (2): 167–193. 2003. doi:10.1016/s0160-2896(02)00115-0. https://www.zora.uzh.ch/id/eprint/97155/1/intelligence.pdf. 
  34. "The relational integration task explains fluid reasoning above and beyond other working memory tasks". Memory & Cognition 42 (3): 448–463. April 2014. doi:10.3758/s13421-013-0366-x. PMID 24222318. 
  35. The Interplay of Long-Term Memory and Working Memory: When Does Object-Color Prior Knowledge Affect Color Visual Working Memory?. doi:10.31234/osf.io/8a2jw. https://psyarxiv.com/8a2jw/. Retrieved 2023-03-22. 
  36. "Working memory capacity and its relation to general intelligence". Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7 (12): 547–552. December 2003. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2003.10.005. PMID 14643371. 
  37. "Working memory, short-term memory, and general fluid intelligence: a latent-variable approach". Journal of Experimental Psychology. General 128 (3): 309–331. September 1999. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.128.3.309. PMID 10513398. 
  38. 38.0 38.1 "The role of prefrontal cortex in working-memory capacity, executive attention, and general fluid intelligence: an individual-differences perspective". Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 9 (4): 637–671. December 2002. doi:10.3758/BF03196323. PMID 12613671. 
  39. "How many variables can humans process?". Psychological Science 16 (1): 70–76. January 2005. doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.00782.x. PMID 15660854. 
  40. 40.0 40.1 "A capacity theory of comprehension: individual differences in working memory". Psychological Review 99 (1): 122–149. January 1992. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.99.1.122. PMID 1546114. 
  41. "On the interpretation of working memory span in adults". Memory & Cognition 28 (3): 341–348. April 2000. doi:10.3758/BF03198549. PMID 10881551. 
  42. "PRIMARY MEMORY". Psychological Review 72 (2): 89–104. March 1965. doi:10.1037/h0021797. PMID 14282677. 
  43. "Some tests of the decay theory of immediate memory". Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 10: 12–21. 1958. doi:10.1080/17470215808416249. 
  44. "Short-term retention of individual verbal items". Journal of Experimental Psychology 58 (3): 193–198. September 1959. doi:10.1037/h0049234. PMID 14432252. 
  45. Working memory. 11. Oxford: Clarendon. 1986. ISBN 978-0-19-852116-7. 
  46. "Time constraints and resource sharing in adults' working memory spans". Journal of Experimental Psychology. General 133 (1): 83–100. March 2004. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.133.1.83. PMID 14979753. 
  47. "Time and cognitive load in working memory", J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 33 (3): 570–585, May 2007, doi:10.1037/0278-7393.33.3.570, PMID 17470006, https://archive-ouverte.unige.ch/unige:88299 
  48. "Factorial comparison of working memory models". Psychological Review 121 (1): 124–149. January 2014. doi:10.1037/a0035234. PMID 24490791. 
  49. "Modeling working memory: an interference model of complex span". Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 19 (5): 779–819. October 2012. doi:10.3758/s13423-012-0272-4. PMID 22715024. http://doc.rero.ch/record/320568/files/13423_2012_Article_272.pdf. 
  50. "A formal model of capacity limits in working memory". Journal of Memory and Language 55 (4): 601–26. November 2006. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2006.08.009. 
  51. "Distractor frequency influences performance in vibrotactile working memory". Experimental Brain Research 208 (4): 529–532. February 2011. doi:10.1007/s00221-010-2501-2. PMID 21132280. 
  52. "The relationship between processing and storage in working memory span: Not two sides of the same coin". Journal of Memory and Language 56 (2): 212–228. February 2007. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2006.07.009. 
  53. "Selection from Working Memory: on the Relationship between Processing and Storage Components". Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition 6 (2): 99–116. June 1999. doi:10.1076/anec.6.2.99.784. 
  54. "Time does not cause forgetting in short-term serial recall". Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 11 (5): 771–790. October 2004. doi:10.3758/BF03196705. PMID 15732687. [yes|permanent dead link|dead link}}]
  55. "Forgetting in immediate serial recall: decay, temporal distinctiveness, or interference?". Psychological Review 115 (3): 544–576. July 2008. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.115.3.544. PMID 18729591. https://research-repository.uwa.edu.au/en/publications/82b67264-e611-4786-8e39-9fa8659f98fb. 
  56. 56.0 56.1 "The structure of working memory from 4 to 15 years of age". Developmental Psychology 40 (2): 177–190. March 2004. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.40.2.177. PMID 14979759. 
  57. "The aging of working memory". Neuropsychology 8 (4): 535–543. 1994. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.8.4.535. 
  58. "A mathematical model for the transition rule in Piaget's developmental stages". Acta Psychologica 32: 301–345. 1970. doi:10.1016/0001-6918(70)90108-3. 
  59. Intellectual development. Birth to adulthood.. New York: Academic Press. 1985. 
  60. "Variation in working memory due to typical and atypical development.". Variation in working memory. New York: Oxford University Press. 2007. pp. 137–161. ISBN 978-0-19-516864-8. OCLC 1222332615. 
  61. "Longitudinal evidence that increases in processing speed and working memory enhance children's reasoning". Psychological Science 18 (4): 312–313. April 2007. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01895.x. PMID 17470254. 
  62. "A cognitive complexity metric applied to cognitive development". Cognitive Psychology 45 (2): 153–219. September 2002. doi:10.1016/S0010-0285(02)00002-6. PMID 12528901. 
  63. "Theories of Working Memory: Differences in Definition, Degree of Modularity, Role of Attention, and Purpose". Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools 49 (3): 340–355. July 2018. doi:10.1044/2018_LSHSS-17-0114. PMID 29978205. 
  64. "N-back Working Memory Task: Meta-analysis of Normative fMRI Studies With Children". Child Development 89 (6): 2010–2022. November 2018. doi:10.1111/cdev.13080. PMID 29732553. 
  65. "Latent change models of adult cognition: are changes in processing speed and working memory associated with changes in episodic memory?". Psychology and Aging 18 (4): 755–769. December 2003. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.18.4.755. PMID 14692862. 
  66. 66.0 66.1 "Models of visuospatial and verbal memory across the adult life span". Psychology and Aging 17 (2): 299–320. June 2002. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.17.2.299. PMID 12061414. 
  67. "The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition". Psychological Review 103 (3): 403–428. July 1996. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.103.3.403. PMID 8759042. 
  68. "Sequential and coordinative processing dynamics in figural transformations across the life span". Cognition 59 (1): 61–90. April 1996. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(95)00689-3. PMID 8857471. 
  69. "Working memory, comprehension, and aging: A review and new view.". The psychology of learning and motivation. 22. New York: Academic Press. 1988. pp. 193–225. ISBN 978-0-08-086373-3. OCLC 476167241. 
  70. "Inhibitory control, circadian arousal, and age.". Attention and Performance. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. 1999. pp. 653–675. ISBN 978-0-262-31576-0. OCLC 1120891520. 
  71. "An application of prefrontal cortex function theory to cognitive aging". Psychological Bulletin 120 (2): 272–292. September 1996. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.120.2.272. PMID 8831298. 
  72. "Prefrontal Cortex Hemodynamics and Functional Connectivity Changes during Performance Working Memory Tasks in Older Adults with Sleep Disorders". Brain Sciences 13 (3): 497. March 2023. doi:10.3390/brainsci13030497. PMID 36979307. 
  73. "Effective connectivity of the posterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortices relates to working memory impairment in schizophrenic and bipolar patients". Schizophrenia Research 158 (1–3): 85–90. September 2014. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2014.06.033. PMID 25043264. 
  74. "High-Frequency Repetitive Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation Could Improve Impaired Working Memory Induced by Sleep Deprivation". Neural Plasticity 2019: 7030286. 2019-12-12. doi:10.1155/2019/7030286. PMID 31915432. 
  75. "Scientists reverse memory decline using electrical pulses" (in en-GB). The Guardian. 2019-04-08. ISSN 0261-3077. https://www.theguardian.com/science/2019/apr/08/scientists-use-electrical-pulses-reverse-memory-decline-ageing. 
  76. "Training of working memory in children with ADHD". Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 24 (6): 781–791. September 2002. doi:10.1076/jcen.24.6.781.8395. PMID 12424652. 
  77. "Increased prefrontal and parietal activity after training of working memory". Nature Neuroscience 7 (1): 75–79. January 2004. doi:10.1038/nn1165. PMID 14699419. 
  78. "Changes in cortical dopamine D1 receptor binding associated with cognitive training". Science 323 (5915): 800–802. February 2009. doi:10.1126/science.1166102. PMID 19197069. Bibcode2009Sci...323..800M. 
  79. 79.0 79.1 "How to play 20 questions with nature and lose: Reflections on 100 years of brain-training research". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 115 (40): 9897–9904. October 2018. doi:10.1073/pnas.1617102114. PMID 30275315. Bibcode2018PNAS..115.9897K. 
  80. "Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 105 (19): 6829–6833. May 2008. doi:10.1073/pnas.0801268105. PMID 18443283. Bibcode2008PNAS..105.6829J. 
  81. "The relationship between n-back performance and matrix reasoning – implications for training and transfer". Intelligence 38 (6): 625–635. 2010. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2010.09.001. ISSN 0160-2896. 
  82. "No evidence of intelligence improvement after working memory training: a randomized, placebo-controlled study". Journal of Experimental Psychology. General 142 (2): 359–379. May 2013. doi:10.1037/a0029082. PMID 22708717. 
  83. "Working memory training does not improve intelligence in healthy young adults". Intelligence 40 (6): 531–542. 2012. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2012.07.004. ISSN 0160-2896. 
  84. "Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory: a meta-analysis". Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 22 (2): 366–377. April 2015. doi:10.3758/s13423-014-0699-x. PMID 25102926. https://escholarship.org/uc/item/1mj701dj. 
  85. "Working Memory Training Does Not Improve Performance on Measures of Intelligence or Other Measures of "Far Transfer": Evidence From a Meta-Analytic Review". Perspectives on Psychological Science 11 (4): 512–534. July 2016. doi:10.1177/1745691616635612. PMID 27474138. 
  86. 86.0 86.1 The Impact of Working Memory Training on Children's Cognitive and Noncognitive Skills (Report). 2020. doi:10.2139/ssrn.3622985. 
  87. "Studies of cerebral function in primates". Comparative Psychology Monographs 13 (3): 1–68. 1938. OCLC 250695441. 
  88. "Unit activity in prefrontal cortex during delayed-response performance: neuronal correlates of transient memory". Journal of Neurophysiology 36 (1): 61–78. January 1973. doi:10.1152/jn.1973.36.1.61. PMID 4196203. 
  89. "FROST: a distributed neurocomputational model of working memory maintenance". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 17 (11): 1728–1743. November 2005. doi:10.1162/089892905774589271. PMID 16269109. 
  90. "Cellular basis of working memory". Neuron 14 (3): 477–485. March 1995. doi:10.1016/0896-6273(95)90304-6. PMID 7695894. 
  91. "Destruction and creation of spatial tuning by disinhibition: GABA(A) blockade of prefrontal cortical neurons engaged by working memory". The Journal of Neuroscience 20 (1): 485–494. January 2000. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-01-00485.2000. PMID 10627624. 
  92. "Dynamic Network Connectivity: A new form of neuroplasticity". Trends in Cognitive Sciences 14 (8): 365–375. August 2010. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2010.05.003. PMID 20554470. 
  93. "The neuropsychopharmacology of fronto-executive function: monoaminergic modulation". Annual Review of Neuroscience 32: 267–287. 2009. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.051508.135535. PMID 19555290. 
  94. "A cortical mechanism for binding in visual working memory". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 13 (6): 766–785. August 2001. doi:10.1162/08989290152541430. PMID 11564321. 
  95. "Three forms of binding and their neural substrates: Alternatives to temporal synchrony". The unity of consciousness: Binding, integration, and dissociation. Oxford: Oxford University Press. 2003. pp. 168–90. ISBN 978-0-19-850857-1. OCLC 50747505. http://psycnet.apa.org/psycinfo/2003-88180-008. 
  96. "Binding principles in the theta frequency range". Handbook of binding and memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press. 2006. pp. 115–144. 
  97. "Binding of verbal and spatial information in human working memory involves large-scale neural synchronization at theta frequency". NeuroImage 35 (4): 1654–1662. May 2007. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.02.011. PMID 17379539. 
  98. "Dorsolateral prefrontal contributions to human working memory". Cortex; A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior 49 (5): 1195–1205. May 2013. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2012.05.022. PMID 22789779. 
  99. "The functional organization of working memory processes within human lateral frontal cortex: the contribution of functional neuroimaging". The European Journal of Neuroscience 9 (7): 1329–1339. July 1997. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.1997.tb01487.x. PMID 9240390. 
  100. "Storage and executive processes in the frontal lobes". Science 283 (5408): 1657–1661. March 1999. doi:10.1126/science.283.5408.1657. PMID 10073923. Bibcode1999Sci...283.1657.. 
  101. "Components of verbal working memory: evidence from neuroimaging". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 95 (3): 876–882. February 1998. doi:10.1073/pnas.95.3.876. PMID 9448254. Bibcode1998PNAS...95..876S. 
  102. "Effects of verbal working memory load on corticocortical connectivity modeled by path analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging data". NeuroImage 17 (2): 573–582. October 2002. doi:10.1016/S1053-8119(02)91193-6. PMID 12377135. 
  103. "Interfering with working memory in humans". Neuroscience 139 (1): 85–90. April 2006. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.05.037. PMID 16337091. 
  104. "Persistent activity in the prefrontal cortex during working memory". Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7 (9): 415–423. September 2003. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00197-9. PMID 12963473. 
  105. "Working memory as an emergent property of the mind and brain". Neuroscience 139 (1): 23–38. April 2006. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.06.005. PMID 16324795. 
  106. "Exploration of the neural substrates of executive functioning by functional neuroimaging". Neuroscience 139 (1): 209–221. April 2006. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.05.035. PMID 16324796. 
  107. 107.0 107.1 "Revisiting the role of persistent neural activity during working memory". Trends in Cognitive Sciences 18 (2): 82–89. February 2014. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2013.12.001. PMID 24439529. 
  108. "Just above Chance: Is It Harder to Decode Information from Prefrontal Cortex Hemodynamic Activity Patterns?". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 30 (10): 1473–1498. October 2018. doi:10.1162/jocn_a_01291. PMID 29877764. 
  109. "Neuroimaging studies of working memory: a meta-analysis". Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience 3 (4): 255–274. December 2003. doi:10.3758/cabn.3.4.255. PMID 15040547. 
  110. 110.0 110.1 "What "works" in working memory? Separate systems for selection and updating of critical information". The Journal of Neuroscience 29 (43): 13735–13741. October 2009. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2547-09.2009. PMID 19864586. 
  111. "What has functional neuroimaging told us about the mind (so far)?". Cortex; A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior 42 (3): 323–331. April 2006. doi:10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70358-7. PMID 16771037. 
  112. "Cooperation of the anterior cingulate cortex and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex for attention shifting". NeuroImage 23 (2): 670–679. October 2004. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.06.014. PMID 15488417. 
  113. "The neural basis of executive function in working memory: an fMRI study based on individual differences". NeuroImage 21 (2): 623–631. February 2004. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.09.069. PMID 14980565. 
  114. "Keeping memory clear and stable—the contribution of human basal ganglia and prefrontal cortex to working memory". The Journal of Neuroscience 30 (29): 9788–9792. July 2010. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.1513-10.2010. PMID 20660261. 
  115. 115.0 115.1 "Prefrontal cortex and basal ganglia contributions to visual working memory". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 107 (42): 18167–18172. October 2010. doi:10.1073/pnas.1007277107. PMID 20921401. Bibcode2010PNAS..10718167V. 
  116. "Psychological intervention with working memory training increases basal ganglia volume: A VBM study of inpatient treatment for methamphetamine use". NeuroImage. Clinical 12: 478–491. 2016-02-01. doi:10.1016/j.nicl.2016.08.019. PMID 27625988. 
  117. "The biology of being frazzled". Science 280 (5370): 1711–1712. June 1998. doi:10.1126/science.280.5370.1711. PMID 9660710. 
  118. "Stress signalling pathways that impair prefrontal cortex structure and function". Nature Reviews. Neuroscience 10 (6): 410–422. June 2009. doi:10.1038/nrn2648. PMID 19455173. 
  119. "Repeated stress induces dendritic spine loss in the rat medial prefrontal cortex". Cerebral Cortex 16 (3): 313–320. March 2006. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhi104. PMID 15901656. 
  120. "Inhibition of protein kinase C signaling protects prefrontal cortex dendritic spines and cognition from the effects of chronic stress". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 106 (42): 17957–17962. October 2009. doi:10.1073/pnas.0908563106. PMID 19805148. Bibcode2009PNAS..10617957H. 
  121. "Acute psychological stress reduces working memory-related activity in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex". Biological Psychiatry 66 (1): 25–32. July 2009. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.03.006. PMID 19403118. 
  122. "Psychosocial stress reversibly disrupts prefrontal processing and attentional control". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 106 (3): 912–917. January 2009. doi:10.1073/pnas.0807041106. PMID 19139412. Bibcode2009PNAS..106..912L. 
  123. Human Cognition : Theory and Practice. (International ed.). New York, NY: Worth Pub. 2007. p. 147. ISBN 978-0-7167-5667-5. 
  124. "Drug-related decrease in neuropsychological functions of abstinent drug users". Current Drug Abuse Reviews 4 (1): 42–56. March 2011. doi:10.2174/1874473711104010042. PMID 21466500. 
  125. "Neurotoxic effects of alcohol in adolescence". Annual Review of Clinical Psychology 9 (1): 703–721. 2013. doi:10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050212-185610. PMID 23245341. 
  126. "Resiliency in adolescents at high risk for substance abuse: flexible adaptation via subthalamic nucleus and linkage to drinking and drug use in early adulthood". Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 36 (8): 1355–1364. August 2012. doi:10.1111/j.1530-0277.2012.01741.x. PMID 22587751. 
  127. "fMRI measurement of brain dysfunction in alcohol-dependent young women". Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 25 (2): 236–245. February 2001. doi:10.1111/j.1530-0277.2001.tb02204.x. PMID 11236838. 
  128. "Neuropsychological performance of South African treatment-naïve adolescents with alcohol dependence". Drug and Alcohol Dependence 110 (1–2): 8–14. July 2010. doi:10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2010.01.019. PMID 20227839. 
  129. "Binge drinking affects attentional and visual working memory processing in young university students". Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 33 (11): 1870–1879. November 2009. doi:10.1111/j.1530-0277.2009.01025.x. PMID 19673739. 
  130. "The separate and combined effects of nicotine and alcohol on working memory capacity in nonabstinent smokers". Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology 18 (2): 120–128. April 2010. doi:10.1037/a0018782. PMID 20384423. 
  131. "Adolescent binge drinking linked to abnormal spatial working memory brain activation: differential gender effects". Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 35 (10): 1831–1841. October 2011. doi:10.1111/j.1530-0277.2011.01527.x. PMID 21762178. 
  132. "Acute effects of moderate alcohol on psychomotor, set shifting, and working memory function in older and younger social drinkers". Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs 75 (5): 870–879. September 2014. doi:10.15288/jsad.2014.75.870. PMID 25208205. 
  133. 133.0 133.1 "Strong genetic overlap between executive functions and intelligence". Journal of Experimental Psychology. General 145 (9): 1141–1159. September 2016. doi:10.1037/xge0000195. PMID 27359131. 
  134. 134.0 134.1 "Genetic structure of spatial and verbal working memory". Behavior Genetics 31 (6): 615–624. November 2001. doi:10.1023/A:1013353613591. PMID 11838538. 
  135. "Heritability of working memory brain activation". The Journal of Neuroscience 31 (30): 10882–10890. July 2011. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.5334-10.2011. PMID 21795540. 
  136. "Genetic variance in a component of the language acquisition device: ROBO1 polymorphisms associated with phonological buffer deficits". Behavior Genetics 41 (1): 50–57. January 2011. doi:10.1007/s10519-010-9402-9. PMID 20949370. 
  137. "Gene links short-term memory to unexpected brain area" (in en). 29 September 2020. https://news.cornell.edu/stories/2020/09/gene-links-short-term-memory-unexpected-brain-area. 
  138. "A functional promoter variant of the human formimidoyltransferase cyclodeaminase (FTCD) gene is associated with working memory performance in young but not older adults". Neuropsychology 32 (8): 973–984. November 2018. doi:10.1037/neu0000470. PMID 29927301. 
  139. "Individual differences in working memory and reading". Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 19 (4): 450–466. 1980-08-01. doi:10.1016/S0022-5371(80)90312-6. 
  140. "Working memory and language comprehension: A meta-analysis". Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 3 (4): 422–433. December 1996. doi:10.3758/BF03214546. PMID 24213976. 
  141. "The Relationship Between Working Memory and Mathematical Problem Solving in Children at Risk and Not at Risk for Serious Math Difficulties". Journal of Educational Psychology 96 (3): 471–491. 2004. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.96.3.471. 
  142. "Investigating the predictive roles of working memory and IQ in academic attainment". Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 106 (1): 20–29. May 2010. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2009.11.003. PMID 20018296. https://www.research.ed.ac.uk/en/publications/8a871fe8-5117-4a4b-8d6c-74277e9a79e1. 
  143. "The cognitive and behavioral characteristics of children with low working memory". Child Development 80 (2): 606–621. 2009. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01282.x. PMID 19467014. 
  144. "Working memory deficits in children with low achievements in the national curriculum at 7 years of age". The British Journal of Educational Psychology 70 (2): 177–194. June 2000. doi:10.1348/000709900158047. PMID 10900777. 
  145. "Working Memory, but Not IQ, Predicts Subsequent Learning in Children with Learning Difficulties". European Journal of Psychological Assessment 25 (2): 92–8. 2009. doi:10.1027/1015-5759.25.2.92. 
  146. Working memory in dyslexia. New York, NY: Psychology Press. 2006. ISBN 978-1-84169-560-0. OCLC 63692704. 
  147. Short-term memory deficits in developmental dyslexia. New York, NY: Psychology Press. 2006. ISBN 978-1-84169-560-0. OCLC 63692704. 
  148. Working memory function in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. New York, NY: Psychology Press. 2006. ISBN 978-1-84169-560-0. OCLC 63692704. 
  149. Working memory skills in children with developmental coordination disorder. New York, NY: Psychology Press. 2006. ISBN 978-1-84169-560-0. OCLC 63692704. 
  150. "Neural suppression of irrelevant information underlies optimal working memory performance". The Journal of Neuroscience 29 (10): 3059–3066. March 2009. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4621-08.2009. PMID 19279242. 
  151. "Practice-related improvement in working memory is modulated by changes in processing external interference". Journal of Neurophysiology 102 (3): 1779–1789. September 2009. doi:10.1152/jn.00179.2009. PMID 19587320. 
  152. 152.0 152.1 "Human variation in overriding attentional capture". The Journal of Neuroscience 29 (27): 8726–8733. July 2009. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2145-09.2009. PMID 19587279. 
  153. "Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention". Annual Review of Neuroscience 18: 193–222. 1995. doi:10.1146/annurev.ne.18.030195.001205. PMID 7605061. 
  154. "Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: voluntary versus automatic allocation". Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance 16 (1): 121–134. February 1990. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.16.1.121. PMID 2137514. 
  155. "Visual selective attention is equally functional for individuals with low and high working memory capacity: evidence from accuracy and eye movements". Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 76 (7): 1998–2014. October 2014. doi:10.3758/s13414-013-0610-2. PMID 24402698. https://orca.cardiff.ac.uk/id/eprint/105362/1/Morey.%20Visual%20selective.pdf. 
  156. Barkley; Castellanos and Tannock; Pennington and Ozonoff; Schachar (according to the source)
  157. 157.0 157.1 "Validity of the executive function theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analytic review". Biological Psychiatry 57 (11): 1336–1346. June 2005. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.02.006. PMID 15950006. 
  158. "Working Memory as a Core Deficit in ADHD: Preliminary Findings and Implications.". The ADHD Report 16 (6): 8–14. December 2008. doi:10.1521/adhd.2008.16.6.8. http://guilfordjournals.com/doi/abs/10.1521/adhd.2008.16.6.8. 
  159. "Association between response inhibition and working memory in adult ADHD: a link to right frontal cortex pathology?". Biological Psychiatry 61 (12): 1395–1401. June 2007. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.07.020. PMID 17046725. 
  160. "Working memory function in attention deficit hyperactivity disordered and reading disabled children". British Journal of Developmental Psychology 19 (3): 325–337. 2001. doi:10.1348/026151001166128. ISSN 0261-510X. 
  161. "Visual working memory deficits in patients with Parkinson's disease are due to both reduced storage capacity and impaired ability to filter out irrelevant information". Brain 133 (9): 2677–2689. September 2010. doi:10.1093/brain/awq197. PMID 20688815. 
  162. "Different patterns of short-term memory deficit in Alzheimer's disease, Parkinson's disease and subjective cognitive impairment". Cortex; A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior 132: 41–50. November 2020. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2020.06.016. PMID 32919108. 
  163. "Visual short-term memory binding deficit in familial Alzheimer's disease". Cortex; A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior 78: 150–164. May 2016. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2016.01.015. PMID 27085491. 
  164. "Working Memory in Alzheimer's Disease and Parkinson's Disease". Current Topics in Behavioral Neurosciences 41: 325–344. 2019. doi:10.1007/7854_2019_103. ISBN 978-3-030-31025-7. PMID 31347008. 
  165. "Functional connectivity in a rat model of Alzheimer's disease during a working memory task". Current Alzheimer Research 11 (10): 981–991. December 2014. doi:10.2174/1567205011666141107125912. PMID 25387338. 
  166. "Functional changes during working memory in Huntington's disease: 30-month longitudinal data from the IMAGE-HD study". Brain Structure & Function 220 (1): 501–512. January 2015. doi:10.1007/s00429-013-0670-z. PMID 24240602. 
  167. "Scientists Pinpoint the Uncertainty of Our Working Memory". https://www.nyu.edu/about/news-publications/news/2021/september/scientists-pinpoint-the-uncertainty-of-our-working-memory-.html.