Philosophy:Eye contact effect

From HandWiki
Short description: Psychological phenomenon

The eye-contact effect is a psychological phenomenon in human selective attention and cognition. It is the effect that the perception of eye contact with another human face has on certain mechanisms in the brain.[1] This contact has been shown to increase activation in certain areas of what has been termed the ‘social brain’.[2] This social brain network processes social information as the face,[3] theory of mind,[4] empathy,[5] and goal-directedness.[6]

Activation in the brain

When gaze is direct, the eye-contact effect produces activation in the social brain.[2] These six regions demonstrate that perceived eye contact increases activation of elements within this network, with the area of activation depending on task demands and the social context.

Fusiform gyrus

Increase in regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) shows larger activation for direct than averted gaze in this area.[7] It has been suggested that this increased activation is related to initial increased face encoding. However, these effects are absent when one has already been presented with a face and its gaze shifts towards the participant. This indicates that when attending to face identity, face encoding effects can be masked.[1]

Anterior, right side of superior temporal sulcus

When directing gaze specifically towards the eye area, the anterior, right side of the superior temporal sulcus is activated, indicating facilitation of gaze direction encoding in this region when eye contact is present.[8] Like the fusiform gyrus, this effect can also be masked in this area.[7]

Posterior, right side of superior temporal sulcus

The activation in this region of the brain during the eye contact effect is not always consistent. Although it has been demonstrated in several studies when dynamic stimuli are used,[9] activation is not demonstrated across all literatures.[10] The studies that demonstrate activation provided social or communicative context in their experiments, suggesting that the eye-contact effect only activates the posterior right side of superior temporal sulcus in these instances.

Medial prefrontal cortex and orbitofrontal cortex

These two areas activate when dynamic facial expressions are presented as well as in a communication context when participants are required to decode the intention of the presented face. Like the posterior right side of superior temporal sulcus, this suggests that context could be a factor in this activation. However, some studies have shown higher activation in the medial prefrontal cortex when averted gaze is perceived than direct gaze. This activation was in a slightly posterior position compared to the areas which had higher activation for direct gaze.

Amygdala

The activation in the amygdala, like the posterior right side of superior temporal sulcus, is not hugely consistent either. While three studies have found activation for direct gaze in this area, several studies have found no effect. It is speculated that due to the small size of the amygdala, neural imaging methods are not sensitive enough to correctly detect activation.

Underlying mechanisms

Three models have been developed to explain the mechanisms underlying the eye-contact effect. These models show the areas of the brain that are activated by direct eye contact and where they overlap in activation with areas in the brain related to the social brain network.

The affective arousal model

This model proposes that eye contact directly activates brain arousal systems and emotional responses which influences perceptual and cognitive processing. Because reciprocated eye contact elevates emotional arousal,[11] activation becomes widespread across cortical structures as emotional arousal is often associated with the amygdala. As gaze is directed towards the perceiver, activation increases in a region in the right amygdala.[12] However, this model fails to account for the selective nature of activation effects. Activation must be more widespread across the network if general arousal is the main effect at work.

The communicative intention detector model

Eye contact signals intent of communication and the social significance of eye gaze engages theory of mind computations.[13] Because there is an overlap of activation in structures involved in theory of mind computation with regions associated with eye contact detection, this model proposes that this is the mechanism that causes the eye contact effect. However, only parts of the theory of mind network (medial prefrontal cortex, posterior right side of superior temporal sulcus, and sometimes precuneus and amygdala) are activated depending on task demands and context.

The first-track modulator model

Proposed by Senju and Johnson, this model argues that the eye contact effect is facilitated by the subcortical face detection pathway. This pathway involves the superior colliculus, pulvinar, and amygdala. This route is fast and operates on low spatial frequency and modulates cortical face processing.[1][14]

Development

Sensitivity to eye contact is present in newborns. From as early as four months old cortical activation as a result of eye contact has suggested that infants are able to detect and orient towards faces that make eye contact with them.[15] This sensitivity to eye contact remains as the presence of eye contact has an effect on the processing of social stimuli in slightly older infants. For example, a 9-month-old infant will shift its gaze towards an object in response to another face shifting its gaze towards the same object.[16]

As humans get older, the eye contact effect develops as well. Accurate face recognition facilitated by direct gaze improves over the period of development from 6 to 11 years of age.[17]

Atypical development

Autism spectrum disorders

Autism spectrum disorders (ASDs) which include autism and Asperger syndrome are characterized by social interaction and communication difficulties. Atypical responses to direct gaze, a characteristic of ASD, have been demonstrated to manifest in infancy,[18] suggesting that these responses are present from early in development.

Due to these difficulties, the development of the eye contact effect may be obstructed. However, studies addressing eye contact in individuals with ASD can elicit mixed results. Response to eye contact has been identified as stronger neuro-physiologically for direct gaze than indirect gaze.[19] This may be due to individuals with ASD responding faster to eye contact based on their detection of features, rather than in the facial context.[20]

See also

References

  1. 1.0 1.1 1.2 Senju, Atsushi; Johnson, Mark H. (2009-02-13). "The eye contact effect: mechanisms and development" (in en). Trends in Cognitive Sciences 13 (3): 127–134. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2008.11.009. PMID 19217822. https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S1364661309000199. 
  2. 2.0 2.1 Johnson, Mark H.; Griffin, Richard; Csibra, Gergely; Halit, Hanife; Farroni, Teresa; De Haan, Michelle; Tucker, Leslie A.; Baron–Cohen, Simon et al. (2005). "The emergence of the social brain network: Evidence from typical and atypical development" (in en). Development and Psychopathology 17 (3): 599–619. doi:10.1017/S0954579405050297. ISSN 0954-5794. PMID 16262984. 
  3. Hoffman, Elizabeth A.; Haxby, James V. (January 2000). "Distinct representations of eye gaze and identity in the distributed human neural system for face perception" (in en). Nature Neuroscience 3 (1): 80–84. doi:10.1038/71152. ISSN 1097-6256. PMID 10607399. http://www.nature.com/articles/nn0100_80. 
  4. Frith, Chris D.; Frith, Uta (2006-05-18). "The Neural Basis of Mentalizing" (in en). Neuron 50 (4): 531–534. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2006.05.001. PMID 16701204. https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0896627306003448. 
  5. Singer, Tania (2006). "The neuronal basis and ontogeny of empathy and mind reading: Review of literature and implications for future research" (in en). Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 30 (6): 855–863. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.06.011. PMID 16904182. https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0149763406000522. 
  6. Pelphrey, Kevin A.; Morris, James P. (June 2006). "Brain Mechanisms for Interpreting the Actions of Others From Biological-Motion Cues" (in en). Current Directions in Psychological Science 15 (3): 136–140. doi:10.1111/j.0963-7214.2006.00423.x. ISSN 0963-7214. PMID 18079992. 
  7. 7.0 7.1 George, Nathalie; Driver, Jon; Dolan, Raymond J. (June 2001). "Seen Gaze-Direction Modulates Fusiform Activity and Its Coupling with Other Brain Areas during Face Processing" (in en). NeuroImage 13 (6): 1102–1112. doi:10.1006/nimg.2001.0769. PMID 11352615. https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S1053811901907694. 
  8. Calder, Andrew J; Lawrence, Andrew D; Keane, Jill; Scott, Sophie K; Owen, Adrian M; Christoffels, Ingrid; Young, Andrew W (2002-01-01). "Reading the mind from eye gaze" (in en). Neuropsychologia 40 (8): 1129–1138. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00008-8. ISSN 0028-3932. PMID 11931917. https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0028393202000088. 
  9. Conty, Laurence; N’Diaye, Karim; Tijus, Charles; George, Nathalie (2007-10-01). "When eye creates the contact! ERP evidence for early dissociation between direct and averted gaze motion processing" (in en). Neuropsychologia 45 (13): 3024–3037. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.05.017. PMID 17644145. https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0028393207002102. 
  10. Wicker, Bruno; Perrett, David I; Baron-Cohen, Simon; Decety, Jean (2003). "Being the target of another's emotion: a PET study" (in en). Neuropsychologia 41 (2): 139–146. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00144-6. PMID 12459212. https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0028393202001446. 
  11. Nichols, K. A.; Champness, B. G. (1971-11-01). "Eye gaze and the GSR" (in en). Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 7 (6): 623–626. doi:10.1016/0022-1031(71)90024-2. ISSN 0022-1031. https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0022-1031%2871%2990024-2. 
  12. Kawashima, Ryuta; Sugiura, Motoaki; Kato, Takashi; Nakamura, Akinori; Hatano, Kentaro; Ito, Kengo; Fukuda, Hiroshi; Kojima, Shozo et al. (April 1999). "The human amygdala plays an important role in gaze monitoring" (in en). Brain 122 (4): 779–783. doi:10.1093/brain/122.4.779. ISSN 1460-2156. PMID 10219788. https://academic.oup.com/brain/article-lookup/doi/10.1093/brain/122.4.779. 
  13. Wicker, B., Perrett, D. I., Baron-Cohen, S., & Decety, J. (2003) Being the target of another's emotion: a PET study. Neuropsychologia, 41(2) 139-146.
  14. Johnson, Mark H. (2005-10-01). "Subcortical face processing" (in en). Nature Reviews Neuroscience 6 (10): 766–774. doi:10.1038/nrn1766. ISSN 1471-0048. PMID 16276354. https://www.nature.com/articles/nrn1766. 
  15. Farroni, Teresa; Massaccesi, Stefano; Menon, Enrica; Johnson, Mark H. (March 2007). "Direct gaze modulates face recognition in young infants" (in en). Cognition 102 (3): 396–404. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2006.01.007. PMID 16540101. https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0010027706000230. 
  16. Senju, Atsushi; Csibra, Gergely; Johnson, Mark H. (2008-08-01). "Understanding the referential nature of looking: Infants' preference for object-directed gaze" (in en). Cognition 108 (2): 303–319. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2008.02.009. ISSN 0010-0277. PMID 18371943. https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0010027708000383. 
  17. Smith, Alastair D.; Hood, Bruce M.; Hector, Karen (September 2006). "Eye remember you two: gaze direction modulates face recognition in a developmental study" (in en). Developmental Science 9 (5): 465–472. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00513.x. ISSN 1363-755X. PMID 16911448. https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00513.x. 
  18. Elsabbagh, Mayada; Volein, Agnes; Csibra, Gergely; Holmboe, Karla; Garwood, Holly; Tucker, Leslie; Krljes, Sanya; Baron-Cohen, Simon et al. (2009-01-01). "Neural Correlates of Eye Gaze Processing in the Infant Broader Autism Phenotype" (in en). Biological Psychiatry 65 (1): 31–38. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.09.034. PMID 19064038. https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0006322308012043. 
  19. Kylliäinen, Anneli; Hietanen, Jari K. (2006-03-23). "Skin Conductance Responses to Another Person's Gaze in Children with Autism" (in en). Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 36 (4): 517–525. doi:10.1007/s10803-006-0091-4. ISSN 0162-3257. PMID 16555137. http://link.springer.com/10.1007/s10803-006-0091-4. 
  20. Senju, Atsushi; Hasegawa, Toshikazu; Tojo, Yoshikuni (2005-11-01). "Does perceived direct gaze boost detection in adults and children with and without autism? The stare-in-the-crowd effect revisited". Visual Cognition 12 (8): 1474–1496. doi:10.1080/13506280444000797. ISSN 1350-6285. https://doi.org/10.1080/13506280444000797.