Philosophy:Nudge theory

From HandWiki
Revision as of 07:22, 5 February 2024 by TextAI2 (talk | contribs) (fix)
(diff) ← Older revision | Latest revision (diff) | Newer revision → (diff)
Short description: Concept in behavioral economics, political theory and behavioral sciences

Nudge theory is a concept in behavioral economics, decision making, behavioral policy, social psychology, consumer behavior, and related behavioral sciences[1][2][3][4] that proposes adaptive designs of the decision environment (choice architecture) as ways to influence the behavior and decision-making of groups or individuals. Nudging contrasts with other ways to achieve compliance, such as education, legislation or enforcement.[5]

The nudge concept was popularized in the 2008 book Nudge: Improving Decisions About Health, Wealth, and Happiness, by behavioral economist Richard Thaler and legal scholar Cass Sunstein, two American scholars at the University of Chicago. It has influenced British and American politicians. Several nudge units exist around the world at the national level (UK, Germany, Japan, and others) as well as at the international level (e.g. World Bank, United Nations , and the European Commission).[6] It is disputed whether "nudge theory" is a recent novel development in behavioral economics or merely a new term for one of many methods for influencing behavior, investigated in the science of behavior analysis.[1][7]

There have been some controversies regarding effectiveness of nudges. Maier et al. wrote that, after correcting the publication bias found by Mertens et al., there is no evidence that nudging would have any effect.[8] "Nudging" is an umbrella term referring to many techniques, and skeptics believe some nudges (e.g. default effect) can be highly effective while others have little to no effect, and call for future work that shift away from investigating average effects but focus on moderators instead.[9] A meta analysis of all unpublished nudging studies carried by nudge units with over 23 million individuals in the United Kingdom and United States found support for many nudges, but with substantially weaker effects than effects found in published studies.[10] Moreover, some researchers criticized the "one-nudge-for-all" approach and advocated for more studies and implementations of personalized nudging (based on individual differences), which appear to be substantially more effective, with a more robust and consistent evidence base.[11][12]

Nudges

Definition

The first formulation of the term nudge and associated principles was developed in cybernetics by James Wilk before 1995 and described by Brunel University academic D. J. Stewart as "the art of the nudge" (sometimes referred to as micronudges).[13] It also drew on methodological influences from clinical psychotherapy tracing back to Gregory Bateson, including contributions from Milton Erickson, Watzlawick, Weakland and Fisch, and Bill O'Hanlon.[14] In this variant, the nudge is a microtargeted design geared toward a specific group of people, irrespective of the scale of intended intervention.

In 2008, Richard Thaler and Cass Sunstein's book Nudge: Improving Decisions About Health, Wealth, and Happiness brought nudge theory to prominence.[15][16] The authors refer to the influencing of behaviour without coercion as libertarian paternalism and the influencers as choice architects.[17][18]

Thaler and Sunstein defined their concept as the following:[19]:6

A nudge, as we will use the term, is any aspect of the choice architecture that alters people's behavior in a predictable way without forbidding any options or significantly changing their economic incentives. To count as a mere nudge, the intervention must be easy and cheap to avoid. Nudges are not mandates. Putting fruit at eye level counts as a nudge. Banning junk food does not.

In this form, drawing on behavioral economics, the nudge is more generally applied in order to influence behaviour.

One of the most frequently cited examples of a nudge is the etching of the image of a housefly into the men's room urinals at Amsterdam's Schiphol Airport, which is intended to "improve the aim."[19]

A fly image at the bottom of a urinal has been proven to improve men's aim, leading to lowered cleaning costs.[20]

The book also gained a following among US and UK politicians, in the private sector and in public health.[21]

Overview

A nudge makes it more likely that an individual will make a particular choice, or behave in a particular way, by altering the environment so that automatic cognitive processes are triggered to favour the desired outcome.[22][23]

An individual's behaviour is not always in alignment with their intentions (a discrepancy known as a value-action gap).[23] It is common knowledge that humans are not fully rational beings; that is, people will often do something that is not in their own self-interest, even when they are aware that their actions are not in their best interest.[16] As an example, when hungry, people who diet often underestimate their ability to lose weight, and their intentions to eat healthy can be temporarily weakened until they are satiated.[24]

Nobel Laureate Daniel Kahneman describes two distinct systems for processing information as to why people sometimes act against their own self-interest: System 1 is fast, automatic, and highly susceptible to environmental influences; System 2 processing is slow, reflective, and takes into account explicit goals and intentions.[22] When situations are overly complex or overwhelming for an individual's cognitive capacity, or when an individual is faced with time-constraints or other pressures, System 1 processing takes over decision-making.[23][25] System 1 processing relies on various judgmental heuristics to make decisions, resulting in faster decisions.[25] Unfortunately, this can also lead to suboptimal decisions. In fact, Thaler and Sunstein[19] trace maladaptive behaviour to situations in which System 1 processing overrides an individual's explicit values and goals.[23] It is well documented that habitual behaviour is resistant to change without a disruption to the environmental cues that trigger that behaviour.[25]

Nudging techniques aim to use judgmental heuristics to the advantage of the party that is creating the set of choices. In other words, a nudge alters the environment so that when heuristic, or System 1, decision-making is used, the resulting choice will be the most positive or desired outcome.[25] An example of such a nudge is switching the placement of junk food in a store, so that fruit and other healthy options are located next to the cash register, while junk food is relocated to another part of the store.[26]

Types

Nudges are small changes in the environment that are easy and inexpensive to implement.[22] Several different techniques exist for nudging, including defaults, social-proof heuristics, and increasing the salience of the desired option.

A default option is the option that person automatically receives for doing nothing. People are more likely to choose a particular option if it is the default option.[25] For example, Pichert & Katsikopoulos (2008) found that a greater number of consumers chose the renewable energy option for electricity when it was offered as the default option.[27] Similarly, the default options given to mobile apps developers in advertising networks can significantly impact consumers' privacy.[28]

A social-proof heuristic refers to the tendency of people to look at the behavior of others to help guide their own behavior. Studies have found some success in using social-proof heuristics to nudge people to make healthier food choices.[29]

When people's attention is drawn toward a particular option, that option will become more salient and they will be more likely to choose it. As an example, in snack shops at train stations in the Netherlands, consumers purchased more fruit and healthy snack options when they were relocated next to the cash register.[26] Since then, other similar studies have been made regarding the placement of healthier food options close to the checkout counter and the effect on the consuming behavior of the customers and this is now considered an effective and well-accepted nudge.[30]

Application

Behavioral insights and nudges are currently used in many countries around the world.[31][32]

Government

In the 1938 Austrian Anschluss referendum,[33] the larger centered circle was "a 'subtle' hint to the people, to help them understand which way to vote."[34] The intended result, a Ja ("yes") to the annexation of Austria into Germany with Hitler as its leader, was finally achieved through this and other blunter measures.
In the 1978 Chilean national consultation, in addition to the biased question text, the SI ("Yes" to Pinochet and his government) is under a sketch of the flag of Chile while NO is under a lower dark flag. This nudge[33] and other measures got a 79% support for "yes".

There are various notable examples of government applications of nudge theory.

During their terms, both U.K. Prime Minister David Cameron and U.S. President Barack Obama may have sought to employ nudge theory to advance domestic policy goals in their respective countries.[35] In 2008, the United States appointed Cass Sunstein, who helped develop the theory, as administrator of the Office of Information and Regulatory Affairs.[17][36][37] In 2010, the British Behavioural Insights Team, or "Nudge Unit," was established at the British Cabinet Office and headed by psychologist David Halpern.[38]

In Australia, the state Government of New South Wales established a Nudge Unit of its own in 2012.[39] In 2016, the federal government followed suit, forming the Behavioural Economics Team of Australia (BETA) as the "central unit for applying behavioural insights...to public policy."[40]

In 2020, the British government of Boris Johnson decided to rely on nudge theory to fight the coronavirus pandemic, with Chief Scientific Adviser Patrick Vallance seeking to encourage “herd immunity” with this strategy.[41]

Business

Nudge theory has also been applied to business management and corporate culture.

For instance, nudge is applied to health, safety, and environment (HSE) with the primary goals of achieving a "zero accident culture."[42] The concept is also used as a key component in a lot of human-resources software.

Particular forerunners in the application of nudge theory in corporate settings are top Silicon Valley companies. These companies are using nudges in various forms to increase productivity and happiness of employees. Recently, more companies are gaining interest in using what is called "nudge management" to improve the productivity of their white-collar workers.[43]

Healthcare

Lately, nudge theory has also been used in different ways to help healthcare professionals make more deliberate decisions in numerous areas. For example, nudging has been used as a way to improve hand hygiene among healthcare workers to decrease the number of healthcare-associated infections.[44] It has also been used as a way to make fluid administration a more thought-out decision in intensive care units, with the intention of reducing well known complications of fluid overload.[45]

Mandatory display of inspector reports of eatery hygiene as a public 'nudge', have received mixed responses in different countries. A recent meta-analytic review of the hygiene ratings across North America, Europe, Asia, and Oceania has shown that inspector ratings (usually a smiley or a letter grade) is useful at times, but not informative enough for consumers. [46]

Fundraising

Nudge theory can also be applied to fundraising, helping to increase donor contributions and increase continuous donations from the same individual, as well as to entice new donors to give.[47]

There are some simple strategies used when applying nudge theory to this area.[47] The first strategy is to make donating easy: creating default settings that automatically enroll a donor for continuous giving or prompts them to give every so often encourages individuals to continue giving. The second strategy to increase donors is to make giving more enticing, which can include increasing a person's motivation to give through rewards, personalized messages, or focusing on their interests. Personalized messages,[48] small thank-you gifts,[47] and demonstrating the impact one's donation can have on others, have been shown to be more effective when increasing donations.[49][50] Another strategy helpful to increasing donors is using social influence, as people are very influenced by group norms.[51] By allowing donors to become visible to the public and increasing their identifiability, other individuals will be more inclined to give as they conform to the social norms around them.[47] Using peer effects has been shown to increase donations.[52] Finally, timing is important: many studies have demonstrated that there are specific times when individuals are more likely to give,[53] for example during holidays.

Although many nudging theories have been useful to increase donations and donors, many scholars question the ethics of using such techniques on the population.[54] Ruehle et al. (2020), state that one has to always consider an individual's autonomy when designing nudges for a fundraising campaign. They state that the power of others behind messaging and potentially intrusive prompting can cause concern and may be seen as manipulative of donor's autonomy.[54]

Artificial intelligence

Nudges are used at many levels in AI algorithms, for example recommender systems, and their consequences are still being investigated. Two articles appeared in Minds & Machines in 2018 addressed the relation between nudges and Artificial Intelligence, explaining how persuasion and psychometrics can be used by personalised targeting algorithms to influence individual and collective behaviour, sometimes also in unintended ways. [55] [56] In 2020 an article in AI & Society addressed the use of this technology in Algorithmic Regulation. [57]

A piece in the Harvard Business Review published in 2021 was one of the first articles to coin the term "Algorithmic Nudging" (see also Algorithmic Management). The author stresses "Companies are increasingly using algorithms to manage and control individuals not by force, but rather by nudging them into desirable behavior — in other words, learning from their personalized data and altering their choices in some subtle way."[58]

While the concept builds on the work by University of Chicago economist Richard Thaler and Harvard Law School professor Cass Sunstein, "due to recent advances in AI and machine learning, algorithmic nudging is much more powerful than its non-algorithmic counterpart. With so much data about workers’ behavioral patterns at their fingertips, companies can now develop personalized strategies for changing individuals’ decisions and behaviors at large scale. These algorithms can be adjusted in real-time, making the approach even more effective."[58]

Tourism

One concern researchers in enjoyment-focused contexts, such as tourism, raised is a gap between attitude, intention and behaviour[59][60] because tourists seek pleasure.[61] Several empirical pieces of evidence in the tourism suggest the nudge theory's high effectiveness in reducing the burden of tourists' activities on the environment.[62][63] For instance, tourists consumed more ethical foods,[64] selected more sustainable hotels,[65][66] reused towels and bed linen during hotel stays,[67] increased their intentions to reduce their energy consumption,[68] increased the adoption of tourists' voluntary carbon offsetting[69] and many other examples.

Critique

The evidence on nudging having any effect has been criticized as "limited", so Mertens et al. produced a comprehensive meta-analysis. They found that nudging is effective but there is a moderate publication bias. Later Maier et al. computed that, after correcting for this publication bias appropriately, there is no evidence that nudging would have any effect.[8]

Tammy Boyce, from the public health foundation The King's Fund, has said: "We need to move away from short-term, politically motivated initiatives such as the 'nudging people' idea, which are not based on any good evidence and don't help people make long-term behaviour changes."[70] Likewise, Mols et al. (2015), acknowledge nudges may at times be useful, but argue that covert nudges offer limited scope for securing lasting behavior change.[7]

Cass Sunstein has responded to criticism at length in his 2016 book, The Ethics of Influence: Government in the Age of Behavioral Science,[71] making the case in favor of nudging, against charges that nudges diminish autonomy,[72] threaten dignity, violate liberties, or reduce welfare. He previously defended nudge theory in his 2014 book Why Nudge?: The Politics of Libertarian Paternalism[73] by arguing that choice architecture is inevitable and that some form of paternalism cannot be avoided.

Ethicists have debated nudge theory rigorously.[74] These charges have been made by various participants in the debate from Bovens (2009)[75] to Goodwin (2012).[76] Wilkinson, for example, charges nudges for being manipulative, while others such as Yeung (2012) question their scientific credibility.[77]

Public opinion on the ethicality of nudges has also been shown to be susceptible to “partisan nudge bias.”[78] Research from David Tannenbaum, Craig R. Fox, and Todd Rogers (2017) found that adults and policymakers in the United States believed behavioral policies to be more ethical when they aligned with their own political leanings.[78] Conversely, people took these same mechanisms to be more unethical when they differed from their politics.[78] The researchers also found that nudges are not inherently partisan: when evaluating behavioral policies absent of political cues, people across the political spectrum were alike in their assessments.[78]

Some, such as Hausman and Welch (2010)[79] as well as Roberts (2018)[80] have inquired whether nudging should be permissible on grounds of distributive justice. Though Roberts (2018) argued that nudges do not benefit vulnerable, low-income individuals as much as individuals who are less vulnerable, some empirical research suggests that nudges benefit low-income and low-SES people most, if anything increasing distributive justice.[81] Lepenies and Malecka (2015)[82] have questioned whether nudges are compatible with the rule of law. Similarly, legal scholars have discussed the role of nudges and the law.[83][84]

Behavioral economists such as Bob Sugden have pointed out that the underlying normative benchmark of nudging is still homo economicus, despite the proponents' claim to the contrary.[85]

It has been remarked that nudging is also a euphemism for psychological manipulation as practiced in social engineering.[86][87]

There exists an anticipation and, simultaneously, implicit criticism of the nudge theory in works of Hungarian social psychologists Ferenc Mérei[88] and László Garai,[89] who emphasize the active participation in the nudge of its target.

The authors of a book titled Neuroliberalism: Behavioural Government in the Twenty-First Century (2017), argue that, while there is much value and diversity in behavioural approaches to government, there are significant ethical issues, including the danger of the neurological sciences being co-opted to the needs of neo-liberal economics.[90]

See also

References

  1. 1.0 1.1 Tagliabue, Marco; Simon, Carsta (2018). "Feeding the behavioral revolution : Contributions of behavior analysis to nudging and vice versa" (in en). Journal of Behavioral Economics for Policy 2 (1): 91–97. 
  2. Hansen, Pelle Guldborg (2016). "The Definition of Nudge and Libertarian Paternalism: Does the Hand Fit the Glove?". European Journal of Risk Regulation 7 (1): 155–174. doi:10.1017/S1867299X00005468. 
  3. Capraro, Valerio; Jagfeld, Glorianna; Klein, Rana; Mul, Mathijs; de Pol, Iris van (2019). "Increasing altruistic and cooperative behaviour with simple moral nudges". Scientific Reports 9 (1): 11880. doi:10.1038/s41598-019-48094-4. PMID 31417106. 
  4. Mongin, Philippe; Cozic, Mikaël (2018). "Rethinking nudge: not one but three concepts". Behavioural Public Policy 2 (1): 107–124. doi:10.1017/bpp.2016.16. 
  5. Gane, Nicholas (2021). "Nudge Economics as Libertarian Paternalism". Theory, Culture & Society 38 (6): 119–142. doi:10.1177/0263276421999447. 
  6. "Behavioural Insights". 2015-05-06. https://ec.europa.eu/jrc/en/research/crosscutting-activities/behavioural-insights. 
  7. 7.0 7.1 Mols, F.; Haslam, S. A.; Jetten, J.; Steffens, N. K. (2015). "Why a nudge is not enough: A social identity critique of governance by stealth". European Journal of Political Research 54 (1): 81–98. doi:10.1111/1475-6765.12073. 
  8. 8.0 8.1 Maximilian Maier, František Bartoš, T. D. Stanley, David R. Shanks, Adam J. L. Harris, and Eric-Jan Wagenmakers (July 19, 2022). "No evidence for nudging after adjusting for publication bias". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 119 (31): e2200300119. doi:10.1073/pnas.2200300119. PMID 35858389. PMC 9351501. Bibcode2022PNAS..11900300M. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2200300119. 
  9. Szaszi, Barnabas; Higney, Anthony; Charlton, Aaron; Gelman, Andrew; Ziano, Ignazio; Aczel, Balazs; Goldstein, Daniel G.; Yeager, David Scott et al. (2022). "No reason to expect large and consistent effects of nudge interventions". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 119 (31): e2200732119. doi:10.31234/osf.io/mwhf3. PMID 35858388. PMC 9351519. https://psyarxiv.com/mwhf3/. 
  10. DellaVigna, Stefano; Linos, Elizabeth (January 2022). "RCTS to Scale: Comprehensive Evidence from Two Nudge Units". Econometrica 90 (1). doi:10.3982/ECTA18709. https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.3982/ECTA18709. 
  11. Ludolph, Ramona; Schulz, Peter J. (March 2015). "Does regulatory fit lead to more effective health communication? A systematic review". Social Science & Medicine 128: 142–150. doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2015.01.021. PMID 25617673. https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0277953615000295. 
  12. Mills, Stuart (January 2022). "Personalized nudging" (in en). Behavioural Public Policy 6 (1): 150–159. doi:10.1017/bpp.2020.7. https://www.cambridge.org/core/services/aop-cambridge-core/content/view/E854A04226DEA94B623ECA2ACF64C8D0/S2398063X2000007Xa.pdf/personalized_nudging.pdf. Retrieved 5 December 2022. 
  13. Wilk, J. (1999), "Mind, Nature and the Emerging Science of Change: An Introduction to Metamorphology", in G. Cornelis; S. Smets; J. Van Bendegem, Metadebates on Science, 6, Springer Netherlands, pp. 71–87, doi:10.1007/978-94-017-2245-2_6, ISBN 978-90-481-5242-1 
  14. O'Hanlon, Bill, and Wilk, James (1987), Shifting Contexts: The generation of effective psychotherapy, New York: Guilford Press. 
  15. "Nudge Theory". https://www.businessballs.com/improving-workplace-performance/nudge-theory/. 
  16. 16.0 16.1 Kosters, Mark; Van der Heijden, Jeroen (2015). "From mechanism to virtue: evaluating nudge theory". Evaluation 21 (3): 276–91. doi:10.1177/1356389015590218. https://openresearch-repository.anu.edu.au/bitstream/1885/71637/8/01_Kosters_From_Mechanism_to_Virtue_2015.pdf. 
  17. 17.0 17.1 Andrew Sparrow (2008-08-22). "Speak 'Nudge': The 10 key phrases from David Cameron's favorite book". The Guardian (London). https://www.theguardian.com/politics/blog/2008/aug/22/davidcameron.conservatives. 
  18. Carrasco-Villanueva, Marco (2017-10-18). "Richard Thaler y el auge de la Economía Conductual" (in es). Lucidez. http://lucidez.pe/economia/richard-thaler-y-el-auge-de-la-economia-conductual-por-marco-carrasco/. 
  19. 19.0 19.1 19.2 Thaler, Richard, and Cass Sunstein (2008). Nudge: Improving Decisions About Health, Wealth, and Happiness. Penguin Books. 
  20. Have you been nudged? BBC, 2017
  21. See: Dr. Jennifer Lunt and Malcolm Staves
  22. 22.0 22.1 22.2 Saghai, Yashar (2013). "Salvaging the concept of nudge". Journal of Medical Ethics 39 (8): 487–93. doi:10.1136/medethics-2012-100727. PMID 23427215. 
  23. 23.0 23.1 23.2 23.3 Parkinson, J.A.; Eccles, K.E.; Goodman, A. (2014). "Positive impact by design: the Wales centre for behaviour change". The Journal of Positive Psychology 9 (6): 517–522. doi:10.1080/17439760.2014.936965. 
  24. Nordgren, L.; Van der Pligt, J.; van Harreveld, F. (2008). "The instability of health cognitions: visceral states influence self-efficacy and related health beliefs". Health Psychology 27 (6): 722–727. doi:10.1037/0278-6133.27.6.722. PMID 19025267. 
  25. 25.0 25.1 25.2 25.3 25.4 Campbell-Arvai, V; Arvai, J.; Kalof, L. (2014). "Motivating sustainable food choices: the role of nudges, value orientation, and information provision". Environment and Behavior 46 (4): 453–475. doi:10.1177/0013916512469099. 
  26. 26.0 26.1 Kroese, F.; Marchiori, D.; de Ridder, D. (2016). "Nudging healthy food choices: a field experiment at the train station". Journal of Public Health 38 (2): e133-7. doi:10.1093/pubmed/fdv096. PMID 26186924. 
  27. Pichert, D.; Katsikopoulos, K.V. (2008). "Green defaults: information presentation and pro-environmental behavior". Journal of Environmental Psychology 28: 63–73. doi:10.1016/j.jenvp.2007.09.004. 
  28. Tahaei, Mohammad; Frik, Alisa; Vaniea, Kami (2021). "Deciding on Personalized Ads: Nudging Developers About User Privacy". USENIX Symposium on Usable Privacy and Security (SOUPS). doi:10.1145/3411763.3451805. 
  29. Cheung, T.; Kroese, F.; Fennis, B.; de Ridder, D. (2017). "The hunger games: using hunger to promote healthy choices in self-control conflicts". Appetite 116: 401–409. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2017.05.020. PMID 28502739. https://pure.rug.nl/ws/files/47228781/1_s2.0_S0195666316308029_main.pdf. 
  30. Van Gestel, LC (2018). "Nudging at the checkout counter - A longitudinal study of the effect of a food repositioning nudge on healthy food choice.". Psychol Health. 6 (33): 800–809. doi:10.1080/08870446.2017.1416116. PMID 29252010. 
  31. Carrasco-Villanueva, Marco (2016). "中国的环境公共政策:一个行为经济学的选择 [Environmental Public Policies in China: An Opportunity for Behavioral Economics"]. in 上海社会科学院 [Shanghai Academy of Social Sciences] (in zh). 2016上海青年汉学家研修计划论文集. 中国社会科学出版社 [China Social Sciences Press]. pp. 368–392. ISBN 9781234567897. https://archive.org/details/guidetolcshinfor00doej/page/368. 
  32. Bradshaw, Della (2015-11-15). "How a little nudge can lead to better decisions" (in en-GB). Financial Times (Financial Times LTD). https://www.ft.com/content/e98e2018-70ca-11e5-ad6d-f4ed76f0900a. 
  33. 33.0 33.1 Sammut, Gordon; Bauer, Martin W. (7 January 2021) (in en). The Psychology of Social Influence: Modes and Modalities of Shifting Common Sense. Cambridge University Press. pp. 207–208. ISBN 978-1-108-24441-1. https://books.google.com/books?id=5GEQEAAAQBAJ&pg=PA208. Retrieved 9 September 2021. 
  34. Chaim Walder (2011). People Speak 4. Feldheim Publ. pp. 23–24. ISBN 978-1-59826-669-6. 
  35. "First Obama, now Cameron embraces 'nudge theory'". 12 August 2010. https://www.independent.co.uk/news/uk/politics/first-obama-now-cameron-embraces-nudge-theory-2050127.html. 
  36. Carol Lewis (2009-07-22). "Why Barack Obama and David Cameron are keen to 'nudge' you". The Times (London). http://business.timesonline.co.uk/tol/business/career_and_jobs/article4330267.ece. 
  37. James Forsyth (2009-07-16). "Nudge, nudge: meet the Cameroons' new guru". The Spectator. http://www.spectator.co.uk/the-magazine/features/839676/nudge-nudge-meet-the-cameroons-new-guru.thtml. Retrieved 2009-09-09. 
  38. "About Us". https://www.bi.team/about-us/. 
  39. "About Us". NSW Department of Premier and Cabinet. https://www.dpc.nsw.gov.au/programs-and-services/behavioural-insights/about-us/. 
  40. "Behavioural Insights". http://bi.dpc.nsw.gov.au/. 
  41. "The UK's Covid-19 strategy dangerously leaves too many questions unanswered". 15 March 2020. https://www.theguardian.com/commentisfree/2020/mar/15/uk-covid-19-strategy-questions-unanswered-coronavirus-outbreak. 
  42. "Cast No Shadow". http://www.rydermarsh.co.uk/pdfs/SHP.0112.pdf. 
  43. Ebert, Philip; Freibichler, Wolfgang (2017). "Nudge management: applying behavioural science to increase knowledge worker productivity". Journal of Organization Design 6 (4). doi:10.1186/s41469-017-0014-1. 
  44. Caris, MG (2018). "Nudging to improve hand hygiene". J Hosp Infect 98 (4): 352–358. doi:10.1016/j.jhin.2017.09.023. PMID 28974467. 
  45. Horst, Kawati, Rasmusson, Pikwer, Castegren, Lipscey (2018). "Impact of resuscitation fluid bag size availability on volume of fluid administration in the intensive care unit.". Acta Anaesthesiol. Scand. 9 (62): 1261–1266. doi:10.1111/aas.13161. PMID 29851027. 
  46. Awasthi, Bhuvanesh (2021). "Does the food safety ranking system act as a nudge?". International Journal of Applied Behavioral Economics 10 (4): 42–58. doi:10.4018/IJABE.2021100104. http://doi.org/10.4018/IJABE.2021100104. 
  47. 47.0 47.1 47.2 47.3 Behavioural Insights Team. 2013 May 24. "Applying behavioural insights to charitable giving." London: Cabinet Office and Charities Aid Foundation. pp. 1-25.
  48. Behavioural Insights Team. 2012 February 6. "Applying behavioural insights to reduce fraud, error and debt." London: Cabinet Office. pp. 185–86.
  49. Small, D. A., and G. Loewenstein. 2003. "Helping a victim or helping the victim: Altruism and identifiability." Journal of Risk and uncertainty 26(1):5-16.
  50. Small, D. A., G. Loewenstein, and P. Slovic. 2007. "Sympathy and callousness: The impact of deliberative thought on donations to identifiable and statistical victims." Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 102(2):143–53.
  51. Bernheim, B. D. 1994. "A theory of conformity." Journal of political Economy 102(5):841-77.
  52. Carman, K. G. (2004). Social influences and the designation of charitable contributions: evidence from the workplace. Harvard University Journal, 1.
  53. Breman, A. 2011. "Give more tomorrow: Two field experiments on altruism and intertemporal choice." Journal of Public Economics 95(11-12):1349-57.
  54. 54.0 54.1 Ruehle, R. C., B. Engelen, and A. Archer. 2020. "Nudging Charitable Giving: What (If Anything) Is Wrong With It?" Nonprofit and Voluntary Sector Quarterly 0899764020954266.
  55. Burr, Christopher; Cristianini, Nello; Ladyman, James (2018). "An analysis of the interaction between intelligent software agents and human users.". Minds and Machines 28 (4): 735–774. doi:10.1007/s11023-018-9479-0. PMID 30930542. 
  56. Burr, Christopher; Cristianini, Nello (2019). "Can machines read our minds?". Minds and Machines 29 (3): 461–494. doi:10.1007/s11023-019-09497-4. 
  57. Cristianini, Nello; Scantamburlo, teresa (2020). "On social machines for algorithmic regulation.". AI & Society 35 (3): 645–662. doi:10.1007/s00146-019-00917-8. https://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s00146-019-00917-8. 
  58. 58.0 58.1 Möhlmann, Mareike (April 22, 2021). "Algorithmic Nudges Don't Have to Be Unethical". Harvard Business Review. https://hbr.org/2021/04/algorithmic-nudges-dont-have-to-be-unethical. 
  59. Souza-Neto, Valério; Marques, Osiris (2022) (in English). Attitude-behaviour gap. Edward Elgar Publishing. ISBN 978-1-80037-747-9. https://www.elgaronline.com/view/book/9781800377486/b-9781800377486.attitude.behaviour.gap.xml. 
  60. Dolnicar, Sara (September 2020). "Designing for more environmentally friendly tourism". Annals of Tourism Research 84: 102933. doi:10.1016/j.annals.2020.102933. ISSN 0160-7383. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.annals.2020.102933. 
  61. Dai, Fengwei; Wang, Dan; Kirillova, Ksenia (June 2022). "Travel inspiration in tourist decision making". Tourism Management 90: 104484. doi:10.1016/j.tourman.2021.104484. ISSN 0261-5177. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tourman.2021.104484. 
  62. Marques, Osiris Ricardo Bezerra; de Souza Neto, Valério Rodrigues (2022-07-28) (in en), Behavioural Nudging, Edward Elgar Publishing, doi:10.4337/9781800377486.behavioural.nudging, ISBN 978-1-80037-747-9, http://www.elgaronline.com/view/nlm-book/9781800377479/b-9781800377486.behavioural.nudging.xml, retrieved 2022-08-25 
  63. Souza-Neto, Valério; Marques, Osiris; Mayer, Verônica Feder; Lohmann, Gui (2022-02-06). "Lowering the harm of tourist activities: a systematic literature review on nudges". Journal of Sustainable Tourism 31 (9): 2173–2194. doi:10.1080/09669582.2022.2036170. ISSN 0966-9582. https://doi.org/10.1080/09669582.2022.2036170. 
  64. Cozzio, Claudia; Volgger, Michael; Taplin, Ross; Woodside, Arch G. (2020-09-01). "Nurturing tourists' ethical food consumption: Testing the persuasive strengths of alternative messages in a natural hotel setting" (in en). Journal of Business Research 117: 268–279. doi:10.1016/j.jbusres.2020.05.050. ISSN 0148-2963. https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0148296320303568. 
  65. Cui, Yuanyuan (Gina); Errmann, Amy; Kim, Jungkeun; Seo, Yuri; Xu, Yingzi; Zhao, Fang (July 2020). "Moral Effects of Physical Cleansing and Pro-environmental Hotel Choices" (in en). Journal of Travel Research 59 (6): 1105–1118. doi:10.1177/0047287519872821. ISSN 0047-2875. http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1177/0047287519872821. 
  66. Kim, Jungkeun; Kim, Seongseop (Sam); Lee, Jin-Soo; Kim, Peter Beomcheol; Cui, Yuanyuan (Gina) (March 2020). "Influence of Choice Architecture on the Preference for a Pro-Environmental Hotel" (in en). Journal of Travel Research 59 (3): 512–527. doi:10.1177/0047287519841718. ISSN 0047-2875. http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1177/0047287519841718. 
  67. Gössling, Stefan; Araña, Jorge E.; Aguiar-Quintana, J. Teresa (2019-02-01). "Towel reuse in hotels: Importance of normative appeal designs" (in en). Tourism Management 70: 273–283. doi:10.1016/j.tourman.2018.08.027. ISSN 0261-5177. https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S026151771830205X. 
  68. Chang, Howook “Sean”; Huh, Chang; Lee, Myong Jae (May 2016). "Would an Energy Conservation Nudge in Hotels Encourage Hotel Guests to Conserve?" (in en). Cornell Hospitality Quarterly 57 (2): 172–183. doi:10.1177/1938965515588132. ISSN 1938-9655. http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1177/1938965515588132. 
  69. Kim, Haeok Liz; Hyun, Sunghyup Sean (2021-07-03). "The anchoring effect of aviation green tax for sustainable tourism, based on the nudge theory". Journal of Sustainable Tourism 29 (7): 1082–1097. doi:10.1080/09669582.2020.1820017. ISSN 0966-9582. https://doi.org/10.1080/09669582.2020.1820017. 
  70. Lakhani, Nina (December 7, 2008). "Unhealthy lifestyles here to stay, in spite of costly campaigns". The Independent (London). https://www.independent.co.uk/life-style/health-and-wellbeing/health-news/unhealthy-lifestyles-here-to-stay-in-spite-of-costly-campaigns-1055693.html. 
  71. Sunstein, Cass R. (2016-08-24) (in en). The Ethics of Influence: Government in the Age of Behavioral Science. Cambridge University Press. ISBN 9781107140707. https://books.google.com/books?id=TlvWDAAAQBAJ&q=ethics+of+influence+sunstein&pg=PR8. 
  72. Schubert, Christian (2015-10-12). "On the Ethics of Public Nudging: Autonomy and Agency". Social Science Research Network (Rochester, NY). https://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id=2672970. 
  73. Sunstein, Cass R. (2014) (in en). Why Nudge?: The Politics of Libertarian Paternalism. Yale University Press. ISBN 9780300212693. https://books.google.com/books?id=Rx3qAgAAQBAJ. 
  74. Barton, Adrien; Grüne-Yanoff, Till (2015-09-01). "From Libertarian Paternalism to Nudging—and Beyond" (in en). Review of Philosophy and Psychology 6 (3): 341–359. doi:10.1007/s13164-015-0268-x. ISSN 1878-5158. 
  75. Bovens, Luc (2009). "The Ethics of Nudge" (in en). Preference Change. Theory and Decision Library. Springer, Dordrecht. pp. 207–219. doi:10.1007/978-90-481-2593-7_10. ISBN 9789048125920. 
  76. Goodwin, Tom (2012-06-01). "Why We Should Reject 'Nudge'" (in en). Politics 32 (2): 85–92. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9256.2012.01430.x. ISSN 0263-3957. 
  77. Yeung, Karen (2012-01-01). "Nudge as Fudge" (in en). The Modern Law Review 75 (1): 122–148. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2230.2012.00893.x. ISSN 1468-2230. 
  78. 78.0 78.1 78.2 78.3 Tannenbaum, David; Fox, Craig R.; Rogers, Todd (2017-07-10). "On the misplaced politics of behavioural policy interventions". Nature Human Behaviour. https://scholar.harvard.edu/files/todd_rogers/files/tannenbaum_fox_rogers.2017.pdf. 
  79. Hausman, Daniel M.; Welch, Brynn (2010-03-01). "Debate: To Nudge or Not to Nudge*" (in en). Journal of Political Philosophy 18 (1): 123–136. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9760.2009.00351.x. ISSN 1467-9760. 
  80. Roberts, Jessica L. (2018-04-01). "Nudge-proof: Distributive Justice and the Ethics of Nudging*" (in en). Michigan Law Review 116 (6): 1045–1066. doi:10.36644/mlr.116.6.nudge-proof. ISSN 0026-2234. https://repository.law.umich.edu/mlr/vol116/iss6/13. 
  81. Mrkva, Kellen; Posner, Nathaniel A.; Reeck, Crystal; Johnson, Eric J. (2021-07-01). "Do Nudges Reduce Disparities? Choice Architecture Compensates for Low Consumer Knowledge*" (in en). Journal of Marketing 85 (4): 67–84. doi:10.1177/0022242921993186. ISSN 0022-2429. 
  82. Lepenies, Robert; Małecka, Magdalena (2015-09-01). "The Institutional Consequences of Nudging – Nudges, Politics, and the Law" (in en). Review of Philosophy and Psychology 6 (3): 427–437. doi:10.1007/s13164-015-0243-6. ISSN 1878-5158. 
  83. Alemanno, A.; Spina, A. (2014-04-01). "Nudging legally: On the checks and balances of behavioral regulation" (in en). International Journal of Constitutional Law 12 (2): 429–456. doi:10.1093/icon/mou033. ISSN 1474-2640. https://academic.oup.com/icon/article/12/2/429/710410/Nudging-legally-On-the-checks-and-balances-of. 
  84. Kemmerer, Alexandra; Möllers, Christoph; Steinbeis, Maximilian; Wagner, Gerhard (2016-07-15). "Choice Architecture in Democracies: Exploring the Legitimacy of Nudging - Preface". Baden-Barde/Hart Publishing (Rochester, NY). 
  85. Sugden, Robert (2017-06-01). "Do people really want to be nudged towards healthy lifestyles?" (in en). International Review of Economics 64 (2): 113–123. doi:10.1007/s12232-016-0264-1. ISSN 1865-1704. 
  86. Cass R. Sunstein. "NUDGING AND CHOICE ARCHITECTURE: ETHICAL CONSIDERATIONS". http://www.law.harvard.edu/programs/olin_center/papers/pdf/Sunstein_809.pdf. 
  87. "A nudge in the right direction? How we can harness behavioural economics". ABC News. 1 December 2015. http://www.abc.net.au/news/2015-12-01/berg-a-nudge-in-the-right-direction/6988786. 
  88. Mérei, Ferenc (1987). "A peremhelyzet egyik változata: a szociálpszichológiai kontúr" (in hu). Pszichológia 7: 1–24. 
  89. Garai, Laszlo (2017), "The Double-Storied Structure of Social Identity" (in en), Reconsidering Identity Economics: Human Well-Being and Governance (Palgrave Macmillan US): pp. 21–34, doi:10.1057/978-1-137-52561-1_2, ISBN 978-1-137-52561-1 
  90. Whitehead, Mark; Jones, Rhys; Lilley, Rachel; Pykett, Jessica; Howell, Rachel (2017). Neuroliberalism: Behavioural Government in the Twenty-First Century. Routledge. ISBN 9781138923829. https://books.google.com/books?id=l-80DwAAQBAJ. 

Further reading