Biography:Simon Baron-Cohen
Sir Simon Baron-Cohen | |
---|---|
Baron-Cohen in 2011 | |
Born | Simon Philip Baron-Cohen 15 August 1958 Hampstead, London, England |
Education |
|
Known for | Autism research |
Spouse(s) | Bridget Lindley (m. 1987; div. 2016) |
Awards | Kanner-Asperger Medal (2013) |
Scientific career | |
Fields | |
Institutions | University of Cambridge |
Thesis | Social Cognition and Pretend-Play in Autism (1985) |
Doctoral advisor | Uta Frith |
Sir Simon Philip Baron-Cohen FBA FBPsS FMedSci (born 15 August 1958)[1] is a British clinical psychologist and professor of developmental psychopathology at the University of Cambridge. He is the director of the university's Autism Research Centre and a Fellow of Trinity College.
In 1985, Baron-Cohen formulated the mindblindness theory of autism, the evidence for which he collated and published in 1995. In 1997, he formulated the prenatal sex steroid theory of autism, the key test of which was published in 2015. In 2003, he formulated the empathising-systemising (E-S) theory of autism and typical sex differences, the key test of which was published in 2018.
He has also made major contributions to research on autism prevalence and screening, autism genetics, autism neuroimaging, autism and vulnerability, autism intervention and synaesthesia. Baron-Cohen was knighted in the 2021 New Year Honours for services to autism.
Early life and education
Baron-Cohen was born into a middle-class Jewish family in London, the second son of Judith and Hyman Vivian Baron-Cohen.[2][3][4]
He completed a BA in human sciences at New College, Oxford, and an MPhil in clinical psychology at the Institute of Psychiatry, King's College London. He received a PhD in psychology at University College London;[5] his doctoral research was in collaboration with his supervisor Uta Frith.[6]
Research
In 1997, Baron-Cohen developed the "empathizing-systemizing (E-S) theory" which states that humans may be classified on the basis of their scores along two dimensions (empathizing and systemizing). The E-S theory argues that typical females on average score higher on empathizing relative to systemizing (they are more likely to have a brain of type E), and typical males on average score higher on systemizing relative to empathizing (they are more likely to have a brain of type S). Autistic people are predicted to score as an extreme of the typical male (they are more likely to have a brain of type S or extreme type S).[7][8]
Baron-Cohen's "assortative mating theory" states that if individuals with a "systemizing" focus or "type S" brain are selecting each other as mates, they are more likely to have children with autism.[8][9] This has been supported in a population study in Eindhoven, where autism rates are twice as high in that city that is an IT hub, compared to other Dutch cities.[10]
In 2001, he developed the autism-spectrum quotient (AQ), a set of fifty questions that can be used to help determine whether or not an adult exhibits symptoms of autism.[11] The AQ has subsequently been used in hundreds of studies including one study of half a million people, showing robust sex differences and higher scores in those who work in STEM.[7][12]
Prenatal neuroendocrinology
Baron-Cohen's work in E-S theory led him to investigate whether higher levels of prenatal testosterone explain the increased rate of autism among males.[8] His prenatal sex steroid theory of autism gained additional support in 2015 and 2019 in finding elevated prenatal androgens and estrogens[clarification needed] are associated with autism.[13][14]
In his 2004 book Prenatal Testosterone in Mind (MIT Press), Baron-Cohen put forward the prenatal sex steroid theory of autism.[15] He proposed this theory to understand why autism is more common in males. Using the Cambridge Child Development Project that he established in 1997, a longitudinal study studying children of 600 women who had undergone amniocentesis in pregnancy, he followed these children postnatally. This study demonstrated, for the first time in humans, how normative variation in amniotic prenatal testosterone levels correlates with individual differences in typical postnatal brain and behavioral development. His team discovered that in typical children, amount of eye contact, rate of vocabulary development, quality of social relationships, theory of mind performance, and scores on the empathy quotient are all inversely correlated with prenatal testosterone levels. In contrast, he found that scores on the embedded figures test (of attention to detail), on the systemizing quotient (SQ), measures of narrow interests, and number of autistic traits are positively correlated with prenatal testosterone levels.[16] Within this study his team conducted the first human neuroimaging studies of brain grey matter regional volumes and brain activity associated with prenatal testosterone.[17] Other clues for the theory came from Baron-Cohen's postnatal hormonal studies which found that autistic adults have elevated circulating androgens in serum[18] and that the autistic brain in women is ‘masculinized’ in both grey and white matter brain volume.[19] An independent animal model by Xu et al. (2015, Physiology and Behavior, 138, 13–20) showed that elevated prenatal testosterone during pregnancy leads to reduced social interest in the offspring. Baron-Cohen's group also studied the rate of autism in offspring of mothers with polycystic ovary syndrome (PCOS), a medical condition caused by elevated prenatal testosterone. He found that in women with PCOS, the odds of having a child with autism are significantly increased.[20] This has been replicated in three other countries (Sweden, Finland, and Israel) and is in line with the finding that mothers of autistic children themselves have elevated sex steroid hormones.[21][22] But to really test the theory, Baron-Cohen needed a much larger sample than his Cambridge Child Development Project, since autism only occurs in 1% of the population. So, in 2015, he set up a collaboration with the Danish Biobank which has stored over 20 thousand amniotic fluid samples which he linked to later diagnosis of autism via the Danish Psychiatric Register. He tested the prenatal androgens and found that children later diagnosed as autistic were exposed to elevated levels of prenatal testosterone, and the Δ4 sex steroid precursors to prenatal testosterone.[14] In 2019 he tested the same cohort's levels of exposure to prenatal estrogens and again found these were elevated in pregnancies that resulted in autism.[13] These novel studies provide evidence of the role of prenatal hormones, interacting with genetic predisposition, in the cause of autism.
Developmental social cognitive neuroscience
While a member of the Cognitive Development Unit (CDU) in London in 1985, to explain the social-communication deficits in autism, Baron-Cohen and his colleagues Frith and Alan Leslie formulated the "theory of mind" (ToM) hypothesis. ToM (also known as "cognitive empathy") is the brain's partially innate mechanism for rapidly making sense of social behavior by effortlessly attributing mental states to others, enabling behavioral prediction and social communication skills.[23][24] They confirmed this using the false belief test, showing that a typical four-year-old child can infer another person's belief that is different to their own, while autistic children on average are impaired in this ability.[24] Baron-Cohen's 1995 book Mindblindness summarized his subsequent experiments in ToM and its impairment in autism. He went on to show that children with autism are blind to the mentalistic significance of the eyes and show deficits in advanced ToM, measured by the "reading the mind in the eyes test" (or "eyes test") that he designed.[25] He conducted the first neuroimaging study of ToM in typical and autistic adults, and studied patients demonstrating lesions in the orbito- and medial-prefrontal cortex and amygdala can impair ToM.[26] He also reported the first evidence of atypical amygdala function in autism during ToM.[27] In 2017, his team studied 80K genotyped individuals who took the eyes test. He found Single Nucleotide Polymorphisms (SNPs) partly contribute to individual differences on this dimensional trait measure on which autistic people are impaired.[28] This is evidence that cognitive empathy/ToM is partly heritable. This also illustrates Baron-Cohen's approach to autism genetics that relates autism to individual differences in traits such as empathy and systemizing in the general population.[29] The National Institutes of Health recommended Baron-Cohen's eyes test as a core measure that should be used as part of the Research Domain Criteria (RDOC) for assessing social cognition.
Baron-Cohen developed the Mindreading software for special education.[30] His lab developed The Transporters, an animation series designed to teach children with autism to recognize and understand emotions. The series was also nominated for a BAFTA award.[9][31]
Reception
Spectrum News had described the work of Baron-Cohen on theory of mind as “a landmark study”.[32] The Lancet described him as “a man with extraordinary knowledge, but his passionate advocacy for a more tolerant, diverse society, where difference is respected and cultivated, reveals a very human side to his science”.[33]
Baron-Cohen has faced criticism by some for his "empathizing-systemizing theory", which states that humans may be classified on the basis of their scores along two dimensions (empathizing and systemizing); and that females tend to score higher on the empathizing dimension and males tend to score higher on the systemizing dimension. Feminist scientists, including Cordelia Fine, neuroscientist Gina Rippon, and Lise Eliot have opposed his extreme male brain theory of autism, calling it "neurotrash" and neurosexism.[34][35][36][37] Rippon also argues against using "male" and "female" for describing different types of brains, and that brain types do not correspond to genders.[35][38] Baron-Cohen has defended the neuroscience of sex differences against charges of neurosexism, arguing that "Fine's neurosexism allegation is the mistaken blurring of science with politics", adding that "You can be a scientist interested in the nature of sex differences while being a clear supporter of equal opportunities and a firm opponent of all forms of discrimination in society."[39]
Baron-Cohen and his book The Science of Evil were described by The New York Times “an award-winning psychologist” who had “unveiled a simple but persuasive hypothesis for a new way to think about evil.”[40]
A book review published in Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences characterized The Essential Difference as "very disappointing" with a "superficial notion of intelligence", concluding that Baron-Cohen's major claims about mindblindness and systemizing–empathizing are "at best, dubious".[41] According to Time (magazine) magazine, his views on systemizing traits had "earned him the ire of some parents of autistic children, who complain that he underestimates their families' suffering".[9] Time said that while research from Washington University in St. Louis did not support the assortative mating theory, a survey finding that autism was twice as high in Eindhoven had "breathed new life" into Baron-Cohen's theory.[9] Time magazine has also criticized the assortative mating theory proposed by Baron-Cohen, claiming that it is largely speculative and based on anecdotal evidence. The theory claims that autism rates are increasing because "systemizers", individuals with more autistic traits, are more likely to marry each other and are more likely to have autistic offspring due to relatively recent societal changes.[42] James McGrath has criticized the autism-spectrum quotient, writing that the score increases if one indicates interest in mathematics, and decreases if one indicates interest in literature or art. He claims that this leads to the false notion that most autistic people are strong in math.[43]
The developers of the software Baron-Cohen used for a 2009 study which reported that autistic individuals possessed superior visual acuity said that his results were impossible based on the technology used in the study. Baron-Cohen's team responded to this criticism by re-running the study and retracting the claim.[44][45][46]
Baron-Cohen's supposition that Isaac Newton and Albert Einstein displayed autistic traits has been met with scepticism by UCSF psychiatrist Glenn Elliot, who writes that attempting to diagnose on the basis of biographical information is extremely unreliable, and that any behavior can have various causes.[47]
Critics also say that because his work has focused on higher-functioning individuals with autism spectrum disorders, it requires independent replication with broader samples[48] and that his theories are based on subjective perceptions.[38] In response to some of these criticisms, Baron-Cohen agrees that many of his results have not been replicated, and says that he remains "open minded about these hypotheses until there are sufficient data to evaluate them". Still, he says he does not see a problem with introducing theories before definitive evidence has been collected.[48]
Furthermore, the mindblindness hypothesis, in addition to being questioned shortly after its publication,[49] has faced criticism from the scientific community,[50][51][52] in response to the many failed replications of classic ToM studies and the growing body of evidence for the heterogeneity of autism.[53] The theory of the double empathy problem and subsequent findings challenge the notion that autistic people exhibit inherent deficits in empathy and argued that difficulties of autistic people in social interactions with non-autistic people are due to differences in social-cognition and experiences between autistic people and non-autistic people, leading to two-way misunderstanding,[54] some aspects of which have been positively acknowledged by Baron-Cohen.[55]
Baron-Cohen's theories about mindblindness are also questioned by academic philosophers, particularly Autistic academic philosophers, in part on the basis that non-Autistic people are as blind to the internal states of Autistic people as Autistic people are to those of non-Autistics. [56] There is also criticism of Baron-Cohen's application of Theory of Mind to Autism on the grounds that he is not behaving scientifically, and that he is doing real harm to Autistic people.[57] Some also say that Baron-Cohen's work is easy to interpret as classifying Autistic people as not fully human.[57][58]
Organisations
Baron-Cohen is professor of developmental psychopathology at the University of Cambridge in the United Kingdom.[5] He is the director of the university's Autism Research Centre[59] and a Fellow of Trinity College.[5]
He is a Fellow of the British Psychological Society (BPS),[60] the British Academy,[61] the Academy of Medical Sciences, and the Association for Psychological Science.[62] He is a BPS Chartered Psychologist[60] and a Senior Investigator at the National Institute for Health and Care Research (NIHR).[63]
He serves as vice-president of the National Autistic Society (UK),[64] and was the 2012 chairman of the National Institute for Health and Care Excellence (NICE) Guideline Development Group for adults with autism.[65] He has served as vice-president and president of the International Society for Autism Research (INSAR).[5] He is co-editor in chief of the journal Molecular Autism.[66][67]
He was the chair of the Psychology Section of the British Academy.[68]
Recognition
Baron-Cohen was awarded the 1990 Spearman Medal from the BPS,[69] the McAndless Award from the American Psychological Association,[70] the 1993 May Davidson Award for Clinical Psychology from the BPS,[71] and the 2006 presidents' Award from the BPS.[72]
Baron-Cohen's Mindreading and The Transporters special educational software were nominated for the British Academy of Film and Television Arts (BAFTA) awards in 2002 and 2007.[73][74]
Baron-Cohen received an honorary degree from Abertay University in 2012,[75] and was awarded the Kanner-Asperger Medal in 2013 by the Wissenschaftliche Gesellschaft Autismus-Spektrum as a Lifetime Achievement Award for his contributions to autism research.[76]
He was knighted in the 2021 New Year Honours for services to people with autism.[77]
Personal life
In 1987, Baron-Cohen married Bridget Lindley.[78] Together, they had three children.[9]
He has an elder brother Dan Baron Cohen and three younger siblings, brother Ash Baron-Cohen and sisters Suzie and Liz.[4] His cousins include actor and comedian Sacha Baron Cohen and composer Erran Baron Cohen.[2][79][80]
Selected publications
Single-authored books
- Mindblindness: An Essay on Autism and Theory of Mind. MIT Press/Bradford Books. 1995. ISBN 978-0-262-02384-9.
- The Essential Difference: Men, Women and the Extreme Male Brain. Penguin/Basic Books. 2003. ISBN 978-0-7139-9671-5.
- Autism and Asperger Syndrome. Facts. Oxford University Press. 2008. ISBN 978-0-19-850490-0.
- Zero Degrees of Empathy: A New Theory of Human Cruelty. Penguin/Allen Lane. 2011. ISBN 978-0-7139-9791-0. (published in the US as The Science of Evil: On Empathy and the Origins of Human Cruelty, ISBN:978-0-465-02353-0)
- The Pattern Seekers: A New Theory of Human Invention. Allen Lane. 2020. ISBN 978-0241242186. (published in the US as The Pattern Seekers: How Autism Drives Human Invention. Basic Books. 2020. ISBN 978-1541647145.)
Other books
- Understanding Other Minds: Perspectives From Social Cognitive Neuroscience (3rd ed.). Oxford University Press. 2013. ISBN 978-0-19-852446-5.
- Teaching Children with Autism to Mindread: A Practical Guide for Teachers and Parents. Wiley. 2008. ISBN 978-0-471-97623-3.
- Prenatal Testosterone in Mind: Amniotic Fluid Studies. MIT Press/Bradford Books. 2005. ISBN 978-0-262-26774-8.
- An Exact Mind: An Artist with Asperger Syndrome. Jessica Kingsley. 2004. ISBN 978-1-84310-032-4.
- Understanding Other Minds: Perspectives from Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience (2nd ed.). Oxford University Press. 2000. ISBN 978-0-19-852445-8.
- Synaesthesia: Classic and Contemporary Readings. Blackwells. 1997. ISBN 978-0-631-19763-8.
- Baron-Cohen S, ed (1997). The Maladapted Mind: Classic Readings in Evolutionary Psychopathology. East Sussex, UK: Psychology Press/Taylor Francis Group. ISBN 978-0-86377-460-7.
Selected journal articles
- "Does the autistic child have a "theory of mind"?". Cognition 21 (1): 37–46. October 1985. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(85)90022-8. PMID 2934210.
- "Sex differences in the brain: implications for explaining autism". Science 310 (5749): 819–23. November 2005. doi:10.1126/science.1115455. PMID 16272115. Bibcode: 2005Sci...310..819B. http://irep.ntu.ac.uk/id/eprint/2710/1/219535_PubSub1971_Belmonte.pdf.
See also
- Childhood Autism Spectrum Test
- Sally–Anne test
- The NeuroGenderings Network § Sex differences in human neonatal social perception
- Spectrum 10K
References
- ↑ Salman, Saba (2 October 2019). "Simon Baron-Cohen: 'Neurodiversity is the next frontier. But we're failing autistic people'". The Guardian. https://www.theguardian.com/society/2019/oct/02/simon-baron-cohen-autism-neurodiversity-brains-money.
- ↑ 2.0 2.1 "The Provocative Baron Cohen Clan - Page 7 of 9" (in en-US). July–August 2010. https://www.momentmag.com/the-provocative-baron-cohen-clan/.
- ↑ "Simon Baron-Cohen: Ali G's smarter cousin and Britain's leading expert" (in en). 2009-05-23. http://www.independent.co.uk/news/people/profiles/simon-baron-cohen-ali-gs-smarter-cousin-and-britains-leading-expert-on-autism-1688427.html.
- ↑ 4.0 4.1 "My special sister Suzie". The Jewish Chronicle. https://www.thejc.com/simon-baron-cohen-my-special-sister-suzie-1.54020.
- ↑ 5.0 5.1 5.2 5.3 "ARC people: Professor Simon Baron-Cohen". Autism Research Center, University of Cambridge. http://www.autismresearchcentre.com/people_Baron-Cohen.
- ↑ "Forty years on: Uta Frith's contribution to research on autism and dyslexia, 1966-2006". Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 61 (1): 16–26. January 2008. doi:10.1080/17470210701508665. PMID 18038335.
- ↑ 7.0 7.1 "Testing the Empathizing-Systemizing theory of sex differences and the Extreme Male Brain theory of autism in half a million people". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 115 (48): 12152–12157. November 2018. doi:10.1073/pnas.1811032115. PMID 30420503. Bibcode: 2018PNAS..11512152G.
- ↑ 8.0 8.1 8.2 Baron-Cohen, Simon (9 November 2012). "Are geeky couples more likely to have kids with autism?". Scientific American. https://www.scientificamerican.com/article/are-geeky-couples-more-likely-to-have-kids-with-autism/. Pdf. Now in "4.4. Autism and the Technical Mind". Understanding Autism: The Search for Answers. Scientific American. 18 March 2013. ISBN 978-1-4668-3385-2. https://books.google.com/books?id=7gyxouMf3ZYC&q=%22Autism+and+the+Technical+Mind%22%22In+1997+my+colleague+Sally+Wheelwright+and+I+conducted+a+study+involving+nearly+2,000+families+in+the+U.K.+We+included+about+half+these+families+because+they+had+at+least+one+child%22&pg=PT73.
- ↑ 9.0 9.1 9.2 9.3 9.4 "Autism's lone wolf". Time (magazine). 29 August 2011. http://www.time.com/time/magazine/article/0,9171,2089358,00.html. Retrieved 28 December 2013.(Subscription content?)
- ↑ "Are autism spectrum conditions more prevalent in an information-technology region? A school-based study of three regions in the Netherlands". Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 42 (5): 734–9. May 2012. doi:10.1007/s10803-011-1302-1. PMID 21681590. http://oro.open.ac.uk/28986/5/Roelfsema_et_al__ASC_prevalence_NL_JADD_in_press.pdf.
- ↑ "Screening adults for Asperger Syndrome using the AQ: a preliminary study of its diagnostic validity in clinical practice". Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 35 (3): 331–5. June 2005. doi:10.1007/s10803-005-3300-7. PMID 16119474.
- ↑ "Sex and STEM Occupation Predict Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ) Scores in Half a Million People". PLOS ONE 10 (10): e0141229. 2015. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0141229. PMID 26488477. Bibcode: 2015PLoSO..1041229R.
- ↑ 13.0 13.1 "Foetal oestrogens and autism". Molecular Psychiatry 25 (11): 2970–2978. November 2020. doi:10.1038/s41380-019-0454-9. PMID 31358906.
- ↑ 14.0 14.1 "Elevated fetal steroidogenic activity in autism". Molecular Psychiatry 20 (3): 369–76. March 2015. doi:10.1038/mp.2014.48. PMID 24888361.
- ↑ "Sex differences in the brain: implications for explaining autism.". Science 310 (5749): 819–23. November 2005. doi:10.1126/science.1115455. PMID 16272115. Bibcode: 2005Sci...310..819B. http://irep.ntu.ac.uk/id/eprint/2710/1/219535_PubSub1971_Belmonte.pdf.
- ↑ "Why are autism spectrum conditions more prevalent in males?". PLOS Biology 9 (6): e1001081. June 2011. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.1001081. PMID 21695109.
- ↑ "Fetal testosterone influences sexually dimorphic gray matter in the human brain". The Journal of Neuroscience 32 (2): 674–80. January 2012. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4389-11.2012. PMID 22238103.
- ↑ "Sex-specific serum biomarker patterns in adults with Asperger's syndrome". Molecular Psychiatry 16 (12): 1213–20. December 2011. doi:10.1038/mp.2010.102. PMID 20877284.
- ↑ "Biological sex affects the neurobiology of autism". Brain: A Journal of Neurology 136 (Pt 9): 2799–815. September 2013. doi:10.1093/brain/awt216. PMID 23935125.
- ↑ "Polycystic ovary syndrome and autism: A test of the prenatal sex steroid theory". Translational Psychiatry 8 (1): 136. August 2018. doi:10.1038/s41398-018-0186-7. PMID 30065244.
- ↑ "Associations of Maternal Androgen-Related Conditions With Risk of Autism Spectrum Disorder in Progeny and Mediation by Cardiovascular, Metabolic, and Fertility Factors". American Journal of Epidemiology 190 (4): 600–610. April 2021. doi:10.1093/aje/kwaa219. PMID 33521821.
- ↑ "Maternal polycystic ovary syndrome and the risk of autism spectrum disorders in the offspring: a population-based nationwide study in Sweden". Molecular Psychiatry 21 (10): 1441–8. October 2016. doi:10.1038/mp.2015.183. PMID 26643539.
- ↑ "1985 paper on the theory of mind". SFARI. 9 May 2008. https://sfari.org/news-and-opinion/classic-paper-reviews/2008/1985-paper-on-the-theory-of-mind-commentary-by-rebecca-saxe.
- ↑ 24.0 24.1 "Does the autistic child have a "theory of mind"?". Cognition 21 (1): 37–46. October 1985. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(85)90022-8. PMID 2934210.
- ↑ "The "Reading the Mind in the Eyes" Test revised version: a study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism". Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines 42 (2): 241–51. February 2001. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00715. PMID 11280420.
- ↑ "Frontal lobe contributions to theory of mind". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 10 (5): 640–56. September 1998. doi:10.1162/089892998562942. PMID 9802997.
- ↑ "Social intelligence in the normal and autistic brain: an fMRI study". The European Journal of Neuroscience 11 (6): 1891–8. June 1999. doi:10.1046/j.1460-9568.1999.00621.x. PMID 10336657.
- ↑ "Genome-wide meta-analysis of cognitive empathy: heritability, and correlates with sex, neuropsychiatric conditions and cognition". Molecular Psychiatry 23 (6): 1402–1409. June 2018. doi:10.1038/mp.2017.122. PMID 28584286.
- ↑ "Social and non-social autism symptoms and trait domains are genetically dissociable". Communications Biology 2 (1): 328. 2019-09-03. doi:10.1038/s42003-019-0558-4. PMID 31508503.
- ↑ "Mind Reading: Frequently Asked Questions: Who developed it?". Jessica Kingsley Publishers. http://www.jkp.com/mindreading/faq.php#_3.
- ↑ "BAFTA Awards: Children's: Learning – Primary in 2007". British Academy of Film and Television Arts. http://awards.bafta.org/award/2007/childrens/learning-primary.
- ↑ Askham, Angie (8 April 2022). "'Theory of mind' in autism: A research field reborn". Spectrum. https://www.spectrumnews.org/features/deep-dive/theory-of-mind-in-autism-a-research-field-reborn/.
- ↑ Morgan, Jules (November 1, 2015). "Simon Baron-Cohen: cultivating diversity". The Lancet. https://www.thelancet.com/journals/lanpsy/article/PIIS2215-0366(15)00461-7/fulltext.
- ↑ (in en) The Gendered Brain: The new neuroscience that shatters the myth of the female brain. Random House. 2019-02-28. ISBN 978-1-4735-4897-8. https://books.google.com/books?id=rlBfDwAAQBAJ&q=The+Gendered+Brain:+The+new+neuroscience+that+shatters+the+myth+of+the+female+brain&pg=PT7.
- ↑ 35.0 35.1 "The Gendered Brain by Gina Rippon review – exposing a myth" (in en-GB). The Guardian. 2019-03-02. ISSN 0261-3077. https://www.theguardian.com/books/2019/mar/02/the-gendered-brain-by-gina-rippon-review.
- ↑ (in en) Fighting The Neurotrash, https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=2RWvDTKbFHg, retrieved 2020-01-02
- ↑ "Neurosexism: the myth that men and women have different brains" (in en). Nature 566 (7745): 453–454. 2019-02-27. doi:10.1038/d41586-019-00677-x. Bibcode: 2019Natur.566..453E.
- ↑ 38.0 38.1 "Simon Baron-Cohen: Theorizing on the mind in autism". 9 May 2011. https://www.spectrumnews.org/news/profiles/simon-baron-cohen-theorizing-on-the-mind-in-autism/.
- ↑ Baron-Cohen, Simon. Delusions of gender - 'neurosexism', biology and politics. https://www.academia.edu/31446128.
- ↑ Bouton, Katherine (13 June 2011). "From Hitler to Mother Teresa: 6 Degrees of Empathy". The New York Times. https://www.nytimes.com/2011/06/14/science/14scibks.html.
- ↑ "Book review: Understanding blindness". Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 3 (3): 315–324. September 2004. doi:10.1023/B:PHEN.0000049328.20506.a1. https://philarchive.org/rec/LEVBRU.
- ↑ "Could the Way We Mate and Marry Boost Rates of Autism?". Time. http://healthland.time.com/2011/08/19/could-the-way-we-mate-and-marry-boost-rates-of-autism/.
- ↑ "Not all autistic people are good at maths and science – despite the stereotypes" (in en). 3 April 2019. https://theconversation.com/not-all-autistic-people-are-good-at-maths-and-science-despite-the-stereotypes-114128.
- ↑ "Regarding "Eagle-eyed visual acuity: an experimental investigation of enhanced perception in autism"". Biological Psychiatry 66 (10): e19-20; author reply e23-4. November 2009. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.02.035. PMID 19576570.
- ↑ "A close eye on the eagle-eyed visual acuity hypothesis of autism". Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 42 (5): 726–33. May 2012. doi:10.1007/s10803-011-1300-3. PMID 21660498.
- ↑ "Psychophysical measures of visual acuity in autism spectrum conditions". Vision Research 51 (15): 1778–80. August 2011. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2011.06.004. PMID 21704058. Discussed in "Eagle-Eyed Autism? No.". Neuroskeptic. 29 June 2011. http://blogs.discovermagazine.com/neuroskeptic/2011/06/29/eagle-eyed-autism-no/#.W7eFJfYpDIV.
- ↑ "Einstein and Newton showed signs of autism". New Scientist. 30 April 2003. https://www.newscientist.com/article/dn3676-einstein-and-newton-showed-signs-of-autism/.
- ↑ 48.0 48.1 "Scientists and autism: When geeks meet". Nature 479 (7371): 25–7. November 2011. doi:10.1038/479025a. PMID 22051657. Bibcode: 2011Natur.479...25B.
- ↑ "The Roots of Mindblindness" (in en). Theory & Psychology 14 (5): 685–703. 2004-10-01. doi:10.1177/0959354304046179. ISSN 0959-3543. http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1177/0959354304046179.
- ↑ "Empirical Failures of the Claim That Autistic People Lack a Theory of Mind" (in en). Archives of Scientific Psychology 7 (1): 102–118. 2019. doi:10.1037/arc0000067. PMID 31938672.
- ↑ "Individual differences in autistic children's homograph reading: Evidence from Hebrew" (in en). Autism & Developmental Language Impairments 2: 239694151771494. January 2017. doi:10.1177/2396941517714945. ISSN 2396-9415.
- ↑ "Autism spectrum heterogeneity: fact or artifact?" (in en). Molecular Psychiatry 25 (12): 3178–3185. 2020-04-30. doi:10.1038/s41380-020-0748-y. ISSN 1476-5578. PMID 32355335.
- ↑ "Etiological heterogeneity in autism spectrum disorders: More than 100 genetic and genomic disorders and still counting" (in en). Brain Research. The Emerging Neuroscience of Autism Spectrum Disorders 1380: 42–77. 2011-03-22. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2010.11.078. ISSN 0006-8993. PMID 21129364. https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0006899310025916.
- ↑ * "On the ontological status of autism: the 'double empathy problem'" (in en). Disability & Society 27 (6): 883–887. October 2012. doi:10.1080/09687599.2012.710008. ISSN 0968-7599. http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/09687599.2012.710008.
* "What do New Findings About Social Interaction in Autistic Adults Mean for Neurodevelopmental Research?". Perspectives on Psychological Science 16 (3): 649–653. 2021. doi:10.1177/1745691620958010. PMID 33560175.
* "Autism and the double empathy problem: Implications for development and mental health". British Journal of Developmental Psychology 39 (1): 1–18. 2021. doi:10.1111/bjdp.12350. PMID 33393101.
* "Mutual (Mis)understanding: Reframing Autistic Pragmatic "Impairments" Using Relevance Theory". Frontiers in Psychology 12: 616664. 2021. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2021.616664. PMID 33995177. - ↑ * "A Conversation About 'The Pattern Seekers' by Simon Baron-Cohen". 19 December 2020. https://thinkingautismguide.com/2020/12/a-conversation-about-pattern-seekers-by.html.
* "Evidence of partner similarity for autistic traits, systemizing, and theory of mind via facial expressions". Scientific Reports 12 (1): 8451. 2022-05-19. doi:10.1038/s41598-022-11592-z. PMID 35589769. PMC 9118825. Bibcode: 2022NatSR..12.8451R. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-11592-z.
* "Risk markers for suicidality in autistic adults" (in en). Molecular Autism 9 (1): 42. 31 July 2018. doi:10.1186/s13229-018-0226-4. ISSN 2040-2392. PMID 30083306. - ↑ Mcgeer, Victoria (2009). "The thought and talk of individuals with autism: Reflections on Ian Hacking". Metaphilosophy 40 (¾): 517–530. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9973.2009.01601.x. https://www.jstor.org/stable/24439799.
- ↑ 57.0 57.1 Yergeu and Huebner, Michele and Bryce (2017). "Minding Theory of Mind". Journal of Social Philosophy 48 (3): 273–296. doi:10.1111/josp.12191. https://philpapers.org/rec/YERMTO.
- ↑ Yergeau, Melanie (2013). "Clinically Significant Disturbance: On Theorists Who Theorize Theory of Mind". Disability Studies Quarterly 33 (4). doi:10.18061/dsq.v33i4.3876. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/297657345.
- ↑ "ARC researchers, collaborators and staff". Autism Research Center, University of Cambridge. http://www.autismresearchcentre.com/arc_people.
- ↑ 60.0 60.1 "Chartered Psychologist emphasises the importance of empathy". British Psychological Society. 28 April 2011. http://www.bps.org.uk/news/chartered-psychologist-emphasises-importance-empathy.
- ↑ "Seven Cambridge academics elected as Fellows of The British Academy". University of Cambridge. 17 July 2009. http://www.cam.ac.uk/news/seven-cambridge-academics-elected-as-fellows-of-the-british-academy.
- ↑ "Reflecting on a lifetime of achievement: Uta Frith". Aps Observer 26 (8). 30 September 2013. http://www.psychologicalscience.org/index.php/publications/observer/2013/october-13/reflecting-on-a-lifetime-of-achievement-5.html. Retrieved 28 December 2013.
- ↑ "Professor Sir Simon Baron-Cohen wins Senior Investigator award" (in en-GB). 2021-03-01. https://www.psychiatry.cam.ac.uk/blog/2021/03/01/professor-sir-simon-baron-cohen-wins-senior-investigator-award/.
- ↑ "Vice presidents". National Autistic Society. http://www.autism.org.uk/news-and-events/about-the-nas/who-we-are/structure/vice-presidents.aspx.
- ↑ "Autism: recognition, referral, diagnosis and management of adults on the autism spectrum". National Institute for Health and Care Excellence. http://publications.nice.org.uk/autism-recognition-referral-diagnosis-and-management-of-adults-on-the-autism-spectrum-cg142/appendix-a-the-guideline-development-group-national-collaborating-centre-and-nice-project-team.
- ↑ "Molecular Autism. Editorial Board". Molecular Autism (BioMed Central Ltd). https://molecularautism.biomedcentral.com/about/editorial-board. Retrieved 16 November 2018.
- ↑ "INSAR Board Elections 2016 - President-Elect". International Society for Autism Research. http://www.autism-insar.org/about/board-nominees.
- ↑ "Professor Simon Baron-Cohen FBA". British Academy. https://www.thebritishacademy.ac.uk/fellows/simon-baron-cohen-FBA.
- ↑ "Spearman medal". British Psychological Society: History of Psychology Centre. https://webcache.googleusercontent.com/search?q=cache:https://www.bps.org.uk/about-us/awards-and-grants/research-board-awards/spearman-medal+1990+%22Simon%20Baron-Cohen%22.
- ↑ "Boyd McCandless Award: Past recipients: 1990". American Psychological Association. http://www.apa.org/about/awards/div-7-mccandless.aspx.
- ↑ "Previous winners: May Davidson Award". British Psychological Society. http://www.bps.org.uk/networks-and-communities/member-networks/division-clinical-psychology/previous-winners.
- ↑ "Presidents' Award for distinguished contributions to psychological knowledge". British Psychological Society: History of Psychology Centre. https://webcache.googleusercontent.com/search?q=cache:https://www.bps.org.uk/about-us/awards-and-grants/research-board-awards/presidents-award-distinguished-contributions+2006+%22Simon%20Baron-Cohen%22.
- ↑ "BAFTA Awards: Interactive: Offline Learning in 2002". British Academy of Film and Television Arts. http://awards.bafta.org/award/2002/interactive/offline-learning.
- ↑ "2007 Children's Learning - Primary". awards.bafta.org. http://awards.bafta.org/award/2007/childrens/learning-primary.
- ↑ "More than 850 students to tread graduation boards". https://www.pressreader.com/uk/the-courier-advertiser-perth-and-perthshire-edition/20120705/282681864357661.
- ↑ "Awardees". Wissenschaftliche Gesellschaft Autismus-Spektrum (WGAS). http://www.wgas-autismus.org/index.php?option=com_content&view=article&id=81&Itemid=103&lang=en.
- ↑ No. 63218. 31 December 2020. p. N2. https://www.thegazette.co.uk/London/issue/63218/supplement/N2
- ↑ "Obituary: Bridget Lindley". The Times. 22 April 2016. https://www.thetimes.co.uk/article/bridget-lindley-d8h3s05mz.
- ↑ "Time Out with Nick Cohen". New Statesman. 26 February 2007. http://www.newstatesman.com/life-and-society/2007/02/baron-cohen-autism-children.
- ↑ "Q&A: Psychologist Simon Baron-Cohen on empathy and the science of evil". Time. 30 May 2011. http://healthland.time.com/2011/05/30/mind-reading-psychologist-simon-baron-cohen-on-empathy-and-the-science-of-evil/. Retrieved 2 January 2014.
External links
- Profile – Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge
- "They just can't help it", Simon Baron-Cohen, The Guardian (17 April 2003)
- "The Male Condition", Simon Baron-Cohen, The New York Times Op-Ed Section (8 August 2005)
- "The Assortative Mating Theory: A Talk with Simon Baron-Cohen", Edge Foundation discussion, 2005
- "The Short Life of a Diagnosis", Simon Baron-Cohen The New York Times Op-Ed Section (9 November 2009)
- "Why a lack of empathy is the root of all evil", Clint Witchalls, The Independent (5 April 2011)
- The Science of Evil: On Empathy and the Origins of Cruelty, Simon Baron-Cohen (The Montréal Review, October 2011)
Original source: https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Simon Baron-Cohen.
Read more |