Biology:Genetic correlation

From HandWiki

In multivariate quantitative genetics, a genetic correlation (denoted [math]\displaystyle{ r_g }[/math] or [math]\displaystyle{ r_a }[/math]) is the proportion of variance that two traits share due to genetic causes,[1][2][3] the correlation between the genetic influences on a trait and the genetic influences on a different trait[4][5][6][7][8][9] estimating the degree of pleiotropy or causal overlap. A genetic correlation of 0 implies that the genetic effects on one trait are independent of the other, while a correlation of 1 implies that all of the genetic influences on the two traits are identical. The bivariate genetic correlation can be generalized to inferring genetic latent variable factors across > 2 traits using factor analysis. Genetic correlation models were introduced into behavioral genetics in the 1970s–1980s. Genetic correlations have applications in validation of genome-wide association study (GWAS) results, breeding, prediction of traits, and discovering the etiology of traits & diseases.

They can be estimated using individual-level data from twin studies and molecular genetics, or even with GWAS summary statistics.[10][11] Genetic correlations have been found to be common in non-human genetics[12] and to be broadly similar to their respective phenotypic correlations,[13] and also found extensively in human traits, dubbed the 'phenome'.[14][15][16][17][18][19][20][21][22][23][24]

This finding of widespread pleiotropy has implications for artificial selection in agriculture, interpretation of phenotypic correlations, social inequality,[25] attempts to use Mendelian randomization in causal inference,[26][27][28][29] the understanding of the biological origins of complex traits, and the design of GWASes.

A genetic correlation is to be contrasted with environmental correlation between the environments affecting two traits (e.g. if poor nutrition in a household caused both lower IQ and height); a genetic correlation between two traits can contribute to the observed (phenotypic) correlation between two traits, but genetic correlations can also be opposite observed phenotypic correlations if the environment correlation is sufficiently strong in the other direction, perhaps due to tradeoffs or specialization.[30][31] The observation that genetic correlations usually mirror phenotypic correlations is known as "Cheverud's Conjecture"[32] and has been confirmed in animals[33][34] and humans, and showed they are of similar sizes;[35] for example, in the UK Biobank, of 118 continuous human traits, only 29% of their intercorrelations have opposite signs,[23] and a later analysis of 17 high-quality UKBB traits reported correlation near-unity.[36]

Interpretation

Genetic correlations are not the same as heritability, as it is about the overlap between the two sets of influences and not their absolute magnitude; two traits could be both highly heritable but not be genetically correlated or have small heritabilities and be completely correlated (as long as the heritabilities are non-zero).

For example, consider two traits – dark skin and black hair. These two traits may individually have a very high heritability (most of the population-level variation in the trait due to genetic differences, or in simpler terms, genetics contributes significantly to these two traits), however, they may still have a very low genetic correlation if, for instance, these two traits were being controlled by different, non-overlapping, non-linked genetic loci.

A genetic correlation between two traits will tend to produce phenotypic correlations – e.g. the genetic correlation between intelligence and SES[16] or education and family SES[37] implies that intelligence/SES will also correlate phenotypically. The phenotypic correlation will be limited by the degree of genetic correlation and also by the heritability of each trait. The expected phenotypic correlation is the bivariate heritability' and can be calculated as the square roots of the heritabilities multiplied by the genetic correlation. (Using a Plomin example,[38] for two traits with heritabilities of 0.60 & 0.23, [math]\displaystyle{ r_g=0.75 }[/math], and phenotypic correlation of r=0.45 the bivariate heritability would be [math]\displaystyle{ \sqrt{0.60} \cdot 0.75 \cdot \sqrt{0.23} = 0.28 }[/math], so of the observed phenotypic correlation, 0.28/0.45 = 62% of it is due to genetics.)

Cause

Genetic correlations can arise due to:[19]

  1. linkage disequilibrium (two neighboring genes tend to be inherited together, each affecting a different trait)
  2. biological pleiotropy (a single gene having multiple otherwise unrelated biological effects, or shared regulation of multiple genes[39])
  3. mediated pleiotropy (a gene causes trait X and trait X causes trait Y).
  4. biases: population stratification such as ancestry or assortative mating (sometimes called "gametic phase disequilibrium"), spurious stratification such as ascertainment bias/self-selection[40] or Berkson's paradox, or misclassification of diagnoses

Uses

Causes of changes in traits

Genetic correlations are scientifically useful because genetic correlations can be analyzed over time within an individual longitudinally[41] (e.g. intelligence is stable over a lifetime, due to the same genetic influences – childhood genetically correlates [math]\displaystyle{ r_g=0.62 }[/math] with old age[42]), or across studies or populations or ethnic groups/races [citation needed], or across diagnoses, allowing discovery of whether different genes influence a trait over a lifetime (typically, they do not[4]), whether different genes influence a trait in different populations due to differing local environments, whether there is disease heterogeneity across times or places or sex (particularly in psychiatric diagnoses there is uncertainty whether 1 country's 'autism' or 'schizophrenia' is the same as another's or whether diagnostic categories have shifted over time/place leading to different levels of ascertainment bias), and to what degree traits like autoimmune or psychiatric disorders or cognitive functioning meaningfully cluster due sharing a biological basis and genetic architecture (for example, reading & mathematics disability genetically correlate, consistent with the Generalist Genes Hypothesis, and these genetic correlations explain the observed phenotypic correlations or 'co-morbidity';[43] IQ and specific measures of cognitive performance such as verbal, spatial, and memory tasks, reaction time, long-term memory, executive function etc. all show high genetic correlations as do neuroanatomical measurements [citation needed], and the correlations may increase with age, with implications for the etiology & nature of intelligence)[citation needed]. This can be an important constraint on conceptualizations of the two traits: traits which seem different phenotypically but which share a common genetic basis require an explanation for how these genes can influence both traits.

Boosting GWASes

Genetic correlations can be used in GWASes by using polygenic scores or genome-wide hits for one (often more easily measured) trait to increase the prior probability of variants for a second trait; for example, since intelligence and years of education are highly genetically correlated, a GWAS for education will inherently also be a GWAS for intelligence and be able to predict variance in intelligence as well[44] and the strongest SNP candidates can be used to increase the statistical power of a smaller GWAS,[45] a combined analysis on the latent trait done where each measured genetically-correlated trait helps reduce measurement error and boosts the GWAS's power considerably (e.g. Krapohl et al. 2017, using elastic net and multiple polygenic scores, improving intelligence prediction from 3.6% of variance to 4.8%;[46] Hill et al. 2017b[47] uses MTAG[48] to combine 3 g-loaded traits of education, household income, and a cognitive test score to find 107 hits & doubles predictive power of intelligence) or one could do a GWAS for multiple traits jointly.[49][50]

Genetic correlations can also quantify the contribution of correlations <1 across datasets which might create a false "missing heritability", by estimating the extent to which differing measurement methods, ancestral influences, or environments create only partially overlapping sets of relevant genetic variants.[51]

Breeding

Hairless dogs have imperfect teeth; long-haired and coarse-haired animals are apt to have, as is asserted, long or many horns; pigeons with feathered feet have skin between their outer toes; pigeons with short beaks have small feet, and those with long beaks large feet. Hence if man goes on selecting, and thus augmenting any peculiarity, he will almost certainly modify unintentionally other parts of the structure, owing to the mysterious laws of correlation.

Genetic correlations are also useful in applied contexts such as plant/animal breeding by allowing substitution of more easily measured but highly genetically correlated characteristics (particularly in the case of sex-linked or binary traits under the liability-threshold model, where differences in the phenotype can rarely be observed but another highly correlated measure, perhaps an endophenotype, is available in all individuals), compensating for different environments than the breeding was carried out in, making more accurate predictions of breeding value using the multivariate breeder's equation as compared to predictions based on the univariate breeder's equation using only per-trait heritability & assuming independence of traits, and avoiding unexpected consequences by taking into consideration that artificial selection for/against trait X will also increase/decrease all traits which positively/negatively correlate with X.[52][53][54][55][56] The limits to selection set by the inter-correlation of traits, and the possibility for genetic correlations to change over long-term breeding programs, lead to Haldane's dilemma limiting the intensity of selection and thus progress.

Breeding experiments on genetically correlated traits can measure the extent to which correlated traits are inherently developmentally linked & response is constrained, and which can be dissociated.[57] Some traits, such as the size of eyespots on the butterfly Bicyclus anynana can be dissociated in breeding,[58] but other pairs, such as eyespot colors, have resisted efforts.[59]

Mathematical definition

Given a genetic covariance matrix, the genetic correlation is computed by standardizing this, i.e., by converting the covariance matrix to a correlation matrix. Generally, if [math]\displaystyle{ \Sigma }[/math] is a genetic covariance matrix and [math]\displaystyle{ D=\sqrt{\operatorname{diag}(\Sigma)} }[/math], then the correlation matrix is [math]\displaystyle{ D^{-1} \Sigma D^{-1} }[/math]. For a given genetic covariance [math]\displaystyle{ \operatorname{cov}_g }[/math] between two traits, one with genetic variance [math]\displaystyle{ V_{g1} }[/math] and the other with genetic variance [math]\displaystyle{ V_{g2} }[/math], the genetic correlation is computed in the same way as the correlation coefficient [math]\displaystyle{ r_g = \frac{\operatorname{cov}_g}{\sqrt{V_{g1}V_{g2}}} }[/math].

Computing the genetic correlation

Genetic correlations require a genetically informative sample. They can be estimated in breeding experiments on two traits of known heritability and selecting on one trait to measure the change in the other trait (allowing inferring the genetic correlation), family/adoption/twin studies (analyzed using SEMs or DeFries–Fulker extremes analysis), molecular estimation of relatedness such as GCTA,[60] methods employing polygenic scores like HDL (High-Definition Likelihood),[11] LD score regression,[17][61] BOLT-REML,[62] CPBayes,[63] or HESS,[64] comparison of genome-wide SNP hits in GWASes (as a loose lower bound), and phenotypic correlations of populations with at least some related individuals.[65]

As with estimating SNP heritability and genetic correlation, the better computational scaling & the ability to estimate using only established summary association statistics is a particular advantage for HDL[11] and LD score regression over competing methods. Combined with the increasing availability of GWAS summary statistics or polygenic scores from datasets like the UK Biobank, such summary-level methods have led to an explosion of genetic correlation research since 2015.[citation needed]

The methods are related to Haseman–Elston regression & PCGC regression.[66] Such methods are typically genome-wide, but it is also possible to estimate genetic correlations for specific variants or genome regions.[67]

One way to consider it is using trait X in twin 1 to predict trait Y in twin 2 for monozygotic and dizygotic twins (i.e. using twin 1's IQ to predict twin 2's brain volume); if this cross-correlation is larger for the more genetically-similar monozygotic twins than for the dizygotic twins, the similarity indicates that the traits are not genetically independent and there is some common genetics influencing both IQ and brain volume. (Statistical power can be boosted by using siblings as well.[68])

Genetic correlations are affected by methodological concerns; underestimation of heritability, such as due to assortative mating, will lead to overestimates of longitudinal genetic correlation,[69] and moderate levels of misdiagnoses can create pseudo correlations.[70]

As they are affected by heritabilities of both traits, genetic correlations have low statistical power, especially in the presence of measurement errors biasing heritability downwards, because "estimates of genetic correlations are usually subject to rather large sampling errors and therefore seldom very precise": the standard error of an estimate [math]\displaystyle{ r_g }[/math] is [math]\displaystyle{ \sigma(r_g) = \frac{1 - r_g^2}{\sqrt{2}} \cdot \sqrt{\frac{\sigma(h^2_x) \cdot \sigma(h^2_y)}{h^2_x \cdot h^2_y}} }[/math].[71] (Larger genetic correlations & heritabilities will be estimated more precisely.[72]) However, inclusion of genetic correlations in an analysis of a pleiotropic trait can boost power for the same reason that multivariate regressions are more powerful than separate univariate regressions.[73]

Twin methods have the advantage of being usable without detailed biological data, with human genetic correlations calculated as far back as the 1970s and animal/plant genetic correlations calculated in the 1930s, and require sample sizes in the hundreds for being well-powered, but they have the disadvantage of making assumptions which have been criticized, and in the case of rare traits like anorexia nervosa it may be difficult to find enough twins with a diagnosis to make meaningful cross-twin comparisons, and can only be estimated with access to the twin data; molecular genetic methods like GCTA or LD score regression have the advantage of not requiring specific degrees of relatedness and so can easily study rare traits using case-control designs, which also reduces the number of assumptions they rely on, but those methods could not be run until recently, require large sample sizes in the thousands or hundreds of thousands (to obtain precise SNP heritability estimates, see the standard error formula), may require individual-level genetic data (in the case of GCTA but not LD score regression).

More concretely, if two traits, say height and weight have the following additive genetic variance-covariance matrix:

Height Weight
Height 36 36
Weight 36 117

Then the genetic correlation is .55, as seen is the standardized matrix below:

Height Weight
Height 1
Weight .55 1

In practice, structural equation modeling applications such as Mx or OpenMx (and before that, historically, LISREL[74]) are used to calculate both the genetic covariance matrix and its standardized form. In R, cov2cor() will standardize the matrix.

Typically, published reports will provide genetic variance components that have been standardized as a proportion of total variance (for instance in an ACE twin study model standardised as a proportion of V-total = A+C+E). In this case, the metric for computing the genetic covariance (the variance within the genetic covariance matrix) is lost (because of the standardizing process), so you cannot readily estimate the genetic correlation of two traits from such published models. Multivariate models (such as the Cholesky decomposition[better source needed]) will, however, allow the viewer to see shared genetic effects (as opposed to the genetic correlation) by following path rules. It is important therefore to provide the unstandardised path coefficients in publications.

See also

References

  1. Falconer, Ch. 19
  2. Lynch, M. and Walsh, B. (1998) Genetics and Analysis of Quantitative Traits, Sinauer,Ch21, "Correlations Between Characters", "Ch25, Threshold Characters" ISBN:9780878934812
  3. Neale & Maes (1996), Methodology for genetics studies of twins and families (6th ed.). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer.
  4. 4.0 4.1 Plomin et al., p. 123
  5. Martin, N. G.; Eaves, L. J. (1977). "The genetical analysis of covariance structure". Heredity 38 (1): 79–95. doi:10.1038/hdy.1977.9. PMID 268313. https://www.genepi.qimr.edu.au/contents/p/staff/CV009.pdf. 
  6. Eaves, L. J.; Last, K. A.; Young, P. A.; Martin, N. G. (1978). "Model-fitting approaches to the analysis of human behaviour". Heredity 41 (3): 249–320. doi:10.1038/hdy.1978.101. PMID 370072. 
  7. Loehlin & Vandenberg (1968) "Genetic and environmental components in the covariation of cognitive abilities: An additive model", in Progress in Human Behaviour Genetics, ed. S. G. Vandenberg, pp. 261–278. Johns Hopkins, Baltimore.
  8. Purcell, S.; Sham, P. (2002). "Variance components models for gene-environment interaction in quantitative trait locus linkage analysis". Twin Research 5 (6): 572–6. doi:10.1375/136905202762342035. PMID 12573188. 
  9. Kohler, H. P.; Behrman, J. R.; Schnittker, J. (2011). "Social Science Methods for Twins Data: Integrating Causality, Endowments and Heritability". Biodemography and Social Biology 57 (1): 88–141. doi:10.1080/19485565.2011.580619. PMID 21845929. 
  10. Bulik-Sullivan, Brendan; Finucane, Hilary K.; Anttila, Verneri; Gusev, Alexander; Day, Felix R.; Loh, Po-Ru; Duncan, Laramie; Perry, John R. B. et al. (November 2015). "An atlas of genetic correlations across human diseases and traits". Nature Genetics 47 (11): 1236–1241. doi:10.1038/ng.3406. PMID 26414676. 
  11. 11.0 11.1 11.2 Ning, Zheng; Pawitan, Yudi; Shen, Xia (August 2020). "High-definition likelihood inference of genetic correlations across human complex traits" (in en). Nature Genetics 52 (8): 859–864. doi:10.1038/s41588-020-0653-y. PMID 32601477. https://www.pure.ed.ac.uk/ws/files/144684519/High_definition_likelihood_inference_of_genetic_correlations_across_human_complex_traits.pdf. 
  12. Wagner, G. P.; Zhang, J. (2011). "The pleiotropic structure of the genotype-phenotype map: The evolvability of complex organisms". Nature Reviews. Genetics 12 (3): 204–13. doi:10.1038/nrg2949. PMID 21331091. http://www.academia.edu/download/30788791/Wagner_2011_NRG_12_204.pdf. [|permanent dead link|dead link}}]
  13. Cheverud, James M. (1988). "A Comparison of Genetic and Phenotypic Correlations". Evolution 42 (5): 958–968. doi:10.2307/2408911. PMID 28581166. 
  14. Krapohl, E.; Euesden, J.; Zabaneh, D.; Pingault, J. B.; Rimfeld, K.; von Stumm, S.; Dale, P. S.; Breen, G. et al. (2016). "Phenome-wide analysis of genome-wide polygenic scores". Molecular Psychiatry 21 (9): 1188–93. doi:10.1038/mp.2015.126. PMID 26303664. PMC 4767701. http://www.hungrymindlab.com/wp-content/uploads/2015/10/Krapohl-et-al-2015.pdf. Retrieved 2016-10-24. 
  15. Hagenaars, S. P.; Harris, S. E.; Davies, G.; Hill, W. D.; Liewald, D C M.; Ritchie, S. J.; Marioni, R. E.; Fawns-Ritchie, C. et al. (2016). "Shared genetic aetiology between cognitive functions and physical and mental health in UK Biobank (N=112 151) and 24 GWAS consortia". Molecular Psychiatry 21 (11): 1624–1632. doi:10.1038/mp.2015.225. PMID 26809841. 
  16. 16.0 16.1 Hill, W. D.; Hagenaars, S. P.; Marioni, R. E.; Harris, S. E.; Liewald DCM; Davies, G.; Okbay, A.; McIntosh, A. M. et al. (2016). "Molecular genetic contributions to social deprivation and household income in UK Biobank (n=112,151)". Current Biology 26 (22): 3083–3089. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2016.09.035. PMID 27818178. 
  17. 17.0 17.1 Zheng, Jie; Erzurumluoglu, A. Mesut; Elsworth, Benjamin L.; Kemp, John P.; Howe, Laurence; Haycock, Philip C.; Hemani, Gibran; Tansey, Katherine et al. (2017). "LD Hub: A centralized database and web interface to perform LD score regression that maximizes the potential of summary level GWAS data for SNP heritability and genetic correlation analysis". Bioinformatics 33 (2): 272–279. doi:10.1093/bioinformatics/btw613. PMID 27663502. 
  18. Sivakumaran, Shanya; Agakov, Felix; Theodoratou, Evropi; Prendergast, James G.; Zgaga, Lina; Manolio, Teri; Rudan, Igor; McKeigue, Paul et al. (2011). "Abundant pleiotropy in human complex diseases and traits". The American Journal of Human Genetics 89 (5): 607–618. doi:10.1016/j.ajhg.2011.10.004. PMID 22077970. 
  19. 19.0 19.1 Solovieff, N.; Cotsapas, C.; Lee, P. H.; Purcell, S. M.; Smoller, J. W. (2013). "Pleiotropy in complex traits: challenges and strategies". Nature Reviews. Genetics 14 (7): 483–495. doi:10.1038/nrg3461. PMID 23752797. 
  20. Cotsapas, Chris; Voight, Benjamin F.; Rossin, Elizabeth; Lage, Kasper; Neale, Benjamin M.; Wallace, Chris; Abecasis, Gonçalo R.; Barrett, Jeffrey C. et al. (2011). "Pervasive sharing of genetic effects in autoimmune disease". PLOS Genetics 7 (8): e1002254. doi:10.1371/journal.pgen.1002254. PMID 21852963. 
  21. Chambers, J. C.; Zhang, W.; Sehmi, J.; Li, X.; Wass, M. N.; Van Der Harst, P.; Holm, H.; Sanna, S. et al. (2011). "Genome-wide association study identifies loci influencing concentrations of liver enzymes in plasma". Nature Genetics 43 (11): 1131–1138. doi:10.1038/ng.970. PMID 22001757. 
  22. Hemani, Gibran; Bowden, Jack; Haycock, Philip; Zheng, Jie; Davis, Oliver; Flach, Peter; Gaunt, Tom; Smith, George Davey (2017). Automating Mendelian randomization through machine learning to construct a putative causal map of the human phenome. doi:10.1101/173682. 
  23. 23.0 23.1 Canela-Xandri, Oriol; Rawlik, Konrad; Tenesa, Albert (2018). "An atlas of genetic associations in UK Biobank". Nature Genetics 50 (11): 1593–1599. doi:10.1038/s41588-018-0248-z. PMID 30349118. 
  24. Socrates, Adam; Bond, Tom; Karhunen, Ville; Auvinen, Juha; Rietveld, Cornelius A.; Veijola, Juha; Jarvelin, Marjo-Riitta; o'Reilly, Paul F. (2017). Polygenic risk scores applied to a single cohort reveal pleiotropy among hundreds of human phenotypes. doi:10.1101/203257. 
  25. Mõttus, René; Marioni, Riccardo; Deary, Ian J. (2017). "Markers of Psychological Differences and Social and Health Inequalities: Possible Genetic and Phenotypic Overlaps". Journal of Personality 85 (1): 104–117. doi:10.1111/jopy.12220. PMID 26292196. 
  26. Burgess, S.; Butterworth, A. S.; Thompson, J. R. (2016). "Beyond Mendelian randomization: how to interpret evidence of shared genetic predictors". Journal of Clinical Epidemiology 69: 208–216. doi:10.1016/j.jclinepi.2015.08.001. PMID 26291580. 
  27. Hagenaars, Saskia P.; Gale, Catharine R.; Deary, Ian J.; Harris, Sarah E. (2017). "Cognitive ability and physical health: A Mendelian randomization study". Scientific Reports 7 (1): 2651. doi:10.1038/s41598-017-02837-3. PMID 28572633. Bibcode2017NatSR...7.2651H. 
  28. Bowden, J.; Davey Smith, G.; Burgess, S. (2015). "Mendelian randomization with invalid instruments: Effect estimation and bias detection through Egger regression". International Journal of Epidemiology 44 (2): 512–525. doi:10.1093/ije/dyv080. PMID 26050253. 
  29. Verbanck, Marie; Chen, Chia-Yen; Neale, Benjamin; Do, Ron (2018). "Detection of widespread horizontal pleiotropy in causal relationships inferred from Mendelian randomization between complex traits and diseases". Nature Genetics 50 (5): 693–698. doi:10.1038/s41588-018-0099-7. PMID 29686387. 
  30. Falconer, p. 315 cites the example of chicken size and egg laying: chickens grown large for genetic reasons lay later, fewer, and larger eggs, while chickens grown large for environmental reasons lay quicker and more but normal sized eggs; Table 19.1 on p. 316 also provides examples of opposite-signed phenotypic & genetic correlations: fleece-weight/length-of-wool & fleece weight/body-weight in sheep, and body-weight/egg-timing & body-weight/egg-production in chicken. One consequence of the negative chicken correlations was that, despite moderate heritabilities and a positive phenotypic correlation, selection had begun to fail to yield any improvements (p. 329) according to "Genetic slippage in response to selection for multiple objectives", Dickerson 1955.
  31. Kruuk, Loeske E. B.; Slate, Jon; Pemberton, Josephine M.; Brotherstone, Sue; Guinness, Fiona; Clutton-Brock, Tim (2002). "Antler Size in Red Deer: Heritability and Selection but No Evolution". Evolution 56 (8): 1683–95. doi:10.1111/j.0014-3820.2002.tb01480.x. PMID 12353761. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/11103156. 
  32. Cheverud, James M. (1988). "A comparison of genetic and phenotypic correlations". Evolution 42 (5): 958–968. doi:10.1111/j.1558-5646.1988.tb02514.x. PMID 28581166. 
  33. Roff, Derek A. (1995). "The estimation of genetic correlations from phenotypic correlations – a test of Cheverud's conjecture". Heredity 74 (5): 481–490. doi:10.1038/hdy.1995.68. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/232778143. 
  34. Kruuk, Loeske E.B.; Slate, Jon; Wilson, Alastair J. (2008). "New answers for old questions: The evolutionary quantitative genetics of wild animal populations". Annual Review of Ecology, Evolution, and Systematics 39: 525–548. doi:10.1146/annurev.ecolsys.39.110707.173542. https://pdfs.semanticscholar.org/3dd4/842bf2bbf55e3065dc4073e0ad906085dfec.pdf. 
  35. Dochtermann, Ned A. (2011). "Testing Cheverud's conjecture for behavioral correlations and behavioral syndromes". Evolution 65 (6): 1814–1820. doi:10.1111/j.1558-5646.2011.01264.x. PMID 21644966. 
  36. Sodini, Sebastian M.; Kemper, Kathryn E.; Wray, Naomi R.; Trzaskowski, Maciej (2018). "Comparison of Genotypic and Phenotypic Correlations: Cheverud's Conjecture in Humans". Genetics 209 (3): 941–948. doi:10.1534/genetics.117.300630. PMID 29739817. 
  37. Krapohl, E.; Plomin, R. (2016). "Genetic link between family socioeconomic status and children's educational achievement estimated from genome-wide SNPS". Molecular Psychiatry 21 (3): 437–443. doi:10.1038/mp.2015.2. PMID 25754083. 
  38. Plomin et al., p. 397
  39. Tong, Pin; Monahan, Jack; Prendergast, James G. D. (2017). "Shared regulatory sites are abundant in the human genome and shed light on genome evolution and disease pleiotropy". PLOS Genetics 13 (3): e1006673. doi:10.1371/journal.pgen.1006673. PMID 28282383. 
  40. Munafò, Marcus R.; Tilling, Kate; Taylor, Amy E.; Evans, David M.; Davey Smith, George (2018). "Collider scope: When selection bias can substantially influence observed associations". International Journal of Epidemiology 47 (1): 226–235. doi:10.1093/ije/dyx206. PMID 29040562. 
  41. Hewitt, J. K.; Eaves, L. J.; Neale, M. C.; Meyer, J. M. (1988). "Resolving causes of developmental continuity or "tracking." I. Longitudinal twin studies during growth". Behavior Genetics 18 (2): 133–51. doi:10.1007/BF01067836. PMID 3377729. 
  42. Deary, I. J.; Yang, J.; Davies, G.; Harris, S. E.; Tenesa, A.; Liewald, D.; Luciano, M.; Lopez, L. M. et al. (2012). "Genetic contributions to stability and change in intelligence from childhood to old age". Nature 482 (7384): 212–5. doi:10.1038/nature10781. PMID 22258510. Bibcode2012Natur.482..212D. https://www.pure.ed.ac.uk/ws/files/8895372/genetic_contributions_to_stability.pdf. 
  43. Plomin et al., pp. 184–185: "The substantial comorbidity between specific cognitive disabilities is largely due to genetic factors, meaning that the same genes affect different learning disabilities although there are also disability-specific genes."
  44. Rietveld, C. A.; Medland, S. E.; Derringer, J.; Yang, J.; Esko, T.; Martin, N. W.; Westra, H. J.; Shakhbazov, K. et al. (2013). "GWAS of 126,559 individuals identifies genetic variants associated with educational attainment". Science 340 (6139): 1467–1471. doi:10.1126/science.1235488. PMID 23722424. Bibcode2013Sci...340.1467R. 
  45. Rietveld, C. A.; Esko, T.; Davies, G.; Pers, T. H.; Turley, P.; Benyamin, B.; Chabris, C. F.; Emilsson, V. et al. (2014). "Common genetic variants associated with cognitive performance identified using the proxy-phenotype method". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 111 (38): 13790–13794. doi:10.1073/pnas.1404623111. PMID 25201988. Bibcode2014PNAS..11113790R. 
  46. Krapohl, E.; Patel, H.; Newhouse, S.; Curtis, C. J.; von Stumm, S.; Dale, P. S.; Zabaneh, D.; Breen, G. et al. (2018). "Multi-polygenic score approach to trait prediction". Molecular Psychiatry 23 (5): 1368–1374. doi:10.1038/mp.2017.163. PMID 28785111. 
  47. Hill, W. D.; Marioni, R. E.; Maghzian, O.; Ritchie, S. J.; Hagenaars, S. P.; McIntosh, A. M.; Gale, C. R.; Davies, G. et al. (2019). "A combined analysis of genetically correlated traits identifies 187 loci and a role for neurogenesis and myelination in intelligence". Molecular Psychiatry 24 (2): 169–181. doi:10.1038/s41380-017-0001-5. PMID 29326435. 
  48. Turley, Patrick; Walters, Raymond K.; Maghzian, Omeed; Okbay, Aysu; Lee, James J.; Fontana, Mark Alan; Nguyen-Viet, Tuan Anh; Wedow, Robbee et al. (2018). "Multi-trait analysis of genome-wide association summary statistics using MTAG". Nature Genetics 50 (2): 229–237. doi:10.1038/s41588-017-0009-4. PMID 29292387. 
  49. Andreassen, Ole A.; Thompson, Wesley K.; Schork, Andrew J.; Ripke, Stephan; Mattingsdal, Morten; Kelsoe, John R.; Kendler, Kenneth S.; O'Donovan, Michael C. et al. (2013). "Improved Detection of Common Variants Associated with Schizophrenia and Bipolar Disorder Using Pleiotropy-Informed Conditional False Discovery Rate". PLOS Genetics 9 (4): e1003455. doi:10.1371/journal.pgen.1003455. PMID 23637625. 
  50. Porter, Heather F.; o'Reilly, Paul F. (2017). "Multivariate simulation framework reveals performance of multi-trait GWAS methods". Scientific Reports 7: 38837. doi:10.1038/srep38837. PMID 28287610. Bibcode2017NatSR...738837P. 
  51. De Vlaming, Ronald; Okbay, Aysu; Rietveld, Cornelius A.; Johannesson, Magnus; Magnusson, Patrik K. E.; Uitterlinden, André G.; Van Rooij, Frank J. A.; Hofman, Albert et al. (2017). "Meta-GWAS Accuracy and Power (MetaGAP) Calculator Shows that Hiding Heritability is Partially Due to Imperfect Genetic Correlations across Studies". PLOS Genetics 13 (1): e1006495. doi:10.1371/journal.pgen.1006495. PMID 28095416. 
  52. Hazel, L. N. (1943). "The Genetic Basis for Constructing Selection Indexes". Genetics 28 (6): 476–490. doi:10.1093/genetics/28.6.476. PMID 17247099. 
  53. Rae, A. L. (1951) "The Importance of Genetic Correlations in Selection"
  54. Hazel, L. N.; Lush, JAY L. (1942). "The Efficiency of Three Methods of Selection". Journal of Heredity 33 (11): 393–399. doi:10.1093/oxfordjournals.jhered.a105102. https://www.dropbox.com/s/a4na5o5hv4la0q2/1943-hazel.pdf. 
  55. Lerner, M. (1950) Population genetics and animal improvement: as illustrated by the inheritance of egg production. New York: Cambridge Univ. Press
  56. Falconer, pp. 324–329
  57. Conner, Jeffrey K. (2012). "Quantitative Genetic Approaches to Evolutionary Constraint: How Useful?". Evolution 66 (11): 3313–3320. doi:10.1111/j.1558-5646.2012.01794.x. PMID 23106699. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/232736616. 
  58. Beldade, Patrícia; Koops, Kees; Brakefield, Paul M. (2002). "Developmental constraints versus flexibility in morphological evolution". Nature 416 (6883): 844–847. doi:10.1038/416844a. PMID 11976682. Bibcode2002Natur.416..844B. http://www.beldade.nl/pubs/PDFs/2002-PBeldade-Nature-sel.pdf. 
  59. Allen, Cerisse E.; Beldade, Patrícia; Zwaan, Bas J.; Brakefield, Paul M. (2008). "Differences in the selection response of serially repeated color pattern characters: Standing variation, development, and evolution". BMC Evolutionary Biology 8: 94. doi:10.1186/1471-2148-8-94. PMID 18366752. 
  60. Lee, S.H.; Yang, J.; Goddard, M.E.; Visscher, P.M.; Wray, N.R. (2012). "Estimation of pleiotropy between complex diseases using single-nucleotide polymorphism-derived genomic relationships and restricted maximum likelihood". Bioinformatics 28 (19): 2540–2542. doi:10.1093/bioinformatics/bts474. PMID 22843982. 
  61. Bulik-Sullivan, B. K.; Loh, P. R.; Finucane, H.; Ripke, S.; Yang, J.; Schizophrenia Working Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium; Patterson, N.; Daly, M. J. et al. (2015). "LD Score regression distinguishes confounding from polygenicity in genome-wide association studies". Nature Genetics 47 (3): 291–295. doi:10.1038/ng.3211. PMID 25642630. 
  62. Loh, Po-Ru; Bhatia, Gaurav; Gusev, Alexander; Finucane, Hilary K.; Bulik-Sullivan, Brendan K.; Pollack, Samuela J.; Psychiatric Genomics Consortium, Schizophrenia Working Group; De Candia, Teresa R. et al. (2015). Contrasting regional architectures of schizophrenia and other complex diseases using fast variance components analysis. doi:10.1101/016527. 
  63. Majumdar, Arunabha; Haldar, Tanushree; Bhattacharya, Sourabh; Witte, John S. (2018). "An efficient Bayesian meta-analysis approach for studying cross-phenotype genetic associations". PLOS Genetics 14 (2): e1007139. doi:10.1371/journal.pgen.1007139. PMID 29432419. 
  64. Shi, Huwenbo; Kichaev, Gleb; Pasaniuc, Bogdan (2016). "Contrasting the Genetic Architecture of 30 Complex Traits from Summary Association Data". The American Journal of Human Genetics 99 (1): 139–153. doi:10.1016/j.ajhg.2016.05.013. PMID 27346688. 
  65. Lynch, Michael (1999). "Estimating genetic correlations in natural populations". Genetical Research 74 (3): 255–264. doi:10.1017/s0016672399004243. PMID 10689802. 
  66. Golan, David; Lander, Eric S.; Rosset, Saharon (2014). "Measuring missing heritability: Inferring the contribution of common variants". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 111 (49): E5272–E5281. doi:10.1073/pnas.1419064111. PMID 25422463. Bibcode2014PNAS..111E5272G. 
  67. Shi, Huwenbo; Mancuso, Nicholas; Spendlove, Sarah; Pasaniuc, Bogdan (2017). "Local Genetic Correlation Gives Insights into the Shared Genetic Architecture of Complex Traits". The American Journal of Human Genetics 101 (5): 737–751. doi:10.1016/j.ajhg.2017.09.022. PMID 29100087. 
  68. Posthuma, Daniëlle; Boomsma, Dorret I. (2000). "A Note on the Statistical Power in Extended Twin Designs". Behavior Genetics 30 (2): 147–158. doi:10.1023/A:1001959306025. PMID 10979605. http://www.tweelingenregister.org/nederlands/verslaggeving/NTR_publicaties/Posthuma_BG_2000_01.pdf. Retrieved 2016-10-24. 
  69. DeFries, J. C., Plomin, Robert, LaBuda, Michele C. (1987). "Genetic Stability of Cognitive Development From Childhood to Adulthood". Developmental Psychology 23 (1): 4–12. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.23.1.4. https://www.dropbox.com/s/86clblsx9vyg6n1/1987-defries.pdf. 
  70. Wray, Naomi R.; Lee, Sang Hong; Kendler, Kenneth S. (2012). "Impact of diagnostic misclassification on estimation of genetic correlations using genome-wide genotypes". European Journal of Human Genetics 20 (6): 668–674. doi:10.1038/ejhg.2011.257. PMID 22258521. 
  71. Falconer, pp. 317–318
  72. Schmitt, J. E.; Eyler, L. T.; Giedd, J. N.; Kremen, W. S.; Kendler, K. S.; Neale, M. C. (2007). "Review of twin and family studies on neuroanatomic phenotypes and typical neurodevelopment". Twin Research and Human Genetics 10 (5): 683–694. doi:10.1375/twin.10.5.683. PMID 17903108. 
  73. Almasy, L.; Dyer, T. D.; Blangero, J. (1997). "Bivariate quantitative trait linkage analysis: Pleiotropy versus co-incident linkages". Genetic Epidemiology 14 (6): 953–8. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1098-2272(1997)14:6<953::AID-GEPI65>3.0.CO;2-K. PMID 9433606. 
  74. Heath, A. C.; Neale, M. C.; Hewitt, J. K.; Eaves, L. J.; Fulker, D. W. (1989). "Testing structural equation models for twin data using LISREL". Behavior Genetics 19 (1): 9–35. doi:10.1007/BF01065881. PMID 2712816. 

Cited sources

External links