Biology:Prepulse inhibition

From HandWiki
Prepulse inhibition: preceding stimulus attenuates the startle response.

Prepulse inhibition (PPI) is a neurological phenomenon in which a weaker prestimulus (prepulse) inhibits the reaction of an organism to a subsequent strong reflex-eliciting stimulus (pulse), often using the startle reflex. The stimuli are usually acoustic, but tactile stimuli (e.g. via air puffs onto the skin)[1] and light stimuli[2] are also used. When prepulse inhibition is high, the corresponding one-time startle response is reduced.

The reduction of the amplitude of startle reflects the ability of the nervous system to temporarily adapt to a strong sensory stimulus when a preceding weaker signal is given to warn the organism. PPI is detected in numerous species including mice and humans. Although the extent of the adaptation affects numerous systems, the most comfortable to measure are the muscular reactions, which are normally diminished as a result of the nervous inhibition.

Deficits of prepulse inhibition manifest in the inability to filter out the unnecessary information; they have been linked to abnormalities of sensorimotor gating. Such deficits are noted in patients with conditions like schizophrenia and Alzheimer's disease, and in people under the influence of drugs, surgical manipulations, or mutations. Human studies of PPI have been summarised in reviews by Braff et al. (2001)[3] and Swerdlow et al. (2008).[4]

PPI and startle reflex apparatus for mice

Procedure

PPI measurement in human.

The main three parts of the procedure are prepulse, startle stimulus, and startle reflex. Different prepulse-to-pulse intervals, or lead intervals, are used: 30, 60, 120, 240 and 480 ms. Lead interval counts from the start of prepulse to the start of the pulse. With the interval exceeding 500 ms, prepulse facilitation – increased response – is most likely to follow.[5]

A burst of white noise is usually used as the acoustic startle stimulus. Typical durations are 20 ms for prepulse and 40 ms for pulse. Background noise with 65-70 dB is used in human studies, and 30–40 dB in rodent experiments. Prepulse is typically set 3–12 dB louder than background. Startle response is measured in rodents using the so-called automated "startle chambers" or "stabilimeter chambers", with detectors recording whole-body reaction.[5]

In humans, the movements of oculomotor muscles ("eye-blink reflex" or "eye-blink response" assessed using electromyographic recording of orbicularis oculi muscle and by oculography) could be used as a measure. Pulse-alone results are compared to prepulse-plus-pulse, and the percentage of the reduction in the startle reflex represents prepulse inhibition. Possible hearing impairment must be taken into account, as, for example, several strains of mice develop high frequency hearing loss when they mature.[5]

The recorded signal needs to be passband filtered between 28 Hz and 500 Hz. By this step, artifacts from eye movements and muscle activity independent of blink responses are removed. To avoid aliasing artifacts the sampling rate of the signal should be at least 1024 Hz which is larger than twice the upper bound of the bandpass filter (twice the Nyquist frequency). After filtering, the resulting signal is rectified and smoothed.[6]

Reporting the signal deflection evoked by the startle stimulus, the term mean amplitude (mA) refers to the average startle response excluding nonresponse trials. However, to calculate the mean magnitude (mM), nonresponse trials are set to zero before averaging. Dividing the number detected responses (number of trials used to compute amplitude) by the total number of eliciting stimuli yields the response probability (P). Thus, increasing response probability shifts average response magnitude towards average response amplitude.[6][7]

[math]\displaystyle{ mM = mA \times P }[/math][8]

It is recommend to use the computed mean magnitude to report the average startle response. Since this metric includes also nonresponse measures, it exhibits increased validity compared to mean amplitude.[6]

Major features

The magnitude of PPI is often significant, reaching as much as 65% in healthy subjects, with maximum inhibition is typically observed at 120 ms interval.[9] The baseline startle response does not affect overall PPI levels – this finding was first discovered in rat studies[10] and later duplicated in the studies of mice.[11] The opposite reaction, Prepulse Facilitation (PPF)-- the tendency for a subject to have an increased startle response following a lower-intensity prepulse stimulus—is typically noted when the interval between stimuli lasts longer than 500 ms. PPF is thought to reflect, at least partially, sustained attention: the prepulse, if not followed within less than half a second by the pulse, will tend to make the subject more likely to have a startle response rather than less.

Another moderating variable in prepulse inhibition and facilitation is sex difference, with men having higher PPI (i.e., when a prepulse is quickly followed by a pulse, all subjects tend to experience a reduced startle response, with men often experiencing lesser startle responses compared to women) and women having higher PPF (i.e., when the prepulse occurs more than half a second before the pulse, all subjects tend to experience heightened startle responses, with women tending to experience greater startle responses than men).[12] For acoustic stimuli, monaural PPI is higher than binaural—the startle response is reduced when the stimulus (noise) is only experienced in one ear rather than both ears.[13][14] Even the very first prepulse of a test session induces inhibition, which indicates that conditioning and learning are not necessary for this effect to occur. However, the lack of conditionality has been questioned.[15] However a thousandth prepulse also induces inhibition; the phenomenon is highly robust [16]

Response and reaction are affected by interval duration and attention. It is thought that the short intervals used in PPI task do not give enough time for the activation of a volitional response: the reaction to pulses and prepulses separated by short time frames is believed to be involuntary. Prepulses could be attended or ignored, and attention affects the outcome. In one study, normal college students were instructed to attend to one of the kind of prepulses, high- or low-pitched, and ignore the other. Attended prepulse caused significantly greater inhibition at the 120 ms interval compared to the ignored one, and significantly greater facilitation at the 2000 ms interval. This reflects the tendency of subjects to expect to hear a particular frequency of pulsed sound, an effect that is highly pronounced when the prepulse occurs two seconds before the pulse and when there are other pulses being heard which are being consciously "ignored".[17] Increased prepulse duration leads to increase in PPI: the longer the prepulse, the greater the reduction in subsequent startle responses. Steady background noise facilitates the startle response, while pulsed background produces inhibition.[18]

History of scientific work

Prepulse inhibition was first described in 1862 by Sechenov and was rediscovered at least twice before Howard S. Hoffman discovered it again in 1963. Hoffman was the first to use the term prepulse. He continued his work on startle into the 1980s. His many papers on the reflex and its modification laid the groundwork for the widespread use of prepulse inhibition today in studies of schizophrenia and other disorders.[19]

One possible reason it was discovered and rediscovered is that it got confused with Pavlovian conditioning. Another is that it didn't fit nicely into theories of the times.[20]

Disruption

Disruptions of PPI are studied in humans and many other species. The most studied are deficits of PPI in schizophrenia, although this disease is not the only one to be associated with such deficits. They have been noted in panic disorder (Ludewig, et al., 2005), schizotypal personality disorder,[21] obsessive-compulsive disorder (Swerdlow et al., 1993), Huntington's disease,[22] nocturnal enuresis and attention deficit disorder (Ornitz et al. 1992), and Tourette's syndrome (Swerdlow et al. 1994; Castellanos et al. 1996). According to one study, people who have temporal lobe epilepsy with psychosis also show decreases in PPI, unlike those who have TLE without psychosis.[23] Therefore, PPI deficits are not typical to specific disease, but rather tell of disruptions in a specific brain circuit.

PPI deficit in schizophrenia

PPI deficits represent a well-described finding in schizophrenia, with the first report dating back to 1978.[24] The abnormalities are also noted in unaffected relatives of the patients.[25][26] In one study, patients failed to show increased PPI to attended prepulses.[27] Dopamine, which plays a major role in schizophrenia, had been shown to regulate sensorimotor gating in rodent models.[28][29] These findings fit to the dopamine hypothesis of schizophrenia. In theory, PPI disruption in schizophrenia may be related to the processes of sensory flooding and cognitive fragmentation.

Antipsychotic medication have been shown to increase PPI in patients, with atypical antipsychotics having more effect. Patients display the same gender difference in PPI as healthy people: males have higher PPI compared to females. One notable finding is that patients are specifically deficient in PPI with 60 ms prepulse intervals relative to intervals of other lengths; this remains so even under antipsychotic treatment.[30]

The other fact is the influence of cigarette smoking: the research suggests that smoking does indeed "calm the nerves". Non-smoking patients have lower PPI compared to smokers, and heavy smokers have the highest PPI.[30][31] This finding runs in accord with the high rates of smoking among schizophrenic patients, estimated at 70%,[32] with many patients smoking more than 30 cigarettes a day.[33] Some studies show association of schizophrenia with the CHRNA7 and CHRFAM7A genes, which code for alpha7 subunit of nicotinic receptors, but other studies are negative.[34][35] Contrary to the predictions, nicotine receptor alpha7 subunit knockout mice do not show disruptions in PPI.[36]

In rodents

Murine models are widely used to test hypotheses linking genetic components of various diseases with sensorimotor gating. While some of the hypotheses stand to the test, others are not, as some mice models show unchanged or increased PPI contrary to the expectations, as in the tests of COMT-deficient mice.[37]

Certain surgical procedures also disrupt PPI in animals, helping to unravel the underlying circuitry.

Many animal studies of PPI are undertaken in order to understand and model the pathology of schizophrenia.[38] Schizophrenia-like PPI disruption techniques in rodents have been classified in one review[39] into four models:

  • PPI impairment driven by dopamine-receptor agonists, most validated for antipsychotic studies;
  • PPI impairment by 5-HT2 receptor agonists;
  • PPI impairment by NMDAR antagonists;
  • PPI impairment by developmental intervention (isolation rearing, maternal deprivation).

Diverse chemical compounds are tested on animals with such deficits. Compounds that are able to restore PPI could be further investigated for their potential antipsychotic role.

A review of the genetic underpinnings of prepulse inhibition can be found in a meta-analysis conducted by Quednow et al. (2017).[40] Additionally an updated summary of both preclinical and clinical findings with PPI can be found in a recent comprehensive review.[4]

See also

References

  1. "Gating and habituation of the startle reflex in schizophrenic patients". Archives of General Psychiatry 49 (3): 206–15. March 1992. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1992.01820030038005. PMID 1567275. 
  2. "Rat strain differences in startle gating-disruptive effects of apomorphine occur with both acoustic and visual prepulses". Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior 88 (3): 306–11. January 2008. doi:10.1016/j.pbb.2007.08.014. PMID 17900675. 
  3. "Human studies of prepulse inhibition of startle: normal subjects, patient groups, and pharmacological studies". Psychopharmacology 156 (2–3): 234–58. July 2001. doi:10.1007/s002130100810. PMID 11549226. 
  4. 4.0 4.1 "Realistic expectations of prepulse inhibition in translational models for schizophrenia research". Psychopharmacology 199 (3): 331–88. August 2008. doi:10.1007/s00213-008-1072-4. PMID 18568339. 
  5. 5.0 5.1 5.2 "Mouse genetic models for prepulse inhibition: an early review". Molecular Psychiatry 7 (10): 1039–53. 2002. doi:10.1038/sj.mp.4001159. PMID 12476318. 
  6. 6.0 6.1 6.2 Blumenthal, Terry D.; Cuthbert, Bruce N.; Filion, Diane L.; Hackley, Steven; Lipp, Ottmar V.; Van Boxtel, Anton (2005). "Committee report: Guidelines for human startle eyeblink electromyographic studies" (in en). Psychophysiology 42 (1): 1–15. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2005.00271.x. ISSN 0048-5772. PMID 15720576. 
  7. Blumenthal, Terry D.; Elden, Aake; Flaten, Magne Arve (2004). "A comparison of several methods used to quantify prepulse inhibition of eyeblink responding" (in en). Psychophysiology 41 (2): 326–332. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2003.00144.x. ISSN 0048-5772. PMID 15032998. 
  8. Blumenthal, Terry D.; Keith Berg, W. (1986). "Stimulus Rise Time, Intensity, and Bandwidth Effects on Acoustic Startle Amplitude and Probability" (in en). Psychophysiology 23 (6): 635–641. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.1986.tb00682.x. ISSN 0048-5772. PMID 3823338. 
  9. "Presidential Address, 1974. The more or less startling effects of weak prestimulation". Psychophysiology 12 (3): 238–48. May 1975. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.1975.tb01284.x. PMID 1153628. 
  10. "Neural circuit regulation of prepulse inhibition of startle in the rat: current knowledge and future challenges". Psychopharmacology 156 (2–3): 194–215. July 2001. doi:10.1007/s002130100799. PMID 11549223. 
  11. "Inbred strain differences in prepulse inhibition of the mouse startle response". Psychopharmacology 132 (2): 169–80. July 1997. doi:10.1007/s002130050333. PMID 9266614. https://zenodo.org/record/1232637. 
  12. "Sex effects in prepulse inhibition and facilitation of the acoustic startle response: implications for pharmacological and treatment studies". Journal of Psychopharmacology 19 (1): 39–45. January 2005. doi:10.1177/0269881105048890. PMID 15671127. 
  13. "Inhibition of the glabella reflex by monaural and binaural stimulation". Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance 6 (4): 769–76. November 1980. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.6.4.769. PMID 6449543. 
  14. "Startle gating in antipsychotic-naïve first episode schizophrenia patients: one ear is better than two". Psychiatry Research 151 (1–2): 21–8. May 2007. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2006.09.013. PMID 17382404. 
  15. Folk, Charles L.; Gibson, Bradley S. (2001). Attraction, Distraction and Action: multiple perspectives on attentional capture. Amsterdam; New York: Elsevier. ISBN 978-0-444-50676-4. https://books.google.com/books?id=hxSyS33QC6IC&q=prepulse+inhibition&pg=PA179. 
  16. "Startle reflex inhibition in the rat: its persistence after extended repetition of the inhibitory stimulus". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 10 (2): 221–8. April 1984. doi:10.1037/0097-7403.10.2.221. PMID 6716052. 
  17. "Modification of the acoustic startle-reflex eyeblink: a tool for investigating early and late attentional processes". Biological Psychology 35 (3): 185–200. July 1993. doi:10.1016/0301-0511(93)90001-O. PMID 8218613. 
  18. "Startle Reaction: Modification by Background Acoustic Stimulation". Science 141 (3584): 928–30. September 1963. doi:10.1126/science.141.3584.928. PMID 14043340. Bibcode1963Sci...141..928H. 
  19. Powers, Alice S; Leitner, Donald (2007). "Obituary: Howard S. Hoffman (1925-2006)". American Psychologist 62 (4): 320. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.62.4.320. 
  20. "Reflex modification in the domain of startle: II. The anomalous history of a robust and ubiquitous phenomenon". Psychological Bulletin 94 (1): 3–17. July 1983. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.94.1.3. PMID 6353464. 
  21. "Impaired startle prepulse inhibition and habituation in patients with schizotypal personality disorder". The American Journal of Psychiatry 150 (12): 1862–7. December 1993. doi:10.1176/ajp.150.12.1862. PMID 8238643. 
  22. "Impaired prepulse inhibition of acoustic and tactile startle response in patients with Huntington's disease". Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 58 (2): 192–200. February 1995. doi:10.1136/jnnp.58.2.192. PMID 7876851. 
  23. Morton, N., Gray, N.S., Mellers, J., Toone, B., Lishman, W.A., & Gray, J.A. (1994). Prepulse inhibition in temporal lobe epilepsy. Schizophrenic Research, 15, 191.
  24. "Prestimulus effects on human startle reflex in normals and schizophrenics". Psychophysiology 15 (4): 339–43. July 1978. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.1978.tb01390.x. PMID 693742. 
  25. "Reduced prepulse inhibition in unaffected siblings of schizophrenia patients". Psychophysiology 42 (5): 588–94. September 2005. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2005.00346.x. PMID 16176381. 
  26. "Modulation of the startle response and startle laterality in relatives of schizophrenic patients and in subjects with schizotypal personality disorder: evidence of inhibitory deficits". The American Journal of Psychiatry 157 (10): 1660–8. October 2000. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.157.10.1660. PMID 11007721. 
  27. "Deficient attentional modulation of startle eyeblink is associated with symptom severity in the schizophrenia spectrum". Schizophrenia Research 93 (1–3): 288–95. July 2007. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2007.03.012. PMID 17478083. 
  28. "Dopaminergic stimulation disrupts sensorimotor gating in the rat". Psychopharmacology 94 (4): 507–14. 1988. doi:10.1007/BF00212846. PMID 3131796. 
  29. "Effects of spiperone, raclopride, SCH 23390 and clozapine on apomorphine inhibition of sensorimotor gating of the startle response in the rat". The Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics 256 (2): 530–6. February 1991. PMID 1825226. 
  30. 30.0 30.1 "Startle gating deficits in a large cohort of patients with schizophrenia: relationship to medications, symptoms, neurocognition, and level of function". Archives of General Psychiatry 63 (12): 1325–35. December 2006. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.63.12.1325. PMID 17146007. 
  31. "Influence of cigarette smoking on prepulse inhibition of the acoustic startle response in schizophrenia". Human Psychopharmacology 16 (4): 321–326. June 2001. doi:10.1002/hup.286. PMID 12404567. 
  32. "Smoking and mental illness". Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior 70 (4): 561–70. December 2001. doi:10.1016/S0091-3057(01)00677-3. PMID 11796154. 
  33. "Schizophrenia and tobacco smoking: a replication study in another US psychiatric hospital". Schizophrenia Research 56 (1–2): 55–65. July 2002. doi:10.1016/S0920-9964(01)00192-X. PMID 12084420. 
  34. Gene Overview of All Published Schizophrenia-Association Studies for CHRFAM7A – Schizophrenia Gene Database.
  35. Gene Overview of All Published Schizophrenia-Association Studies for CHRNA7 – Schizophrenia Gene Database
  36. "Alpha7 nicotinic receptor subunits are not necessary for hippocampal-dependent learning or sensorimotor gating: a behavioral characterization of Acra7-deficient mice". Learning & Memory 5 (4–5): 302–16. 1998. doi:10.1101/lm.5.4.302. PMID 10454356. 
  37. "Catechol-O-methyltransferase-deficient mice exhibit sexually dimorphic changes in catecholamine levels and behavior". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 95 (17): 9991–6. August 1998. doi:10.1073/pnas.95.17.9991. PMID 9707588. Bibcode1998PNAS...95.9991G. 
  38. "Using an animal model of deficient sensorimotor gating to study the pathophysiology and new treatments of schizophrenia". Schizophrenia Bulletin 24 (2): 285–301. 1998. doi:10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a033326. PMID 9613626. 
  39. "Pharmacological studies of prepulse inhibition models of sensorimotor gating deficits in schizophrenia: a decade in review". Psychopharmacology 156 (2–3): 117–54. July 2001. doi:10.1007/s002130100811. PMID 11549216. 
  40. "Meta-analysis on the association between genetic polymorphisms and prepulse inhibition of the acoustic startle response". Schizophrenia Research 198: 52–59. December 2017. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2017.12.011. PMID 29287625. https://kclpure.kcl.ac.uk/portal/en/publications/metaanalysis-on-the-association-between-genetic-polymorphisms-and-prepulse-inhibition-of-the-acoustic-startle-response(8520e7d0-4d76-4c2e-9c94-6c9e444650e2).html. 

External links