Digit ratio

From HandWiki
Short description: Ratio of lengths of fingers
Hand with index finger being shorter than the ring finger, resulting in a small 2D:4D ratio, pointing to a high exposure to testosterone in the uterus.
Image shows the measurement procedure of dorsal digit length using vernier calipers. The hand is placed on the edge of a table so that fingers are rested on table while making an angle of 90 degrees to the palm.

The digit ratio is the ratio taken of the lengths of different digits or fingers on a hand. In modern sciences, the 2D:4D ratio has become the most commonly studied digit ratio and is calculated by dividing the length of the index finger of a given hand by the length of the ring finger of the same hand. On average, males have a lower 2D:4D ratio than females.[1]

Although studies have claimed to show a correlation between the 2D:4D digit ratio and various physical and behavioral traits,[2] these studies have drawn criticism for irreproducible or contradictory findings,[3][4][5] exaggerated claims of usefulness,[6][7] and lack of high quality research protocols.[7] The most recent study, a longitudinal study that looked at children over a period of several years, determined that the digit ratio was not related to prenatal amniotic sex hormone levels.[8]

Measurement

The digit length is typically measured on the palmar (ventral, "front", "palm-side") hand, from the midpoint of the bottom crease to the tip of the finger.[9] Measurement of the digits on the dorsal ("back-side") hand, from the tip of the finger to the proximal phalange-bone protrusion (which occurs when digits are bent at 90 degrees to the palm), has recently also gained acceptance.[10][11] A study has shown that, compared to the palmar digit ratio, the dorsal digit ratio is a better indicator of bone digit ratio.[11] Moreover, differential placing of flexion creases is a factor in the palmar digit ratio.[12]

Other digit ratios are also similarly calculated in the same hand.

It has been suggested that because academics have accepted a variety of techniques and equipment (such as calipers, radiography and photocopy), researchers deliberately record multiple measurements and report only those which have significant findings,[7] a form of reporting bias.

Correlations

The ratio of two digits in particular – the palmar 2nd (index finger) and 4th (ring finger) – is supposedly affected by fetal exposure to hormones, in particular to testosterone, and other androgens; this 2D:4D ratio could therefore be considered a crude measure for prenatal androgen exposure, with lower 2D:4D ratios pointing to higher prenatal androgen exposure.[13][14][15][16][17][18][19][20] There are also studies that suggest that the palmar 2D:4D ratio is influenced by prenatal estrogen exposure, and that it thus correlates negatively not with prenatal testosterone alone, but rather with the prenatal testosterone-to-estrogen ratio (T:O).[21][9][22][23]

However, various studies have challenged the aforementioned findings. A meta analysis of 54 studies (with 8077 participants) found there was no correlation between digit ratio and adult testosterone levels (44 studies), testosterone change (6 studies) and prenatal testosterone (10 studies). The researchers conclude that due to the link being unsupported, it is concerningly common that papers use 2D:4D as a proxy for testosterone.[24] Another paper has also found no relationship between 2D:4D ratios and testosterone or estradiol levels, or even the testosterone-to-estradiol ratio.[25] The largest study on 2D:4D as of 2018 failed to find evidence for 2D:4D being a marker for prenatal androgen exposure,[26] and one frequently cited paper on the topic[21] has failed to be replicated.[27]

A study measuring the 2D:4D of deceased fetuses found the ratio to be sexually dimorphic, being lower in men than in women. However, when comparing the extent of the sexual dimorphism to adults and children, the researchers found the extent was similar to children but lower than adults. This led the authors to suggest that the digit ratio is also affected by the postnatal development process.[28] Similarly, a study of people from Poland found a statistically significant correlation between digit ratio and age groups (divided by children, young adults and adults) for both males and females in the left hand, but only for women with the right hand.[29] In palmar digit ratios, strong sexual dimorphism occurs in those of digit 2.[30][31][32] In dorsal digit ratios, in contrast, strong sexual dimorphism occurs in those of digit 5, with women having shorter fifth digits on average.[10] Overall, the report of sexual dimorphism is much stronger in dorsal digit ratios[10] than in palmar digit ratios, especially as compared to the classic, palmar 2D:4D ratio.[33] Moreover, compared to palmar digit ratio, dorsal digit ratio is a better indicator of bone digit ratio.[11] Thus, while most of the earlier research has focused on palmar 2D:4D ratio, study of other digit ratios is also promising.

History of research

That a greater proportion of men have shorter index fingers than ring fingers than do women was noted in the scientific literature several times through the late 1800s,[34] with the statistically significant sex difference in a sample of 201 men and 109 women established by 1930,[35] after which time the sex difference appears to have been largely forgotten or ignored. In 1983, Glenn Wilson of King's College London published a study examining the correlation between assertiveness in women and their digit ratio, which found that women with a lower 2D:4D ratio reported greater assertiveness.[36] This was the first study to examine the correlation between digit ratio and a psychological trait within members of the same sex.[37] Wilson proposed that skeletal structure and personality were simultaneously affected by sex hormone levels in utero.[36] In 1998, John T. Manning and colleagues reported the sex difference in digit ratios was present in two-year-old children[38] and further developed the idea that the index was a marker of prenatal sex hormones. Since then, research on the topic has burgeoned around the world.

Since 2006, 2D:4D digit ratios have been used alongside other methods in an attempt understand Palaeolithic hand stencils found in prehistoric European and Indonesian cave painting.[39][40][41]

A 2009 study in Biology Letters argues: "Sexual differences in 2D:4D are mainly caused by the shift along the common allometric line with non-zero intercept, which means 2D:4D necessarily decreases with increasing finger length, and the fact that men have longer fingers than women",[42] which may be the basis for the sex difference in digit ratios and/or any putative hormonal influence on the ratios.

A 2011 paper by Zhengui Zheng and Martin J. Cohn reports "the 2D:4D ratio in mice is controlled by the balance of androgen to estrogen signaling during a narrow window of digit development".[13] The formation of the digits in humans, in utero, is thought to occur by 13 weeks, and the bone-to-bone ratio is consistent from this point into an individual's adulthood.[43] During this period if the fetus is exposed to androgens, the exact level of which is thought to be sexually dimorphic, the growth rate of the 4th digit is increased, as can be seen by analyzing the 2D:4D ratio of opposite sex dizygotic twins, where the female twin is exposed to excess androgens from her brother in utero, and thus has a significantly lower 2D:4D ratio.[44]

Importantly, there has been no reliable correlation between the sex hormone levels of an adult and the individual's 2D:4D,[14] which implies that it is strictly the exposure in utero that causes this phenomenon.

Marta Kowal alongside other researchers in 2020 published a paper providing more evidence that there is no relationship between adult testosterone levels and 2D:4D ratio, highlighting that testosterone levels are correlated with other factors (such as smoking,[45] body weight,[46] and diet[47]) that would impact the relationship between 2D:4D and an adult's testosterone level.[3]

The paper also highlights an issue within the research field: "One of the general limitations of studies on the digit ratios is the high number of degrees of freedom within the 2D:4D (the right, left, and right minus left 2D:4D). Running many analyses with different predictors increases the chances of finding allegedly significant results."

In 2020, a paper by John T Manning and Bernhard Fink reported that at the national level, high mean male 2D:4D was associated with high case fatality rates with COVID-19, and suggested that this may be used to "identify for whom it would be advisable to exercise social distancing."[48] Subsequent researchers failed to replicate their findings,[49][5] and criticised Manning and Fink for publishing papers that fail under scrutiny despite the urgent need for high quality science during the pandemic that informs medical decisions.[5]

In 2021, a peer reviewed paper by Professor James Smoliga titled "Giving science the finger—is the second-to-fourth digit ratio (2D:4D) a biomarker of good luck? A cross sectional study" heavily criticized the field of research. The researchers followed the same research methodology as other literature and were able to conclude that there is an association of low digit ratio with good luck. This result is attributed to chance, and an illustration of the reproducibility crisis. This is complemented by a satirical list of "pitfalls to avoid in research" which lists high quality research practices that authors hoping to publish false positive findings regarding 2D:4D digit ratios are likely to avoid, which are:

Smoliga also provides an algorithm for researchers to identify a correlation between 2D:4D digit ratio, and any outcome measure.[7] Smoliga would go on to label the field as pseudoscience.[50] In a response, John T. Manning criticized the researchers for not controlling for ethnicity.[51]

Evidence of androgen effect

The sex difference in 2D:4D is present before birth in humans.[22][52] The ratio of testosterone to estradiol measured in 33 amniocentesis samples correlated with the child's subsequent 2D:4D ratio.[21] The conclusion of this research supported to an association between low 2D:4D and high levels of testosterone compared with estrogen, and high 2D:4D with low testosterone relative to estrogen. However, this study has been criticised for its lack of scrutiny despite being frequently cited, and has failed to be replicated.[27]

The level of estrogen in the amniotic fluid is not correlated with higher 2D:4D, as researchers found no difference in estrogen levels between males and females.[21]

Researchers have highlighted that most research measuring amniotic fluid or umbilical cord blood to make conclusions regarding 2D:4D and early androgen exposure is tenuous, as although the research points towards a negative correlation, the results have not been statistically significant.[53]

Several studies present evidence that digit ratios are heritable.[54][55]

In a non-clinical sample of women, digit ratio correlated with anogenital distance in the expected direction. In other words, women with a greater anogenital distance, indicating greater prenatal androgen exposure, had a smaller digit ratio.[56]

Disorders of sex development

Women with congenital adrenal hyperplasia (CAH), which results in elevated androgen levels before birth, have lower, more masculinized 2D:4D on average.[15][16][57] Other possible physiological effects include an enlarged clitoris and shallow vagina.[58] Males with CAH have smaller (more masculine) digit ratios than control males,[15][16] suggesting that prenatal androgens affect digit ratios. Amniocentesis samples show that prenatal levels of testosterone are in the high-normal range in males with CAH, while levels of the weaker androgen androstenedione are several fold higher than in control males.[59][60][61] These measures indicate that males with CAH are exposed to greater prenatal concentrations of total androgens than are control males.

A greater digit ratio occurs for men with Klinefelter's syndrome, who have reduced testosterone secretion throughout life compared to control males, than in their fathers or control males.[18]

Digit ratio in men may correlate with genetic variation in the androgen receptor gene.[62] Men with genes that produce androgen receptors that are less sensitive to testosterone (because they have more CAG repeats) have greater, i.e. more feminine, digit ratios. There are reports of a failure to replicate this finding.[63] However, men carrying an androgen receptor with more CAG repeats compensate for the less sensitive receptor by secreting more testosterone,[64] probably as a result of reduced negative feedback on gonadotropins. Thus, it is not clear that 2D:4D would be expected to correlate with CAG repeats, even if it accurately reflects prenatal androgen.

XY individuals with androgen insensitivity syndrome (AIS) due to a dysfunctional gene for the androgen receptor present as women and have feminine digit ratios on average, as would be predicted if androgenic hormones affect digit ratios. This finding also demonstrates that the sex difference in digit ratios is unrelated to the Y chromosome per se.[65]

Explanation of the digit ratio effect

It is not clear why digit ratio might be influenced by prenatal hormones, with studies supporting the notion there is no relationship between digit ratios and prenatal hormones.[53][66][25] There is evidence of other similar traits, e.g., otoacoustic emissions and arm-to-trunk length ratio, which show similar effects. Hox genes responsible for both digit and penis development[67] have been implicated in affecting these multiple traits (pleiotropy). Direct effects of sex hormones on bone growth might be responsible, either by regulation of Hox genes in digit development or independently of such genes. Likewise, it is unclear why digit ratio on the right hand should be more responsive than that on the left hand, as is indicated by the greater sex difference on the right than the left.[68] However, because no right–left difference has been found in sexual dimorphism of bone digit ratios (2D:4D[69][52][70]) and dorsal digit ratios[71] and because differential placing of flexion creases contributes to sex differences in palmar digit ratio,[12] right–left differences in the placing of flexion creases may be determining right–left difference in palmar 2D:4D ratio.

Geographic and ethnic variation in 2D:4D

Manning and colleagues have shown that 2D:4D ratios vary greatly between different ethnic groups. In a study with Han, Berber, Uygur and Jamaican children as subjects, Manning et al. found that Han children had the highest mean values of 2D:4D (0.954±0.032), they were followed by the Berbers (0.950±0.033), then the Uygurs (0.946±0.037), and the Jamaican children had the lowest mean 2D:4D (0.935±0.035).[72][73] This variation is far larger than the differences between sexes; in Manning's words, "There's more difference between a Pole and a Finn, than a man and a woman."[74]

The standard deviations associated with each given 2D:4D mean are considerable. For example, the ratio for Han children (0.954±0.032) allows for a ratio as low as 0.922, while the ratio for Jamaican children (0.935±0.035) allows for a ratio as high as 0.970. Thus, some ethnic groups' confidence intervals overlap.[citation needed]

A 2008 study by Lu et al. found that the mean values of 2D:4D of the Hui and the Han in Ningxia were lower than those in European countries like Britain.[75]

In 2007, Manning et al. also found that mean 2D:4D varied across ethnic groups with higher ratios for Whites, non-Chinese Asians, and Middle-Easterners and lower ratios in Chinese and Black samples.[76]

Two studies explored the question of whether geographical differences in 2D:4D ratios were caused by gene pool differences or whether some environmental variable associated with latitude might be involved (e.g., exposure to sunlight or different day-length patterns). The conclusions were that geographical differences in 2D:4D ratio were caused by genetic pool differences, not by geographical latitude.[77][78]

Consanguineous parentage (inbreeding) has been found to lower the 2D:4D ratio in offspring,[79] which may account for some of the geographical and ethnic variation in 2D:4D ratios, as consanguinity rates depend on, among others, religion, culture, and geography.[80]

Digit ratio and development

There is some evidence that 2D:4D ratio may also be indicative for human development and growth. Ronalds et al. (2002) showed that men who had an above average placental weight and a shorter neonatal crown-heel length had higher 2D:4D ratios in adult life.[81] Kumar et al.[82] have reported that a similar effect of hand preference on digit lengths and digit ratios occurs oppositely among children and adults. Moreover, studies about 2D:4D correlations with face shape suggest that testosterone exposure early in life may set some constraints for subsequent development. Prenatal sex steroid ratios (in terms of 2D:4D) and actual chromosomal sex dimorphism were found to operate differently on human faces, but affect male and female face shape by similar patterns.[83] Fink et al. (2004) found that men with low (indicating high testosterone) and women with high (indicating high estrogen) 2D:4D ratios express greater levels of facial symmetry.[84]

Criticism

The digit ratio has received attention within mainstream media because of supposed correlations with health and behavior,[7] however the credibility and usefulness of research into the relationship between digit ratios and traits has been subject to criticism.[85] Critics have found research to be lacking moderate to strong statistical significance,[86][87] consistently failing to be replicated, and useless as a proxy variable.[4]

Although research into digit ratios appears to find statistical significance, there has been criticism of researchers for failing to account for a majority of the variation within a dataset, with one of the most highly publicized papers on the topic failing to account for 96% of variation.[6] Furthermore, skepticism has also been attributed to an abundance of research lacking a cause-and-effect relationship between digit ratio and supposed traits, research that often lacks any theoretical motivation.[88][86] This criticism was demonstrated in the paper "Giving science the finger—is the second-to-fourth digit ratio (2D:4D) a biomarker of good luck? A cross sectional study" by correlating low 2D:4D digit ratio with good luck, despite following established research methods.[7]

The utilization of digit ratio as a proxy variable has come under scrutiny,[88][85] as studies that investigate the relationship between a trait and some unobservable variable fail to consider other possible confounding variables.[89] This is especially problematic in cases where the relationship is likely to be weak, such as prenatal testosterone.[90][91][85] Furthermore, critics have pointed out that controlling for confounding variables is an impossible task due to the expanding list of confounding variables, including ethnicity, sex,[73][92] and physical, medical or behavioral conditions.[93]

Researchers have also pointed out that research into the digit ratio is illustrative of the ongoing replication crisis.[4] Not only have subsequent studies and meta-analysis failed to replicate previous research,[4][85][94] but have often found contradictory results,[95][91][96] or failed closer scrutiny.[97][86] Psychologist Martin Voracek has criticized the field for its irreproducible findings, stating it's "like a house of cards built on an unknown and uncertain base" and subsequently compared "the work on finger ratios to phrenology or physiognomy, the discredited ideas that people's head shape or facial features, respectively, reveal their personalities, character, and intelligence."[4]

Other animals

  • Dennis McFadden and collaborators have demonstrated sexual dimorphism in hind limb digit ratio in a number of great apes, including gorillas and chimpanzees.[31]
  • Emma Nelson and Susanne Shultz are currently investigating how 2D:4D relates to primate mating strategies and the evolution of human sociality.[98]
  • Nancy Burley's research group has demonstrated sexual dimorphism in zebra finches, and found a correlation between digit ratio in females and the strength of their preference for sexually selected traits in males.[99]
  • Alžbeta Talarovičová and collaborators found in rats that elevated testosterone during the prenatal period can influence 4D length, the 2D:4D ratio, and open field motor activity.[100]
  • Peter L. Hurd, Theodore Garland Jr., and their students have examined hindlimb 2D:4D in lines of mice selectively bred for high voluntary wheel-running behavior (see experimental evolution). These high-runner mice exhibit increased 2D:4D. This apparent "feminization" is opposite to the relation seen between 2D:4D and physical fitness in human beings, and is difficult to reconcile with the idea that 2D:4D is a clear proxy for prenatal androgen exposure in mice. The authors suggest that 2D:4D may more accurately reflect effect of glucocorticoids or other factors that regulate any of various genes.[101]
  • In pheasants, the ratio of the 2nd to 4th digit of the foot has been shown to be influenced by manipulations of testosterone in the egg.[102]
  • Studies in mice indicate that prenatal androgen acts primarily by promoting growth of the fourth digit.[13]

See also

References

  1. Zamani Sani, Seyed Hojjat; Sadeghi-Bahmani, Dena; Fathirezaie, Zahra; Aghdasi, Mohammad Taghi; Abbaspour, Kosar; Badicu, Georgian; Brand, Serge (5 June 2022). "Gender Differences and Relationship of 2D:4D-Ratio, Mental Toughness and Dark Triad Traits among Active Young Adults". Biology 11 (6): 864. doi:10.3390/biology11060864. ISSN 2079-7737. PMID 35741385. 
  2. Dolan, Eric W. (2024-01-12). "Surprising connection found between finger length ratios and psychopathological traits" (in en-US). https://www.psypost.org/2024/01/surprising-connection-found-between-finger-length-ratios-and-psychopathological-traits-220666. 
  3. 3.0 3.1 "No relationship between the digit ratios (2D:4D) and salivary testosterone change: Study on men under an acute exercise". Scientific Reports 10 (1): 10068. June 2020. doi:10.1038/s41598-020-66915-9. PMID 32572079. Bibcode2020NatSR..1010068K. 
  4. 4.0 4.1 4.2 4.3 4.4 "Talk to the hand. Scientists try to debunk idea that finger length can reveal personality and health". Science. 2019. doi:10.1126/science.aay2735. https://www.science.org/news/2019/06/talk-hand-scientists-try-debunk-idea-finger-length-can-reveal-personality-and-health. Retrieved 1 October 2021. 
  5. 5.0 5.1 5.2 "(Mis-)understanding COVID-19 and digit ratio: Methodological and statistical issues in Manning and Fink (2020)". Early Human Development 148: 105095. September 2020. doi:10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2020.105095. PMID 32499091. 
  6. 6.0 6.1 "Male Genital Length Study Falls Short". Science. 2011. https://www.psychologytoday.com/au/blog/you-it/201107/male-genital-length-study-falls-short. Retrieved 1 October 2021. 
  7. 7.0 7.1 7.2 7.3 7.4 7.5 "Giving science the finger-is the second-to-fourth digit ratio (2D:4D) a biomarker of good luck? A cross sectional study". BMJ 375: e067849. December 2021. doi:10.1136/bmj-2021-067849. PMID 34911738. 
  8. https://journals.plos.org/plosone/article?id=10.1371/journal.pone.0282253
  9. 9.0 9.1 "Human 2D (index) and 4D (ring) digit lengths: their variation and relationships during the menstrual cycle". Journal of Anatomy 211 (5): 630–638. November 2007. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7580.2007.00801.x. PMID 17764524. 
  10. 10.0 10.1 10.2 "Sexual Dimorphism in Digit Ratios Derived from Dorsal Digit Length among Adults and Children". Frontiers in Endocrinology 8: 41. 6 March 2017. doi:10.3389/fendo.2017.00041. PMID 28321205. 
  11. 11.0 11.1 11.2 "Second to fourth digit ratios, male genital development and reproductive health: a clinical study among fertile men and testis cancer patients". International Journal of Andrology 34 (4 Pt 2): e49–e58. August 2011. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2605.2010.01124.x. PMID 21091719. 
  12. 12.0 12.1 "Differential Placing of Flexion Creases Contributes to Sex Differences in the Second-to-Fourth Digit Ratio (2D:4D)". Frontiers in Endocrinology 10: 537. 2 August 2019. doi:10.3389/fendo.2019.00537. PMID 31428058. 
  13. 13.0 13.1 13.2 "Developmental basis of sexually dimorphic digit ratios". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 108 (39): 16289–16294. September 2011. doi:10.1073/pnas.1108312108. PMID 21896736. 
  14. 14.0 14.1 "Second to fourth digit length ratio (2D:4D) and adult sex hormone levels: new data and a meta-analytic review". Psychoneuroendocrinology 32 (4): 313–321. May 2007. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2007.01.007. PMID 17400395. 
  15. 15.0 15.1 15.2 "Masculinized finger length patterns in human males and females with congenital adrenal hyperplasia". Hormones and Behavior 42 (4): 380–386. December 2002. doi:10.1006/hbeh.2002.1830. PMID 12488105. 
  16. 16.0 16.1 16.2 "The ratio of second- and fourth-digit lengths and congenital adrenal hyperplasia due to 21-hydroxylase deficiency". Early Human Development 70 (1–2): 47–54. December 2002. doi:10.1016/s0378-3782(02)00073-7. PMID 12441204. 
  17. "New studies of second and fourth digit ratio as a morphogenetic trait in subjects with congenital adrenal hyperplasia". American Journal of Human Biology 26 (4): 559–561. 2014. doi:10.1002/ajhb.22545. PMID 24668932. 
  18. 18.0 18.1 "Digit ratio (2D:4D) in Klinefelter's syndrome". Andrology 1 (1): 94–99. January 2013. doi:10.1111/j.2047-2927.2012.00013.x. PMID 23258636. 
  19. "Digit ratio (2D:4D) in newborns: influences of prenatal testosterone and maternal environment". Early Human Development 89 (2): 107–112. February 2013. doi:10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2012.08.009. PMID 23017880. 
  20. "The use of digit ratios as markers for perinatal androgen action". Reproductive Biology and Endocrinology 4: 10. February 2006. doi:10.1186/1477-7827-4-10. PMID 16504142. 
  21. 21.0 21.1 21.2 21.3 "2nd to 4th digit ratios, fetal testosterone and estradiol". Early Human Development 77 (1–2): 23–28. April 2004. doi:10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2003.12.002. PMID 15113628. 
  22. 22.0 22.1 "Fetal development of the hand, digits and digit ratio (2D:4D)". Early Human Development 82 (7): 469–475. July 2006. doi:10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2005.12.002. PMID 16473482. 
  23. "Clinical review: Anogenital distance or digit length ratio as measures of fetal androgen exposure: relationship to male reproductive development and its disorders". The Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism 98 (6): 2230–2238. June 2013. doi:10.1210/jc.2012-4057. PMID 23569219. 
  24. Sorokowski, Piotr; Kowal, Marta (5 October 2023). "Relationship between the 2D : 4D and prenatal testosterone, adult level testosterone, and testosterone change: Meta-analysis of 54 studies" (in en). American Journal of Biological Anthropology 183 (1): 20–38. doi:10.1002/ajpa.24852. ISSN 2692-7691. PMID 37795916. https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/ajpa.24852. 
  25. 25.0 25.1 "Adult digit ratio (2D:4D) is not related to umbilical cord androgen or estrogen concentrations, their ratios or net bioactivity". Early Human Development 91 (2): 111–117. February 2015. doi:10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2014.12.011. PMID 25594498. https://research-repository.uwa.edu.au/en/publications/b3d44281-0fcf-499c-9743-7daac128c486. 
  26. "Genome-wide association study identifies nine novel loci for 2D:4D finger ratio, a putative retrospective biomarker of testosterone exposure in utero". Human Molecular Genetics 27 (11): 2025–2038. June 2018. doi:10.1093/hmg/ddy121. PMID 29659830. 
  27. 27.0 27.1 "Digit ratio (2D:4D) and amniotic testosterone and estradiol: an attempted replication of Lutchmaya et al. (2004)". Journal of Developmental Origins of Health and Disease 12 (6): 859–864. December 2021. doi:10.1017/S2040174420001294. PMID 33472723. https://repository.uel.ac.uk/download/80146174549c10eed26a47263c60f382cfb6cac134adaa6fa3e297afd005044b/349843/Richards%2C%20Brown%20and%20Constantinescu.pdf. 
  28. Galis, Frietson; Ten Broek, Clara M. A.; Van Dongen, Stefan; Wijnaendts, Liliane C. D. (February 2010). "Sexual Dimorphism in the Prenatal Digit Ratio (2D:4D)" (in en). Archives of Sexual Behavior 39 (1): 57–62. doi:10.1007/s10508-009-9485-7. ISSN 0004-0002. PMID 19301112. 
  29. Pruszkowska-Przybylska, Paulina; Kobus, Magdalena; Żądzińska, Elżbieta; Rosset, Iwona; Pruszkowska, Milena; Kuczyński, Wojciech; Sitek, Aneta (2023-12-21). "The age difference in 2D:4D among the Polish population: An exploratory study" (in en). Anthropological Review 86 (4): 99–110. doi:10.18778/1898-6773.86.4.08. ISSN 2083-4594. https://czasopisma.uni.lodz.pl/ar/article/view/18751. 
  30. "Relative finger lengths, sex differences, and psychological traits". Archives of Sexual Behavior 38 (2): 298–305. April 2009. doi:10.1007/s10508-007-9303-z. PMID 18340520. 
  31. 31.0 31.1 "Relative lengths of fingers and toes in human males and females". Hormones and Behavior 42 (4): 492–500. December 2002. doi:10.1006/hbeh.2002.1833. PMID 12488115. 
  32. "Comparative study of digit ratios (2D:4D and other) and novel measures of relative finger length: testing magnitude and consistency of sex differences across samples". Perceptual and Motor Skills 108 (1): 83–93. February 2009. doi:10.2466/pms.108.1.83-93. PMID 19425449. 
  33. "Meta-analysis of digit ratio 2D:4D shows greater sex difference in the right hand". American Journal of Human Biology 22 (5): 619–630. September 2010. doi:10.1002/ajhb.21054. PMID 20737609. 
  34. "Anthropological Notes on the Human Hand". American Anthropologist A1: 51–76. 1888. doi:10.1525/aa.1888.1.1.02a00040. 
  35. "Human finger types". The Anatomical Record 46 (2): 199–204. 1930. doi:10.1002/ar.1090460210. 
  36. 36.0 36.1 "Finger-length as an index of assertiveness in women". Personality and Individual Differences 4: 111–112. 1983. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(83)90061-2. 
  37. "Fingers to feminism: The rise of 2D:4D". Quarterly Review 4: 25–32. 2010. 
  38. "The ratio of 2nd to 4th digit length: a predictor of sperm numbers and concentrations of testosterone, luteinizing hormone and oestrogen". Human Reproduction 13 (11): 3000–3004. November 1998. doi:10.1093/humrep/13.11.3000. PMID 9853845. 
  39. "Sexual dimorphism in Upper Palaeolithic hand stencils". Antiquity 80 (308): 390–404. 2006. doi:10.1017/S0003598X00093704. 
  40. "Sexual Determination of Hand Stencils on the Main Panel of the Gua Masri II Cave (East-Kalimantan/Borneo – Indonesia)". International Newsletter on Rock Art 28: 16–22. 2008. https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-00342551. 
  41. "News Using the length of the 2nd to 4th digit ratio (2D:4D) to sex cave art hand stencils: Factors to consider". Before Farming 2006: 1–7. 2006. doi:10.3828/bfarm.2006.1.6. 
  42. "Differences in the 2nd to 4th digit length ratio in humans reflect shifts along the common allometric line". Biology Letters 5 (5): 643–646. October 2009. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2009.0346. PMID 19553247. 
  43. "Early prenatal attainment of adult metacarpal-phalangeal rankings and proportions". American Journal of Physical Anthropology 43 (3): 327–332. November 1975. doi:10.1002/ajpa.1330430305. PMID 1211429. 
  44. "Finger-length ratios show evidence of prenatal hormone-transfer between opposite-sex twins". Hormones and Behavior 49 (3): 315–319. March 2006. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2005.08.003. PMID 16143332. 
  45. "Cigarette smoking and testosterone in men and women: A systematic review and meta-analysis of observational studies". Preventive Medicine 85: 1–10. April 2016. doi:10.1016/j.ypmed.2015.12.021. PMID 26763163. 
  46. "Lowered testosterone in male obesity: mechanisms, morbidity and management" (in en-US). Asian Journal of Andrology 16 (2): 223–231. March 2014. doi:10.4103/1008-682X.122365. PMID 24407187. 
  47. "How Food Intakes Modify Testosterone Level". The Journal of Sexual Medicine 13 (9): 1292–1296. September 2016. doi:10.1016/j.jsxm.2016.07.001. PMID 27555502. 
  48. "Understanding COVID-19: Digit ratio (2D:4D) and sex differences in national case fatality rates". Early Human Development 146: 105074. July 2020. doi:10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2020.105074. PMID 32419720. 
  49. "No credible evidence for links between 2D:4D and COVID-19 outcomes: A probabilistic perspective on digit ratio, ACE variants, and national case fatalities". Early Human Development 152: 105272. January 2021. doi:10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2020.105272. PMID 33227636. 
  50. "Faculty Spotlight: James M. Smoliga | Tufts University School of Medicine". https://medicine.tufts.edu/news-events/news/faculty-spotlight-james-m-smoliga. 
  51. Manning, John T. (11 June 2023). "The Big Finger Trumps the Lucky Jim Fallacy" (in en). The BMJ. https://www.bmj.com/content/375/bmj-2021-067849/rr. 
  52. 52.0 52.1 "Sexual dimorphism in the prenatal digit ratio (2D:4D)". Archives of Sexual Behavior 39 (1): 57–62. February 2010. doi:10.1007/s10508-009-9485-7. PMID 19301112. 
  53. 53.0 53.1 "Digit ratio (2D:4D) and prenatal/perinatal sex hormones: A response to Manning and Fink (2017)". Early Human Development 113: 75–76. October 2017. doi:10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2017.09.004. PMID 28917583. https://eprint.ncl.ac.uk/fulltext.aspx?url=249530/77FA418E-6576-4475-A6B5-EEE33F7CAF94.pdf&pub_id=249530. 
  54. "The big finger: the second to fourth digit ratio is a predictor of sporting ability in women". British Journal of Sports Medicine 40 (12): 981–983. December 2006. doi:10.1136/bjsm.2006.027193. PMID 17008344. 
  55. "Genetic and environmental influences on 2D:4D finger length ratios: a study of monozygotic and dizygotic male and female twins". Archives of Sexual Behavior 37 (1): 112–118. February 2008. doi:10.1007/s10508-007-9272-2. PMID 18074216. 
  56. "Stability of proposed biomarkers of prenatal androgen exposure over the menstrual cycle". Journal of Developmental Origins of Health and Disease 6 (2): 149–157. April 2015. doi:10.1017/S2040174414000646. PMID 25584807. 
  57. "High fetal testosterone and sexually dimorphic cerebral networks in females". Cerebral Cortex 19 (5): 1167–1174. May 2009. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhn160. PMID 18854582. 
  58. Sex and sexuality. 1. Greenwood Publishing Group. 2006. p. 165. ISBN 9780313049194. https://books.google.com/books?id=KBi9aG0pQAkC&pg=PA165. 
  59. "Amniotic fluid concentrations of delta 5 and delta 4 steroids in fetuses with congenital adrenal hyperplasia due to 21 hydroxylase deficiency and in anencephalic fetuses". The Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism 51 (2): 223–229. August 1980. doi:10.1210/jcem-51-2-223. PMID 6447160. 
  60. "Prenatal dexamethasone treatment in pregnancies at risk for congenital adrenal hyperplasia due to 21-hydroxylase deficiency: effect on midgestational amniotic fluid steroid levels". The Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism 76 (1): 117–120. January 1993. doi:10.1210/jcem.76.1.8421074. PMID 8421074. 
  61. "Profiling steroid hormones in amniotic fluid of midpregnancy by routine stable isotope dilution/gas chromatography-mass spectrometry: reference values and concentrations in fetuses at risk for 21-hydroxylase deficiency". The Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism 84 (8): 2724–2728. August 1999. doi:10.1210/jcem.84.8.5870. PMID 10443667. 
  62. "The second to fourth digit ratio and variation in the androgen receptor gene". Evolution and Human Behavior 24 (6): 399–405. 2003. doi:10.1016/S1090-5138(03)00052-7. 
  63. "Re-examining the Manning hypothesis: androgen receptor polymorphism and the 2D:4D ratio". Evol Hum Behav 33 (5): 557–561. 2012. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2012.02.003. 
  64. "Part of the interindividual variation in serum testosterone levels in healthy men reflects differences in androgen sensitivity and feedback set point: contribution of the androgen receptor polyglutamine tract polymorphism". The Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism 92 (9): 3604–3610. September 2007. doi:10.1210/jc.2007-0117. PMID 17579205. 
  65. "Fingers as a marker of prenatal androgen exposure". Endocrinology 150 (11): 5119–5124. November 2009. doi:10.1210/en.2009-0774. PMID 19819951. 
  66. "No effects of prenatal hormone transfer on digit ratio in a large sample of same- and opposite-sex dizygotic twins" (in en). Personality and Individual Differences 44 (5): 1225–1234. 1 April 2008. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2007.11.017. ISSN 0191-8869. https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0191886907004230. 
  67. "HOX gene links limb, genital defects". Science 275 (5306): 1568. March 1997. doi:10.1126/science.275.5306.1568. PMID 9072822. 
  68. "Meta-analysis of digit ratio 2D:4D shows greater sex difference in the right hand". American Journal of Human Biology 22 (5): 619–630. 2010. doi:10.1002/ajhb.21054. PMID 20737609. 
  69. "A comparison of measurement methods and sexual dimorphism for digit ratio (2D:4D) in Han ethnicity". Archives of Sexual Behavior 43 (2): 329–333. February 2014. doi:10.1007/s10508-013-0179-9. PMID 24013635. 
  70. "Radiographic assessment of the index to ring finger ratio (2D:4D) in adults". Journal of Anatomy 212 (1): 42–48. January 2008. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7580.2007.00830.x. PMID 18005121. 
  71. "The effects of hand preference and sex on right-left asymmetry in dorsal digit lengths among adults and children". Early Human Development 153: 105293. February 2021. doi:10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2020.105293. PMID 33340946. 
  72. "The 2nd:4th digit ratio, sexual dimorphism, population differences, and reproductive success. evidence for sexually antagonistic genes?". Evolution and Human Behavior 21 (3): 163–183. May 2000. doi:10.1016/S1090-5138(00)00029-5. PMID 10828555. 
  73. 73.0 73.1 "Sex and ethnic differences in 2nd to 4th digit ratio of children". Early Human Development 80 (2): 161–168. November 2004. doi:10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2004.06.004. PMID 15500996. 
  74. "Finger-length ratios and sexual orientation". Nature 404 (6777): 455–456. March 2000. doi:10.1038/35006555. PMID 10761903. Bibcode2000Natur.404..455W. 
  75. "Study on the digit ratio of Hui and Han ethnic groups in Ningxia.". Acta Anatomica Sinica 39 (2): 267. 2008. http://en.cnki.com.cn/Article_en/CJFDTOTAL-JPXB200802033.htm. Retrieved 7 November 2016. 
  76. "The effects of sex, ethnicity, and sexual orientation on self-measured digit ratio (2D:4D)". Archives of Sexual Behavior 36 (2): 223–233. April 2007. doi:10.1007/s10508-007-9171-6. PMID 17373585. 
  77. "Population differences in finger-length ratios: ethnicity or latitude?". Archives of Sexual Behavior 35 (6): 739–742. December 2006. doi:10.1007/s10508-006-9039-1. PMID 16688484. 
  78. "The digit ratio (2D:4D) in China: A meta-analysis". American Journal of Human Biology 27 (3): 304–309. 6 May 2015. doi:10.1002/ajhb.22639. PMID 25284473. 
  79. "Inbreeding is associated with lower 2D: 4D digit ratio". American Journal of Human Biology 26 (2): 183–188. 2014. doi:10.1002/ajhb.22491. PMID 24288299. 
  80. "Consanguinity and reproductive health among Arabs". Reproductive Health 6: 17. October 2009. doi:10.1186/1742-4755-6-17. PMID 19811666. 
  81. "The ratio of second to fourth digit lengths: a marker of impaired fetal growth?". Early Human Development 68 (1): 21–26. June 2002. doi:10.1016/s0378-3782(02)00009-9. PMID 12191526. 
  82. "Effects of hand preference on digit lengths and digit ratios among children and adults". Early Human Development 151: 105204. December 2020. doi:10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2020.105204. PMID 33059164. https://epublications.marquette.edu/math_fac/56. 
  83. "Second to fourth digit ratio and face shape". Proceedings. Biological Sciences 272 (1576): 1995–2001. October 2005. doi:10.1098/rspb.2005.3179. PMID 16191608. 
  84. "Second to fourth digit ratio and facial asymmetry". Evolution and Human Behavior 25 (2): 125–32. March 2004. doi:10.1016/S1090-5138(03)00084-9. 
  85. 85.0 85.1 85.2 85.3 "Which Data to Meta-Analyze, and How?". Zeitschrift für Psychologie (Hogrefe Publishing Group) 227 (1): 64–82. 2019. doi:10.1027/2151-2604/a000357. ISSN 2190-8370. 
  86. 86.0 86.1 86.2 "No credible evidence for links between 2D:4D and COVID-19 outcomes: A probabilistic perspective on digit ratio, ACE variants, and national case fatalities". Early Human Development (Elsevier BV) 152: 105272. January 2021. doi:10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2020.105272. PMID 33227636. 
  87. "Digit ratio (2D:4D) and sex-role orientation: Further evidence and meta-analysis". Personality and Individual Differences (Elsevier BV) 51 (4): 417–422. September 2011. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2010.06.009. ISSN 0191-8869. 
  88. 88.0 88.1 "Facing off with the phalangeal phenomenon and editorial policies: A commentary on Swift-Gallant, Johnson, Di Rita and Breedlove (2020)". Hormones and Behavior (Elsevier BV) 120: 104710. April 2020. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2020.104710. PMID 32057822. 
  89. "A comprehensive allometric analysis of 2nd digit length to 4th digit length in humans". Proceedings. Biological Sciences 284 (1857): 20170356. June 2017. doi:10.1098/rspb.2017.0356. PMID 28659446. 
  90. "Adult digit ratio (2D:4D) is not related to umbilical cord androgen or estrogen concentrations, their ratios or net bioactivity". Early Human Development (Elsevier BV) 91 (2): 111–117. February 2015. doi:10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2014.12.011. PMID 25594498. https://research-repository.uwa.edu.au/en/publications/b3d44281-0fcf-499c-9743-7daac128c486. 
  91. 91.0 91.1 "Interpreting digit ratio (2D:4D)-behavior correlations: 2D:4D sex difference, stability, and behavioral correlates and their replicability in young children". Hormones and Behavior (Elsevier BV) 78: 86–94. February 2016. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2015.10.022. PMID 26542674. 
  92. "Digit ratio (2D:4D) and Cattell's personality traits". European Journal of Personality (SAGE Publications) 22 (4): 347–356. June 2008. doi:10.1002/per.664. ISSN 0890-2070. 
  93. "Does digit ratio (2D:4D) predict penile length?". Asian Journal of Andrology (Medknow) 13 (5): 667–668. September 2011. doi:10.1038/aja.2011.81. PMID 21725331. 
  94. "2D:4D and Self-Employment: A Preregistered Replication Study in a Large General Population Sample". Entrepreneurship Theory and Practice 46 (1): 21–43. 5 January 2021. doi:10.1177/1042258720985478. ISSN 1042-2587. 
  95. "A variant in LIN28B is associated with 2D:4D finger-length ratio, a putative retrospective biomarker of prenatal testosterone exposure". American Journal of Human Genetics 86 (4): 519–525. April 2010. doi:10.1016/j.ajhg.2010.02.017. PMID 20303062. 
  96. "Revisiting the Criminological Consequences of Exposure to Fetal Testosterone: A Meta-Analysis of the 2D:4D Digit Ratio". Criminology (Wiley) 54 (4): 587–620. 22 September 2016. doi:10.1111/1745-9125.12115. ISSN 0011-1384. 
  97. "Null Effects of Game Violence, Game Difficulty, and 2D:4D Digit Ratio on Aggressive Behavior". Psychological Science (SAGE Publications) 30 (4): 606–616. April 2019. doi:10.1177/0956797619829688. PMID 30843758. 
  98. "Evolution of Primate Social Systems: Implications for Hominin Social Evolution". Lucy to Language: The Benchmark Papers. OUP Oxford. 2014. pp. 317–405. ISBN 978-0-19-965259-4. https://books.google.com/books?id=VEakAgAAQBAJ&pg=PA317. 
  99. "An eye for detail: selective sexual imprinting in zebra finches". Evolution; International Journal of Organic Evolution 60 (5): 1076–1085. May 2006. doi:10.1554/05-399.1. PMID 16817546. 
  100. "Testosterone enhancement during pregnancy influences the 2D:4D ratio and open field motor activity of rat siblings in adulthood". Hormones and Behavior 55 (1): 235–239. January 2009. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2008.10.010. PMID 19022257. 
  101. "Selective breeding for a behavioral trait changes digit ratio". PLOS ONE 3 (9): e3216. September 2008. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0003216. PMID 18797502. Bibcode2008PLoSO...3.3216Y. 
  102. "Examination marks of male university students positively correlate with finger length ratios (2D:4D)". Biological Psychology 71 (2): 175–182. February 2006. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.03.006. PMID 15978716. 

External links