Biology:Proprioception

From HandWiki
Revision as of 21:04, 10 February 2024 by John Stpola (talk | contribs) (add)
(diff) ← Older revision | Latest revision (diff) | Newer revision → (diff)
Short description: Sense of self-movement, force, and body position
Schematics and images of limb proprioceptors in mammals (top) and insects (bottom)[clarification needed]

Proprioception (/ˌprpri.ˈsɛpʃən, -ə-/[1][2] PROH-pree-oh-SEP-shən, -⁠ə-) is the sense of self-movement, force, and body position.[3][4]

Proprioception is mediated by proprioceptors, mechanosensory neurons located within muscles, tendons, and joints.[3] Most animals possess multiple subtypes of proprioceptors, which detect distinct kinematic parameters, such as joint position, movement, and load. Although all mobile animals possess proprioceptors, the structure of the sensory organs can vary across species.

Proprioceptive signals are transmitted to the central nervous system, where they are integrated with information from other sensory systems, such as the visual system and the vestibular system, to create an overall representation of body position, movement, and acceleration. In many animals, sensory feedback from proprioceptors is essential for stabilizing body posture and coordinating body movement.

System overview

In vertebrates, limb movement and velocity (muscle length and the rate of change) are encoded by one group of sensory neurons (type Ia sensory fiber) and another type encode static muscle length (group II neurons).[5] These two types of sensory neurons compose muscle spindles. There is a similar division of encoding in invertebrates; different subgroups of neurons of the Chordotonal organ[6] encode limb position and velocity.

To determine the load on a limb, vertebrates use sensory neurons in the Golgi tendon organs:[7] type Ib afferents. These proprioceptors are activated at given muscle forces, which indicate the resistance that muscle is experiencing. Similarly, invertebrates have a mechanism to determine limb load: the Campaniform sensilla.[8] These proprioceptors are active when a limb experiences resistance.[citation needed]

A third role for proprioceptors is to determine when a joint is at a specific position. In vertebrates, this is accomplished by Ruffini endings and Pacinian corpuscles. These proprioceptors are activated when the joint is at a threshold position, usually at the extremes of joint position. Invertebrates use hair plates[9] to accomplish this; a field of bristles located within joints that detects the relative movement of limb segments through the deflection of the associated cuticular hairs.

Reflexes

The sense of proprioception is ubiquitous across mobile animals and is essential for the motor coordination of the body. Proprioceptors can form reflex circuits with motor neurons to provide rapid feedback about body and limb position. These mechanosensory circuits are important for flexibly maintaining posture and balance, especially during locomotion. For example, consider the stretch reflex, in which stretch across a muscle is detected by a sensory receptor (e.g., muscle spindle, chordotonal neurons), which activates a motor neuron to induce muscle contraction and oppose the stretch. During locomotion, sensory neurons can reverse their activity when stretched, to promote rather than oppose movement.[10][11]

Conscious and nonconscious

In humans, a distinction is made between conscious proprioception and nonconscious proprioception:

  • Conscious proprioception is communicated by the dorsal column-medial lemniscus pathway to the cerebrum.[12]
  • Nonconscious proprioception is communicated primarily via the dorsal spinocerebellar tract[13] and ventral spinocerebellar tract,[14] to the cerebellum.
  • A nonconscious reaction is seen in the human proprioceptive reflex, or righting reflex—in the event that the body tilts in any direction, the person will cock their head back to level the eyes against the horizon.[15] This is seen even in infants as soon as they gain control of their neck muscles. This control comes from the cerebellum, the part of the brain affecting balance.[citation needed]

Mechanisms

Proprioception is mediated by mechanically sensitive proprioceptor neurons distributed throughout an animal's body. Most vertebrates possess three basic types of proprioceptors: muscle spindles, which are embedded in skeletal muscles, Golgi tendon organs, which lie at the interface of muscles and tendons, and joint receptors, which are low-threshold mechanoreceptors embedded in joint capsules. Many invertebrates, such as insects, also possess three basic proprioceptor types with analogous functional properties: chordotonal neurons, campaniform sensilla, and hair plates.[3]

The initiation of proprioception is the activation of a proprioceptor in the periphery.[16] The proprioceptive sense is believed to be composed of information from sensory neurons located in the inner ear (motion and orientation) and in the stretch receptors located in the muscles and the joint-supporting ligaments (stance). There are specific nerve receptors for this form of perception termed "proprioceptors", just as there are specific receptors for pressure, light, temperature, sound, and other sensory experiences. Proprioceptors are sometimes known as adequate stimuli receptors.[citation needed]

Members of the transient receptor potential family of ion channels have been found to be important for proprioception in fruit flies,[17] nematode worms,[18] African clawed frogs,[19] and zebrafish.[20] PIEZO2, a nonselective cation channel, has been shown to underlie the mechanosensitivity of proprioceptors in mice.[21] Humans with loss-of-function mutations in the PIEZO2 gene exhibit specific deficits in joint proprioception,[lower-alpha 1] as well as vibration and touch discrimination, suggesting that the PIEZO2 channel is essential for mechanosensitivity in some proprioceptors and low-threshold mechanoreceptors.[23]

Although it was known that finger kinesthesia relies on skin sensation, recent research has found that kinesthesia-based haptic perception relies strongly on the forces experienced during touch.[24] This research allows the creation of "virtual", illusory haptic shapes with different perceived qualities.[25]

Anatomy

Proprioception of the head stems from the muscles innervated by the trigeminal nerve, where the general somatic afferent fibers pass without synapsing in the trigeminal ganglion (first-order sensory neuron), reaching the mesencephalic tract and the mesencephalic nucleus of trigeminal nerve.[26] Proprioception of limbs often occurs due to receptors in connective tissue near joints.[27]

Function

Stability

An important role for proprioception is to allow an animal to stabilize itself against perturbations.[28] For instance, for a person to walk or stand upright, they must continuously monitor their posture and adjust muscle activity as needed to provide balance. Similarly, when walking on unfamiliar terrain or even tripping, the person must adjust the output of their muscles quickly based on estimated limb position and velocity. Proprioceptor reflex circuits are thought to play an important role to allow fast and unconscious execution of these behaviors, To make control of these behaviors efficient, proprioceptors are also thought to regulate reciprocal inhibition in muscles, leading to agonist-antagonist muscle pairs.

Planning and refining movements

When planning complex movements such as reaching or grooming, an animal must consider the current position and velocity of its limb and use that information to adjust dynamics to target a final position. If the animal's estimate of its limb's initial position is wrong, then a deficiency in the movement can result. Furthermore, proprioception is crucial in refining the movement if it deviates from the trajectory.

Development

In adult fruit flies, each proprioceptor class arises from a specific cell lineage (i.e. each chordotonal neuron is from the chordotonal neuron lineage, although multiple lineages give rise to sensory bristles). After the last cell division, proprioceptors send out axons toward the central nervous system and are guided by hormonal gradients to reach stereotyped synapses. [29] The mechanisms underlying axon guidance are similar across invertebrates and vertebrates.[citation needed]

In mammals with longer gestation periods, muscle spindles are fully formed at birth. Muscle spindles continue to grow throughout post-natal development as muscles grow. [30]

Mathematical models

Proprioceptors transfer the mechanical state of the body into patterns of neural activity. This transfer can be modeled mathematically, for example to better understand the internal workings of a proprioceptor[31][32][33] or to provide more realistic feedback in neuromechanical simulations.[34][35]

Various proprioceptor models of complexity have been developed. They range from simple phenomenological models to complex structural models, in which the mathematical elements correspond to anatomical features of the proprioceptor. The focus has been on muscle spindles,[31][32][33][36] but Golgi tendon organs[37][38] and insects' hair plates[39] have been modeled too.

Muscle spindles

Poppelle and Bowman [40] used linear system theory to model mammalian muscle spindles Ia and II afferents. They obtained a set of de-afferented muscle spindles, measured their response to a series of sinusoidal and step function stretches, and fit a transfer function to the spike rate. They found that the following Laplace transfer function describes the firing rate responses of the primary sensory fibers for a change in length:

[math]\displaystyle{ H(s) = K_1 \frac{s(s+0.44)(s+11.3)(s+44)}{(s + 0.04)(s+0.816)} }[/math]

The following equation describes the response of secondary sensory fibers:

[math]\displaystyle{ H(s) = K_2 \frac{(s+0.44)(s+11.3)}{s+0.816} }[/math]

More recently, Blum et al.[41] showed that the muscle spindle firing rate is modeled better as tracking the force of the muscle, rather than the length. Furthermore, muscle spindle firing rates show history dependence which cannot be modeled by a linear time-invariant system model.

Golgi tendon organs

Houk and Simon [38] provided one of the first mathematical models of a Golgi tendon organ receptor, modeling the firing rate of the receptor as a function of the muscle tension force. Just as for muscle spindles, they find that, as the receptors respond linearly to sine waves of different frequencies and has little variance in response over time to the same stimulus, Golgi tendon organ receptors may be modeled as linear time-invariant systems. Specifically, they find that the firing rate of a Golgi tendon organ receptor may be modeled as a sum of 3 decaying exponentials:

[math]\displaystyle{ r(t) = K[1 + A\exp(-at) + B \exp(-b t) + C \exp(-c t)] u(t) }[/math]

where [math]\displaystyle{ r(t) }[/math] is the firing rate and [math]\displaystyle{ u(t) }[/math] is a step function of force.

The corresponding Laplace transfer function for this system is:

[math]\displaystyle{ H(s) = K\left(1 + \frac{A s }{s + a} + \frac{Bs}{s+b} + \frac{Cs}{s + c} \right) }[/math]

For a soleus receptor, Houk and Simon obtain average values of K=57 pulses/sec/kg, A=0.31, a=0.22 sec−1, B=0.4, b=2.17 sec−1, C=2.5, c=36 sec−1 .

When modeling a stretch reflex, Lin and Crago[42] improved upon this model by adding a logarithmic nonlinearity before the Houk and Simon model and a threshold nonlinearity after.

Impairment

Chronic

Proprioception, a sense vital for rapid and proper body coordination,[43] can be permanently lost or impaired as a result of genetic conditions, disease, viral infections, and injuries. For instance, patients with joint hypermobility or Ehlers–Danlos syndromes, genetic conditions that result in weak connective tissue throughout the body, have chronic impairments to proprioception.[44] Moreover, proprioception may be chronically impaired in physiological aging (presbypropria),[45] autism spectrum disorder,[46] and Parkinson's disease.[47] In regards to Parkinson's disease, it remains unclear whether the proprioceptive-related decline in motor function occurs due to disrupted proprioceptors in the periphery or signaling in the spinal cord or brain.

In rare cases, viral infections result in a loss of proprioception. Ian Waterman and Charles Freed are two such people that lost their sense of proprioception from the neck down from supposed viral infections (i.e. gastric flu and a rare viral infection). After losing their sense of proprioception, Ian and Charles could move their lower body, but could not coordinate their movements. However, both individuals regained some control of their limbs and body by consciously planning their movements and relying solely on visual feedback. Interestingly, both individuals can still sense pain and temperature, indicating that they specifically lost proprioceptive feedback, but not tactile and nociceptive feedback. The impact of losing the sense of proprioception on daily life is perfectly illustrated when Ian Waterman stated, "What is an active brain without mobility".[48][49]

Proprioception is also permanently lost in people who lose a limb or body part through injury or amputation. After the removal of a limb, people may have a confused sense of that limb's existence on their body, known as phantom limb syndrome. Phantom sensations can occur as passive proprioceptive sensations of the limb's presence, or more active sensations such as perceived movement, pressure, pain, itching, or temperature. There are a variety of theories concerning the etiology of phantom limb sensations and experience. One is the concept of "proprioceptive memory", which argues that the brain retains a memory of specific limb positions and that after amputation there is a conflict between the visual system, which actually sees that the limb is missing, and the memory system which remembers the limb as a functioning part of the body.[50] Phantom sensations and phantom pain may also occur after the removal of body parts other than the limbs, such as after amputation of the breast, extraction of a tooth (phantom tooth pain), or removal of an eye (phantom eye syndrome).

Acute

Proprioception is occasionally impaired spontaneously, especially when one is tired. Similar effects can be felt during the hypnagogic state of consciousness, during the onset of sleep. One's body may feel too large or too small, or parts of the body may feel distorted in size. Similar effects can sometimes occur during epilepsy or migraine auras. These effects are presumed to arise from abnormal stimulation of the part of the parietal cortex of the brain involved with integrating information from different parts of the body.[51] Proprioceptive illusions can also be induced, such as the Pinocchio illusion.

Temporary impairment of proprioception has also been known to occur from an overdose of vitamin B6 (pyridoxine and pyridoxamine)[citation needed]. Most of the impaired function returns to normal shortly after the amount of the vitamin in the body returns to a level that is closer to that of the physiological norm. Impairment can also be caused by cytotoxic factors such as chemotherapy.

It has been proposed that even common tinnitus and the attendant hearing frequency-gaps masked by the perceived sounds may cause erroneous proprioceptive information to the balance and comprehension centers of the brain, precipitating mild confusion.

Temporary loss or impairment of proprioception may happen periodically during growth, mostly during adolescence. Growth that might also influence this would be large increases or drops in bodyweight/size due to fluctuations of fat (liposuction, rapid fat loss or gain) and/or muscle content (bodybuilding, anabolic steroids, catabolisis/starvation)[citation needed]. It can also occur in those that gain new levels of flexibility, stretching, and contortion. A limb's being in a new range of motion never experienced (or at least, not for a long time since youth perhaps) can disrupt one's sense of location of that limb. Possible experiences include suddenly feeling that feet or legs are missing from one's mental self-image; needing to look down at one's limbs to be sure they are still there; and falling down while walking, especially when attention is focused upon something other than the act of walking.

Diagnosis

Impaired proprioception may be diagnosed through a series of tests, each focusing on a different functional aspect of proprioception.

The Romberg's test is often used to assess balance. The subject must stand with feet together and eyes closed without support for 30 seconds. If the subject loses balance and falls, it is an indicator for impaired proprioception.

For evaluating proprioception's contribution to motor control, a common protocol is joint position matching.[52] The patient is blindfolded while a joint is moved to a specific angle for a given period of time and then returned to neutral. The subject is then asked to move the joint back to the specified angle. Recent investigations have shown that hand dominance, participant age, active versus passive matching, and presentation time of the angle can all affect performance on joint position matching tasks.[citation needed]

For passive sensing of joint angles, recent studies have found that experiments to probe psychophysical thresholds produce more precise estimates of proprioceptive discrimination than the joint position matching task.[53] In these experiments, the subject holds on to an object (such as an armrest) that moves and stops at different positions. The subject must discriminate whether one position is closer to the body than another. From the subject's choices, the tester may determine the subject's discrimination thresholds.

Proprioception is tested by American police officers using the field sobriety testing to check for alcohol intoxication. The subject is required to touch his or her nose with eyes closed; people with normal proprioception may make an error of no more than 20 mm (0.79 in)[citation needed], while people with impaired proprioception (a symptom of moderate to severe alcohol intoxication) fail this test due to difficulty locating their limbs in space relative to their noses.

Training

Proprioception is what allows someone to learn to walk in complete darkness without losing balance. During the learning of any new skill, sport, or art, it is usually necessary to become familiar with some proprioceptive tasks specific to that activity. Without the appropriate integration of proprioceptive input, an artist would not be able to brush paint onto a canvas without looking at the hand as it moved the brush over the canvas; it would be impossible to drive an automobile because a motorist would not be able to steer or use the pedals while looking at the road ahead; a person could not touch type or perform ballet; and people would not even be able to walk without watching where they put their feet.[citation needed]

Oliver Sacks reported the case of a young woman who lost her proprioception due to a viral infection of her spinal cord.[54] At first she could not move properly at all or even control her tone of voice (as voice modulation is primarily proprioceptive). Later she relearned by using her sight (watching her feet) and inner ear only for movement while using hearing to judge voice modulation. She eventually acquired a stiff and slow movement and nearly normal speech, which is believed to be the best possible in the absence of this sense. She could not judge effort involved in picking up objects and would grip them painfully to be sure she did not drop them.

File:Trabajo propioceptivo en miembro inferior.webm The proprioceptive sense can be sharpened through study of many disciplines. Juggling trains reaction time, spatial location, and efficient movement.[citation needed] Standing on a wobble board or balance board is often used to retrain or increase proprioceptive abilities, particularly as physical therapy for ankle or knee injuries. Slacklining is another method to increase proprioception.

Standing on one leg (stork standing) and various other body-position challenges are also used in such disciplines as yoga, Wing Chun and tai chi.[55] The vestibular system of the inner ear, vision and proprioception are the main three requirements for balance.[56] Moreover, there are specific devices designed for proprioception training, such as the exercise ball, which works on balancing the abdominal and back muscles.

History of study

In 1557, the position-movement sensation was described by Julius Caesar Scaliger as a "sense of locomotion".[57]

In 1826, Charles Bell expounded the idea of a "muscle sense",[58] which is credited as one of the first descriptions of physiologic feedback mechanisms.[59] Bell's idea was that commands are carried from the brain to the muscles, and that reports on the muscle's condition would be sent in the reverse direction.

In 1847, the London neurologist Robert Todd highlighted important differences in the anterolateral and posterior columns of the spinal cord, and suggested that the latter were involved in the coordination of movement and balance.[60]

At around the same time, Moritz Heinrich Romberg, a Berlin neurologist, was describing unsteadiness made worse by eye closure or darkness, now known as the eponymous Romberg's sign, once synonymous with tabes dorsalis, that became recognised as common to all proprioceptive disorders of the legs.[citation needed]

In 1880, Henry Charlton Bastian suggested "kinaesthesia" instead of "muscle sense" on the basis that some of the afferent information (back to the brain) comes from other structures, including tendons, joints, and skin.[61]

In 1889, Alfred Goldscheider suggested a classification of kinaesthesia into three types: muscle, tendon, and articular sensitivity.[62]

In 1906, the term proprio-ception (and also intero-ception and extero-ception) is attested in a publication by Charles Scott Sherrington involving receptors.[63] He explains the terminology as follows:[64]

The main fields of distribution of the receptor organs fundamentally distinguishable seem, therefore, to be two, namely, a surface field constituted by the surface layer of the organism, and a deep field constituted by the tissues of the organism beneath the surface sheet.
[...]
the stimulations occurring in [the] deep field is that the stimuli are traceable to actions of the organism itself, and are so in much greater measure than are the stimulations of the surface field of the organism. Since in the deep field the stimuli to the receptors are delivered by the organism itself,[lower-alpha 2] the deep receptors may be termed proprio-ceptors, and the deep field a field of proprio-ception.

Today, the "exteroceptors" are the organs that provide information originating outside the body, such as the eyes, ears, mouth, and skin. The interoceptors provide information about the internal organs, and the "proprioceptors" provide information about movement derived from muscular, tendon, and articular sources. Using Sherrington's system, physiologists and anatomists search for specialised nerve endings that transmit mechanical data on joint capsule, tendon and muscle tension (such as Golgi tendon organs and muscle spindles), which play a large role in proprioception.[citation needed]

Primary endings of muscle spindles "respond to the size of a muscle length change and its speed" and "contribute both to the sense of limb position and movement".[65] Secondary endings of muscle spindles detect changes in muscle length, and thus supply information regarding only the sense of position.[65] Essentially, muscle spindles are stretch receptors.[66] It has been accepted that cutaneous receptors also contribute directly to proprioception by providing "accurate perceptual information about joint position and movement", and this knowledge is combined with information from the muscle spindles.[67]

Etymology

Proprioception is from Latin proprius, meaning "one's own", "individual", and capio, capere, to take or grasp. Thus to grasp one's own position in space, including the position of the limbs in relation to each other and the body as a whole.[citation needed]

The word kinesthesia or kinæsthesia (kinesthetic sense) refers to movement sense, but has been used inconsistently to refer either to proprioception alone or to the brain's integration of proprioceptive and vestibular inputs. Kinesthesia is a modern medical term composed of elements from Greek; kinein "to set in motion; to move" (from PIE root *keie- "to set in motion") + aisthesis "perception, feeling" (from PIE root *au- "to perceive").

Plants and bacteria

Although they lack neurons, systems responding to stimuli (analogous to the sensory system in animals with a nervous system, which includes the proprioception) have also been described in some plants (angiosperms).[68][69] Terrestrial plants control the orientation of their primary growth through the sensing of several vectorial stimuli such as the light gradient or the gravitational acceleration. This control has been called tropism. A quantitative study of shoot gravitropism demonstrated that, when a plant is tilted, it cannot recover a steady erected posture under the sole driving of the sensing of its angular deflection versus gravity. An additional control through the continuous sensing of its curvature by the organ and the subsequent driving an active straightening process are required.[68][69][70] Being a sensing by the plant of the relative configuration of its parts, it has been called proprioception. This dual sensing and control by gravisensing and proprioception has been formalized into a unifying mathematical model simulating the complete driving of the gravitropic movement. This model has been validated on 11 species sampling the phylogeny of land angiosperms, and on organs of very contrasted sizes, ranging from the small germination of wheat (coleoptile) to the trunk of poplar trees.[68][69]

Further studies have shown that the cellular mechanism of proprioception in plants involves myosin and actin, and seems to occur in specialized cells.[71] Proprioception was then found to be involved in other tropisms and to be central also to the control of nutation.[72]

The discovery of proprioception in plants has generated an interest in the popular science and generalist media.[73][74] This is because this discovery questions a long-lasting a priori that we have on plants. In some cases this has led to a shift between proprioception and self-awareness or self-consciousness. There is no scientific ground for such a semantic shift. Indeed, even in animals, proprioception can be unconscious; so it is thought to be in plants.[69][74]

Recent studies suggest that bacteria have control systems that may resemble proprioception.[75]

See also


Notes

  1. The Piezo channel receptors play key roles in the perception of pressure, touch, and proprioception (Piezo2 receptor).[22]
  2. in Latin: propriō

References

  1. "Proprioception". Merriam-Webster Dictionary. https://www.merriam-webster.com/dictionary/proprioception. 
  2. "proprioceptive". OxfordDictionaries.com. https://www.oxforddictionaries.com/definition/english/proprioceptive. 
  3. 3.0 3.1 3.2 "Proprioception". Current Biology 28 (5): R194–R203. 1 March 2018. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2018.01.064. PMID 29510103. 
  4. Balasubramanian, Ravi; Santos, Veronica (3 January 2014). The Human Hand as an Inspiration for Robot Hand Development. Springer. p. 127. ISBN 978-3-319-03017-3. https://books.google.com/books?id=h3u5BQAAQBAJ&pg=PA127. "Proprioception also includes the ability to perceive force and heaviness, the history of which has been less controversial than the senses of limb position and movement. The sense of force refers to the ability to perceive the force that is generated by the muscles and its primary receptor is the Golgi tendon organ." 
  5. "Role of joint afferents in motor control exemplified by effects on reflex pathways from Ib afferents". The Journal of Physiology 284: 327–43. November 1978. doi:10.1113/jphysiol.1978.sp012543. PMID 215758. 
  6. "Proprioception by the Coxo-Basal Chordotonal Organ, Cb, in Legs of the Crab, Carcinus Maenas". The Journal of Experimental Biology 42 (2): 285–97. April 1965. doi:10.1242/jeb.42.2.285. PMID 14323766. http://jeb.biologists.org/content/42/2/285.long. 
  7. "Afferent-efferent linkages in motor cortex for single forelimb muscles". Journal of Neurophysiology 38 (4): 990–1014. July 1975. doi:10.1152/jn.1975.38.4.990. PMID 125786. 
  8. "Campaniform Sensilla on the Tactile Spines of the Legs of the Cockroach". The Journal of Experimental Biology 42 (2): 191–203. April 1965. doi:10.1242/jeb.42.2.191. PMID 14323763. 
  9. "Distribution and specific central projections of mechanoreceptors in the thorax and proximal leg joints of locusts. I. Morphology, location and innervation of internal proprioceptors of pro- and metathorax and their central projections". Cell and Tissue Research 216 (1): 57–77. 1981. doi:10.1007/bf00234545. PMID 7226209. 
  10. "Pattern generation for stick insect walking movements--multisensory control of a locomotor program". Brain Research. Brain Research Reviews 27 (1): 65–88. June 1998. doi:10.1016/S0165-0173(98)00006-X. PMID 9639677. 
  11. "Mechanosensation and Adaptive Motor Control in Insects". Current Biology 26 (20): R1022–R1038. October 2016. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2016.06.070. PMID 27780045. 
  12. Neuroanatomy. Hagerstown, MD: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. 2002. pp. 127. ISBN 978-0-7817-2829-4. https://archive.org/details/neuroanatomy0003fixj. 
  13. "Review of Clinical and Functional Neuroscience, Chapter 7A: Somatosensory Systems". (online version Dartmouth college). http://www.dartmouth.edu/~rswenson/NeuroSci/chapter_7A.html#Unconscious_sensation. 
  14. Siegel, Allan (2010). Essential Neuroscience. Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. p. 263. 
  15. "TMJ, Forward Head Posture and Neck Pain". Freedom From Pain Institute. http://erikdalton.com/tmj-forward-head-posture-neck-pain/. 
  16. "On the proprioceptive system, especially in its reflex aspect". Brain 29 (4): 467–85. 1907. doi:10.1093/brain/29.4.467. 
  17. "A Drosophila mechanosensory transduction channel". Science 287 (5461): 2229–34. March 2000. doi:10.1126/science.287.5461.2229. PMID 10744543. Bibcode2000Sci...287.2229W. 
  18. "A C. elegans stretch receptor neuron revealed by a mechanosensitive TRP channel homologue". Nature 440 (7084): 684–87. March 2006. doi:10.1038/nature04538. PMID 16572173. Bibcode2006Natur.440..684L. 
  19. "Xenopus TRPN1 (NOMPC) localizes to microtubule-based cilia in epithelial cells, including inner-ear hair cells". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 102 (35): 12572–77. August 2005. doi:10.1073/pnas.0502403102. PMID 16116094. Bibcode2005PNAS..10212572S. 
  20. "NompC TRP channel required for vertebrate sensory hair cell mechanotransduction". Science 301 (5629): 96–99. July 2003. doi:10.1126/science.1084370. PMID 12805553. Bibcode2003Sci...301...96S. 
  21. "Piezo2 is the principal mechanotransduction channel for proprioception". Nature Neuroscience 18 (12): 1756–62. December 2015. doi:10.1038/nn.4162. PMID 26551544. 
  22. The Nobel Assembly at Karolinska Institutet (4 Oct 2021) Press release: The Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine 2021 The Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine 2021: David Julius, and Ardem Patapoutian
  23. "The Role of PIEZO2 in Human Mechanosensation". The New England Journal of Medicine 375 (14): 1355–64. October 2016. doi:10.1056/NEJMoa1602812. PMID 27653382. 
  24. "Force can overcome object geometry in the perception of shape through active touch". Nature 412 (6845): 445–48. July 2001. doi:10.1038/35086588. PMID 11473320. Bibcode2001Natur.412..445R. http://www.roblesdelatorre.com/gabriel/GR-VH-Nature2001.pdf. Retrieved 2006-10-03. 
  25. the MIT Technology Review article "The Cutting Edge of Haptics"
  26. Orhan E. Arslan (7 August 2014). Neuroanatomical Basis of Clinical Neurology (2 ed.). CRC Press. pp. 432–. ISBN 978-1-4398-4834-0. https://books.google.com/books?id=NCPOBQAAQBAJ&pg=PA432. 
  27. van der Wal, Jaap (7 December 2009). "The Architecture of the Connective Tissue in the Musculoskeletal System—An Often Overlooked Functional Parameter as to Proprioception in the Locomotor Apparatus". International Journal of Therapeutic Massage & Bodywork 2 (4): 9–23. doi:10.3822/ijtmb.v2i4.62. ISSN 1916-257X. PMID 21589740. 
  28. David J. Magee; James E. Zachazewski; William S. Quillen (18 September 2008). Pathology and Intervention in Musculoskeletal Rehabilitation - E-Book. Elsevier Health Sciences. pp. 533–. ISBN 978-1-4160-6942-3. OCLC 1017975705. https://books.google.com/books?id=xUFqHSwrXiQC&pg=PA533. 
  29. Jan, Y. N. and Jan, L. Y. (1993). The peripheral nervous system. In: The Development of Drosophila melanogaster (ed. Bate, M and Arias, A. M.), pp. 1207–44. New York: Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press.
  30. "Development and regeneration of muscle spindles in mammals and birds". The International Journal of Developmental Biology 41 (1): 1–17. February 1997. PMID 9074933. 
  31. 31.0 31.1 "Force encoding in muscle spindles during stretch of passive muscle". PLOS Computational Biology 13 (9): e1005767. September 2017. doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1005767. PMID 28945740. Bibcode2017PLSCB..13E5767B. 
  32. 32.0 32.1 "Mathematical models of proprioceptors. I. Control and transduction in the muscle spindle". Journal of Neurophysiology 96 (4): 1772–88. October 2006. doi:10.1152/jn.00868.2005. PMID 16672301. 
  33. 33.0 33.1 "Spindle model responsive to mixed fusimotor inputs and testable predictions of beta feedback effects". Journal of Neurophysiology 89 (5): 2797–809. May 2003. doi:10.1152/jn.00942.2002. PMID 12740414. 
  34. "Multiscale modeling of the neuromuscular system: Coupling neurophysiology and skeletal muscle mechanics". Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews. Systems Biology and Medicine 11 (6): e1457. November 2019. doi:10.1002/wsbm.1457. PMID 31237041. https://ris.utwente.nl/ws/files/286393690/R_hrle_2019_multiscale.pdf. 
  35. "Computing Motion Dependent Afferent Activity During Cat Locomotion Using a Forward Dynamics Musculoskeletal Model" (in en). Neuromechanical Modeling of Posture and Locomotion. Springer Series in Computational Neuroscience. New York: Springer. 2016. pp. 273–307. doi:10.1007/978-1-4939-3267-2_10. ISBN 978-1-4939-3267-2. 
  36. Chapter 11 Quantifying Proprioception. Peripheral and Spinal Mechanisms in the Neural Control of Movement. 123. Elsevier. 1999. pp. 133–42. 
  37. "Mathematical models of proprioceptors. II. Structure and function of the Golgi tendon organ". Journal of Neurophysiology 96 (4): 1789–802. October 2006. doi:10.1152/jn.00869.2005. PMID 16672300. 
  38. 38.0 38.1 "Responses of Golgi tendon organs to forces applied to muscle tendon". Journal of Neurophysiology 30 (6): 1466–81. November 1967. doi:10.1152/jn.1967.30.6.1466. PMID 6066449. 
  39. "A Computational Model of a Descending Mechanosensory Pathway Involved in Active Tactile Sensing". PLOS Computational Biology 11 (7): e1004263. July 2015. doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004263. PMID 26158851. Bibcode2015PLSCB..11E4263A. 
  40. "Quantitative description of linear behavior of mammalian muscle spindles". Journal of Neurophysiology 33 (1): 59–72. January 1970. doi:10.1152/jn.1970.33.1.59. PMID 4243791. 
  41. "Force encoding in muscle spindles during stretch of passive muscle". PLOS Computational Biology 13 (9): e1005767. September 2017. doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1005767. PMID 28945740. Bibcode2017PLSCB..13E5767B. 
  42. Lin, Chou-Ching K.; Crago, Patrick E. (January 2002). "Neural and Mechanical Contributions to the Stretch Reflex: A Model Synthesis". Annals of Biomedical Engineering 30 (1): 54–67. doi:10.1114/1.1432692. ISSN 0090-6964. PMID 11874142. http://dx.doi.org/10.1114/1.1432692. 
  43. "The Importance of the Sense of Touch in Virtual and Real Environments". IEEE MultiMedia 13 (3): 24–30. 2006. doi:10.1109/MMUL.2006.69. http://www.roblesdelatorre.com/gabriel/GR-IEEE-MM-2006.pdf. Retrieved 2006-10-07. 
  44. "Ehlers-danlos syndrome, hypermobility type: an underdiagnosed hereditary connective tissue disorder with mucocutaneous, articular, and systemic manifestations". ISRN Dermatology 2012: 751768. 2012. doi:10.5402/2012/751768. PMID 23227356. 
  45. "Presbypropria: the effects of physiological ageing on proprioceptive control". Age 34 (5): 1179–94. October 2012. doi:10.1007/s11357-011-9300-y. PMID 21850402. 
  46. Blanche, Erna Imperatore; Reinoso, Gustavo; Chang, Megan C; Bodison, Stephanie (1 September 2012). "Proprioceptive Processing Difficulties Among Children With Autism Spectrum Disorders and Developmental Disabilities". The American Journal of Occupational Therapy 66 (5): 621–624. doi:10.5014/ajot.2012.004234. PMID 22917129. 
  47. "Proprioception and motor control in Parkinson's disease". Journal of Motor Behavior 41 (6): 543–52. November 2009. doi:10.3200/35-09-002. PMID 19592360. 
  48. The Man Who Lost His Body (1997). BBC Documentary.
  49. (in en) The Man Who Lost His Body, https://topdocumentaryfilms.com/the-man-who-lost-his-body/, retrieved 2023-02-21 
  50. "Phantom limb pain: theories and therapies". The Neurologist 16 (5): 277–86. September 2010. doi:10.1097/nrl.0b013e3181edf128. PMID 20827116. http://people.usd.edu/~cliff/Courses/Advanced%20Seminars%20in%20Neuroendocrinology/Pain/Weeks10.pdf. 
  51. "Neural substrate of body size: illusory feeling of shrinking of the waist". PLOS Biology 3 (12): e412. December 2005. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.0030412. PMID 16336049. 
  52. "Where was my arm again? Memory-based matching of proprioceptive targets is enhanced by increased target presentation time". Neuroscience Letters 481 (1): 54–58. August 2010. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2010.06.053. PMID 20600603. http://www.flexor.com/sites/default/files/media/memory_based_proprio.PDF. Retrieved 2013-03-15. 
  53. Elangovan, Naveen; Herrmann, Amanda; Konczak, Jürgen (April 2014). "Assessing proprioceptive function: evaluating joint position matching methods against psychophysical thresholds". Physical Therapy 94 (4): 553–561. doi:10.2522/ptj.20130103. ISSN 1538-6724. PMID 24262599. 
  54. Sacks, O. "The Disembodied Lady", in The Man Who Mistook His Wife for a Hat and his autobiographical case study A Leg to Stand On.
  55. cheng man ch'ing (1981). T'ai Chi Ch'uan. Blue Snake Books usa. pp. 86, 88. ISBN 978-0-913028-85-8. 
  56. Hanc, John (15 September 2010). "Staying on Balance, With the Help of Exercises". The New York Times. https://www.nytimes.com/2010/09/16/business/retirementspecial/16BALANCE.html. 
  57. Jerosch, Jörg; Heisel, Jürgen (2010) (in de). Management der Arthrose: Innovative Therapiekonzepte. Deutscher Ärzteverlag. p. 107. ISBN 978-3-7691-0599-5. https://books.google.com/books?id=p8-2j4d-hqwC&pg=PA107. Retrieved 8 April 2011. 
  58. Singh, Arun Kumar (1991). The Comprehensive History of Psychology. Motilal Banarsidass. p. 66. ISBN 978-81-208-0804-1. https://books.google.com/books?id=dXZnwvs6gYIC&pg=PA66. Retrieved 8 April 2011. 
  59. Dickinson, John (1976). Proprioceptive control of human movement. Princeton Book Co.. p. 4. https://books.google.com/books?id=CI8XAQAAMAAJ. Retrieved 8 April 2011. 
  60. Todd, Robert Bentley (1847). The Cyclopaedia of Anatomy and Physiology Vol. 4. London: Longmans. pp. 585–723. 
  61. Foster, Susan Leigh (2010). Choreographing Empathy: Kinesthesia in Performance. Taylor & Francis. p. 74. ISBN 978-0-415-59655-8. https://books.google.com/books?id=MDWuaqZiHZcC&pg=PA74. Retrieved 8 April 2011. 
  62. Brookhart, John M.; Mountcastle, Vernon B.; Geiger, Stephen R. (1984). The Nervous system: Sensory processes;. American Physiological Society. p. 784. ISBN 978-0-683-01108-1. https://books.google.com/books?id=n7fwAAAAMAAJ. Retrieved 8 April 2011. 
  63. Sherrington, C.S. (1906). The Integrative Action of the Nervous System. NewHaven, CT: Yale University Press.
  64. Sherrington, Charles Scott (1907). "On The Proprio-ceptive System, Especially In Its Reflex Aspect". Brain 29 (4): 467–482. doi:10.1093/brain/29.4.467. 
  65. 65.0 65.1 "The kinaesthetic senses". The Journal of Physiology 587 (Pt 17): 4139–46. September 2009. doi:10.1113/jphysiol.2009.175372. PMID 19581378. 
  66. "Muscle spindle signals combine with the sense of effort to indicate limb position". The Journal of Physiology 568 (Pt 3): 1035–46. November 2005. doi:10.1113/jphysiol.2005.092619. PMID 16109730. 
  67. "Cutaneous receptors contribute to kinesthesia at the index finger, elbow, and knee". Journal of Neurophysiology 94 (3): 1699–706. September 2005. doi:10.1152/jn.00191.2005. PMID 15917323. 
  68. 68.0 68.1 68.2 "Unifying model of shoot gravitropism reveals proprioception as a central feature of posture control in plants". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 110 (2): 755–60. January 2013. doi:10.1073/pnas.1214301109. PMID 23236182. Bibcode2013PNAS..110..755B. 
  69. 69.0 69.1 69.2 69.3 "How do plants read their own shapes?". The New Phytologist 212 (2): 333–37. October 2016. doi:10.1111/nph.14143. PMID 27532273. 
  70. "From gravitropism to dynamical posture control: proprioception in plants". University of Cambridge. http://talks.cam.ac.uk/talk/index/44690. 
  71. "Regulation of organ straightening and plant posture by an actin-myosin XI cytoskeleton". Nature Plants 1 (4): 15031. March 2015. doi:10.1038/nplants.2015.31. PMID 27247032. 
  72. "The Kinematics of Plant Nutation Reveals a Simple Relation between Curvature and the Orientation of Differential Growth". PLOS Computational Biology 12 (12): e1005238. December 2016. doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1005238. PMID 27923062. Bibcode2016PLSCB..12E5238B. 
  73. Gabbatiss, Josh (10 January 2017). "Plants can see, hear and smell – and respond". http://www.bbc.com/earth/story/20170109-plants-can-see-hear-and-smell-and-respond. 
  74. 74.0 74.1 plantguy (28 May 2017). "The Selfish Plant 4 – Plant Proprioception?". https://www.howplantswork.com/2017/05/28/the-selfish-plant-4-plant-proprioception/. 
  75. Antani, Jyot D.; Gupta, Rachit; Lee, Annie H.; Rhee, Kathy Y.; Manson, Michael D.; Lele, Pushkar P. (2021-09-14). "Mechanosensitive recruitment of stator units promotes binding of the response regulator CheY-P to the flagellar motor" (in en). Nature Communications 12 (1): 5442. doi:10.1038/s41467-021-25774-2. ISSN 2041-1723. PMID 34521846. Bibcode2021NatCo..12.5442A. 

External links