Medicine:Riddoch syndrome

From HandWiki
Short description: Type of visual impairment
Riddoch syndrome
SpecialtyNeurology

The Riddoch syndrome is a term coined by Zeki and Ffytche (1998) in a paper published in Brain.[1] The term acknowledges the work of George Riddoch who was the first to describe a condition in which a form of visual impairment, caused by lesions in the occipital lobe, leaves the sufferer blind but able to distinguish visual stimuli with specific characteristics when these appear in the patient's blind field. The most common stimuli that can be perceived consciously are the presence and direction of fast moving objects (moving at a speed in excess of 10 degrees per second); in his work these moving objects were described as "vague and shadowy". Riddoch concluded from his observations that "movement may be recognized as a special visual perception".[2]

Riddoch's description was dismissed by Sir Gordon Holmes in a 1918 paper[3] in which he wrote that "The condition described by Riddoch should not be spoken of as a dissociation of the elements of visual sensation" because "occipital lesions do not produce true dissociations of function with intact retinal sensibility".[3] The idea of a separate representation of visual motion was further dismissed by H.L. Teuber [4] and, in general, such an idea was not accepted until physiological studies in the monkey demonstrated the existence of a cortical area lying outside the primary visual cortex (area V1) in which almost all cells were selective for directional motion.[5][6] With that new knowledge, a new study of patient GY, who had been used extensively to demonstrate the phenomenon of blindsight (that is to say the ability to discriminate correctly visual stimuli presented to the blind field without conscious awareness) led to interesting findings. The re-examination showed that, when presented with fast-moving, high contrast, visual stimuli in his blind field, he could discriminate their presence and direction of motion consciously,[7][1] This, in turn, led to a re-classification of blindsight into Type 1 and Type 2[8] the former adhering to the previous definition of blindsight while the latter acknowledging the fact that the experience of such subjects can be conscious even if much degraded.

Only moving objects in the scotoma are visible, static ones being invisible to the patient.[2] The moving objects are not perceived to have color or detail. The subject may only have awareness of the movement without visual perception of it (gnosanopsia),[1] or the general shape of a moving object may be perceivable as a shadow-like outline.[9] The syndrome is named after George Riddoch who had been a temporary officer in the Royal Army Medical Corps and examined soldiers who were blinded by gunshot wounds to their brains.[1]

At least one patient was able to use a rocking chair—putting non-moving surroundings in relative motion to her head—to improve her motion perception. She eventually was able to do the same with movement of her head.[9]

See also

References

  1. 1.0 1.1 1.2 1.3 "The Riddoch syndrome: insights into the neurobiology of conscious vision". Brain (Oxford University Press) 121 ( Pt 1) (1): 25–45. January 1998. doi:10.1093/brain/121.1.25. PMID 9549486. 
  2. 2.0 2.1 "Dissociation of visual perceptions due to occipital injuries, with especial reference to appreciation of movement". Brain (Oxford University Press) 40 (1): 15–57. 1917. doi:10.1093/brain/40.1.15. 
  3. 3.0 3.1 "Disturbances of Visual Orientation". The British Journal of Ophthalmology 2 (9): 449–68. September 1918. doi:10.1136/bjo.2.9.449. PMID 18167816. 
  4. Perception, Handbook of Physiology. Am. Physiological Society. 1960. 
  5. Dubner, R.; Zeki, S.M. (1971-12-24). "Response properties and receptive fields of cells in an anatomically defined region of the superior temporal sulcus in the monkey" (in en). Brain Research 35 (2): 528–532. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(71)90494-X. PMID 5002708. 
  6. "Functional organization of a visual area in the posterior bank of the superior temporal sulcus of the rhesus monkey". The Journal of Physiology 236 (3): 549–73. February 1974. doi:10.1113/jphysiol.1974.sp010452. PMID 4207129. 
  7. "Conscious visual perception without V1". Brain 116 ( Pt 6) (6): 1293–302. December 1993. doi:10.1093/brain/116.6.1293. PMID 8293272. 
  8. "Consciousness and Commentaries" (in fr). International Journal of Psychology 33 (3): 227–233. 1998. doi:10.1080/002075998400411. 
  9. 9.0 9.1 All Things Considered. 26 May 2014. NPR. WAMU.