Philosophy:Self-efficacy

From HandWiki
Short description: Psychology concept

Template:Self sidebarIn psychology, self-efficacy is an individual's belief in their capacity to act in the ways necessary to reach specific goals.[1] The concept was originally proposed by the psychologist Albert Bandura.

Self-efficacy affects every area of human endeavor. By determining the beliefs a person holds regarding their power to affect situations, self-efficacy strongly influences both the power a person actually has to face challenges competently and the choices a person is most likely to make. These effects are particularly apparent, and compelling, with regard to investment behaviors such as in health,[2] education,[3] and agriculture.[4]

A strong sense of self-efficacy promotes human accomplishment and personal well-being. A person with high self-efficacy views challenges as things that are supposed to be mastered rather than threats to avoid. These people are able to recover from failure faster and are more likely to attribute failure to a lack of effort. They approach threatening situations with the belief that they can control them. These things have been linked to lower levels of stress and a lower vulnerability to depression.[5]

In contrast, people with a low sense of self-efficacy view difficult tasks as personal threats and shy away from them. Difficult tasks lead them to look at the skills they lack rather than the ones they have. It is easy for them to lose faith in their own abilities after a failure. Low self-efficacy can be linked to higher levels of stress and depression.[5]

Theoretical approaches

Social cognitive theory

Main page: Philosophy:Social cognitive theory

Psychologist Albert Bandura has defined self-efficacy as one's belief in one's ability to succeed in specific situations or accomplish a task. One's sense of self-efficacy can play a major role in how one approaches goals, tasks, and challenges.[2] The theory of self-efficacy lies at the center of Bandura's social cognitive theory, which emphasizes the role of observational learning and social experience in the development of personality. The main concept in social cognitive theory is that an individual's actions and reactions, including social behaviors and cognitive processes, in almost every situation are influenced by the actions that individual has observed in others. Because self-efficacy is developed from external experiences and self-perception and is influential in determining the outcome of many events, it is an important aspect of social cognitive theory. Self-efficacy represents the personal perception of external social factors.[6][7][8][9] According to Bandura's theory, people with high self-efficacy—that is, those who believe they can perform well—are more likely to view difficult tasks as something to be mastered rather than something to be avoided.

Social learning theory

Social learning theory describes the acquisition of skills that are developed exclusively or primarily within a social group. Social learning depends on how individuals either succeed or fail at dynamic interactions within groups, and promotes the development of individual emotional and practical skills as well as accurate perception of self and acceptance of others. According to this theory, people learn from one another through observation, imitation, and modeling. Self-efficacy reflects an individual's understanding of what skills he/she can offer in a group setting.[10]

Self-concept theory

Main page: Philosophy:Self-concept

Self-concept theory seeks to explain how people perceive and interpret their own existence from clues they receive from external sources, focusing on how these impressions are organized and how they are active throughout life. Successes and failures are closely related to the ways in which people have learned to view themselves and their relationships with others. This theory describes self-concept as learned (i.e., not present at birth); organized (in the way it is applied to the self); and dynamic (i.e., ever-changing, and not fixed at a certain age).[11]

Attribution theory

Main page: Philosophy:Attribution (psychology)

Attribution theory focuses on how people attribute events and how those beliefs interact with self-perception. Attribution theory defines three major elements of cause:

  • Locus is the location of the perceived cause. If the locus is internal (dispositional), feelings of self-esteem and self-efficacy will be enhanced by success and diminished by failure.
  • Stability describes whether the cause is perceived as static or dynamic over time. It is closely related to expectations and goals, in that when people attribute their failures to stable factors such as the difficulty of a task, they will expect to fail in that task in the future.
  • Controllability describes whether a person feels actively in control of the cause. Failing at a task one thinks one cannot control can lead to feelings of humiliation, shame, and/or anger.[12]

Sources of self-efficacy

Mastery experiences

According to Bandura, the most effective way to build self-efficacy is to engage in mastery experiences.[5] These mastery experiences can be defined as a personal experience of success.[13] Achieving difficult goals in the face of adversity helps build confidence and strengthen perseverance.[5]

Vicarious experiences of social models

Another source of self-efficacy is through vicarious experiences of social models. Seeing someone, who you view as similar to yourself, succeed at something difficult can motivate you to believe that you have the skills necessary to achieve a similar goal. However, the inverse of the previous statement is true as well. Seeing someone fail at a task can lead to doubt in personal skills and abilities. "The greater the assumed similarity, the more persuasive are the models' successes and failures."[5]

Belief in success

A third source of self-efficacy is found through strengthening the belief that one has the ability to succeed. Those who are positively persuaded that they have the ability to complete a given task show a greater and more sustained effort to complete a task. It also lowers the effect of self-doubt in a person. However, it is important to remember that those who are doing the encouraging, put the person in a situation where success is more often likely to be attained. If they are put in a situation prematurely with no hope of any success, it can undermine self-efficacy.[5]

Physiological and psychological states

A person's emotional and physiological state can also influence an individual's belief about their ability to perform in a given situation.[14] When judging their own capabilities, people will often take in information from their body, how a person interprets that information impacts self-efficacy. For example, in activities that require physical strength, someone may take fatigue or pain as an indicator of inability or of effort.[15]

How it affects human function

Choices regarding behavior

People generally avoid tasks where self-efficacy is low, but undertake tasks where self-efficacy is high. When self-efficacy is significantly beyond actual ability, it leads to an overestimation of the ability to complete tasks. On the other hand, when self-efficacy is significantly lower than actual ability, it discourages growth and skill development. Research shows that the optimum level of self-efficacy is slightly above ability; in this situation, people are most encouraged to tackle challenging tasks and gain experience.[16] Self-efficacy is made of dimensions like magnitude, strength, and generality to explain how one believes they will perform on a specific task.[17]

Motivation

High self-efficacy can affect motivation in both positive and negative ways. In general, people with high self-efficacy are more likely to make efforts to complete a task, and to persist longer in those efforts, than those with low self-efficacy.[18] The stronger the self-efficacy or mastery expectations, the more active the efforts.[19]

A negative effect of low self-efficacy is that it can lead to a state of learned helplessness. Learned helplessness was studied by Martin Seligman in an experiment in which shocks were applied to animals. Through the experiment, it was discovered that the animals placed in a cage where they could escape shocks by moving to a different part of the cage did not attempt to move if they had formerly been placed in a cage in which escape from the shocks was not possible. Low self-efficacy can lead to this state in which it is believed that no amount of effort will make a difference in the success of the task at hand.[20]

Work performance

Self-efficacy theory has been embraced by management scholars and practitioners because of its applicability in the workplace. Overall, self-efficacy is positively and strongly related to work-related performance as measured by the weighted average correlation across 114 selected studies.[21] The strength of the relationship, though, is moderated by both task complexity and environmental context. For more complex tasks, the relationships between self-efficacy and work performance is weaker than for easier work-related tasks. In actual work environments, which are characterized by performance constraints, ambiguous demands, deficient performance feedback, and other complicating factors, the relationship appears weaker than in controlled laboratory settings. The implications of this research is that managers should provide accurate descriptions of tasks and provide clear and concise instructions. Moreover, they should provide the necessary supporting elements, including training employees in developing their self-efficacy in addition to task-related skills, for employees to be successful. It has also been suggested that managers should factor in self-efficacy when trying to decide candidates for developmental or training programs. It has been found that those who are high in self-efficacy learn more which leads to higher job performance.[22]

Social cognitive theory explains that employees use five basic capabilities to self influence themselves in order to initiate, regulate and sustain their behavior: symbolizing, forethought, observational, self-regulatory and self reflective.[23]

According to one study, the study presents a new questionnaire called Work Agentic Capabilities (WAC) that measures the four agentic capabilities in the organizational context: forethought, self-regulation, self-reflection, and vicarious capability. The WAC questionnaire was validated through exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses, and it was found to be positively correlated with psychological capital, positive job attitudes, proactive organizational behaviors, perceived job performance, and promotion prospects. The study concludes that the WAC questionnaire can reliably measure agentic capabilities and can be useful in understanding the sociodemographic and organizational differences in mean values of agentic capabilities.[24]

Thought patterns and responses

Self-efficacy has several effects on thought patterns and responses:

  • Low self-efficacy can lead people to believe tasks to be harder than they actually are, while high self-efficacy can lead people to believe tasks to be easier than they are. This often results in poor task planning, as well as increased stress.
  • People become erratic and unpredictable when engaging in a task in which they have low self-efficacy.
  • People with high self-efficacy tend to take a wider view of a task in order to determine the best plan.
  • Obstacles often stimulate people with high self-efficacy to greater efforts, where someone with low self-efficacy will tend toward discouragement and giving up.
  • A person with high self-efficacy will attribute failure to external factors, where a person with low self-efficacy will blame low ability. For example, someone with high self-efficacy in regards to mathematics may attribute a poor test grade to a harder-than-usual test, illness, lack of effort, or insufficient preparation. A person with a low self-efficacy will attribute the result to poor mathematical ability.

Health behaviors

A number of studies on the adoption of health practices have measured self-efficacy to assess its potential to initiate behavior change.[2] With increased self-efficacy, individuals have greater confidence in their ability and thus are more likely to engage in healthy behaviors. Greater engagement in healthy behaviors, result in positive patient health outcomes such as improved quality of life. Choices affecting health (such as smoking, physical exercise, dieting, condom use, dental hygiene, seat belt use, and breast self-examination) are dependent on self-efficacy.[25] Self-efficacy beliefs are cognitions that determine whether health behavior change will be initiated, how much effort will be expended, and how long it will be sustained in the face of obstacles and failures. Self-efficacy influences how high people set their health goals (e.g., "I intend to reduce my smoking", or "I intend to quit smoking altogether").

Relationship to locus of control

Bandura showed that difference in self-efficacy correlates to fundamentally different world views.[26][27] People with high self-efficacy generally believe that they are in control of their own lives, that their own actions and decisions shape their lives, while people with low self-efficacy may see their lives as outside their control. For example, a student with high self-efficacy who does poorly on an exam will likely attribute the failure to the fact that they did not study enough. However, a student with low self-efficacy who does poorly on an exam is likely to believe the cause of that failure was due to the test being too difficult or challenging, which the student does not control.

Factors affecting self-efficacy

Bandura identifies four factors affecting self-efficacy.

  1. Experience, or "enactive attainment" – The experience of mastery is the most important factor determining a person's self-efficacy. Success raises self-efficacy, while failure lowers it.[28] According to psychologist Erik Erikson: "Children cannot be fooled by empty praise and condescending encouragement. They may have to accept artificial bolstering of their self-esteem in lieu of something better, but what I call their accruing ego identity gains real strength only from wholehearted and consistent recognition of real accomplishment, that is, achievement that has meaning in their culture."
  2. Modeling, or "vicarious experience" – Modeling is experienced as, "If they can do it, I can do it as well". When we see someone succeeding, our own self-efficacy increases; where we see people failing, our self-efficacy decreases. This process is most effectual when we see ourselves as similar to the model. Although not as influential as direct experience, modeling is particularly useful for people who are particularly unsure of themselves.
  3. Social persuasion – Social persuasion generally manifests as direct encouragement or discouragement from another person. Discouragement is generally more effective at decreasing a person's self-efficacy than encouragement is at increasing it.
  4. Physiological factors – In stressful situations, people commonly exhibit signs of distress: shakes, aches and pains, fatigue, fear, nausea, etc. Perceptions of these responses in oneself can markedly alter self-efficacy. Getting "butterflies in the stomach" before public speaking will be interpreted by someone with low self-efficacy as a sign of inability, thus decreasing self-efficacy further, where high self-efficacy would lead to interpreting such physiological signs as normal and unrelated to ability. It is one's belief in the implications of physiological response that alters self-efficacy, rather than the physiological response itself.[29][better source needed]

Genetic and environmental determinants

In a Norwegian twin study, the heritability of self-efficacy in adolescents was estimated at 75 percent. The remaining variance, 25 percent, was due to environmental influences not shared between family members. The shared family environment did not contribute to individual differences in self-efficacy.[30] The twins reared-together design may overestimate the effect of genetic influences and underestimate shared environmental influences because variables measured on the family level are modeled to be equal for both twins and thus cannot be separated into genetic and environmental components. Employing an alternative design, namely that of adoptive siblings, Buchanan et al. found significant shared environmental effects.[30]

Self-efficacy was also found to be influenced by environmental factors like cultural context, home environment and educational environment. For example, parents provide their children with sets of aspirations, role models and expectations, and form beliefs about their children's abilities. Parents' beliefs are communicated to their children and affect the children's own ability beliefs.[31] The classroom environment can also influence the students' self-efficacy through the amount and type of teacher attention, social comparisons, the tasks, the grading system and more.[32] These are often influenced by school environment, including its culture and its educational philosophy. Studies showed that school environment influences the way the four sources of self-efficacy shape students' academic self-efficacy. For example, in different school systems - Democratic schools, Waldorf schools and mainstream public schools - there were differences in the way academic self-efficacy changed along grade levels, as well as variations in the roles of the various sources of self-efficacy.[33] Both parental and educational environments are embedded in wider cultural contexts which influence the way self-efficacy is formed. For example, the mathematics self-efficacy of students from collectivist cultures was found to be more influenced by vicarious experiences and social persuasions than self-efficacy of students from individualist cultures.[34]

Theoretical models of behavior

A theoretical model of the effect of self-efficacy on transgressive behavior was developed and verified in research with school children.[35]

Prosociality and moral disengagement

Prosocial behavior (such as helping others, sharing, and being kind and cooperative) and moral disengagement (manifesting in behaviors such as making excuses for bad behavior, avoiding responsibility for consequences, and blaming the victim) are negatively correlated.[36] Academic, social, and self-regulatory self-efficacy encourages prosocial behavior, and thus helps prevent moral disengagement.[37]

Over-efficaciousness in learning

In low-performing students, self-efficacy is not a self-fulfilling prophecy.[38] Over-efficaciousness or 'illusional' efficacy discourages the critical examination of one's practices, therefore inhibiting professional learning.[39] One study, which included 101 lower-division Portuguese students at U.T. Austin, examined the foreign students' beliefs about learning, goal attainment, and motivation to continue with language study. It was concluded that over-efficaciousness negatively affected student motivation, so that students who believed they were "good at languages" had less motivation to study.[40]

Health behavior change

Social-cognitive models of health behavior change cast self-efficacy as predictor, mediator, or moderator. As a predictor, self-efficacy is supposed to facilitate the forming of behavioral intentions, the development of action plans, and the initiation of action. As mediator, self-efficacy can help prevent relapse to unhealthy behavior.[41] As a moderator, self-efficacy can support the translation of intentions into action.[42] See Health action process approach.

Possible applications

Academic contexts

Parents' sense of academic efficacy for their child is linked to their children's scholastic achievement. If the parents have higher perceived academic capabilities and aspirations for their child, the child itself will share those same beliefs. This promotes academic self-efficacy for the child, and in turn, leads to scholastic achievement. It also leads to prosocial behavior, and reduces vulnerability to feelings of futility and depression.[43] There is a relationship between low self-efficacy and depression.[44]

In a study, the majority of a group of students questioned felt they had a difficulty with listening in class situations. Instructors then helped strengthen their listening skills by making them aware about how the use of different strategies could produce better outcomes. This way, their levels of self-efficacy were improved as they continued to figure out what strategies worked for them.[45]

STEM

Self-efficacy has proven especially useful for helping undergraduate students to gain insights into their career development in STEM fields.[46] Researchers have reported that mathematics self-efficacy is more predictive of mathematics interest, choice of math-related courses, and math majors than past achievements in math or outcome expectations.[46]

Self-efficacy theory has been applied to the career area to examine why women are underrepresented in male-dominated STEM fields such as mathematics, engineering, and science. It was found that gender differences in self-efficacy expectancies importantly influence the career-related behaviors and career choices of young women.[47]

Technical self-efficacy was found to be a crucial factor for teaching computer programming to school students, as students with higher levels of technological self-efficacy achieve higher learning outcomes. The effect of technical self-efficacy was found to be even stronger than the effect of gender.[48]

Writing

Writing studies research indicates a strong relationship linking perceived self-efficacy to motivation and performance outcomes. Students' academic accomplishments are inextricably connected to their self-thought of efficacy and constructed motivation within their contexts.[6] The resilient efforts that highly self-efficacious individuals exert usually enable them to face the challenge and produce high-performance achievements.[49] Besides, individuals place more value on the academic activities which they used to achieve success [50] Recent writing research accentuated this connection between writers' self-efficacy, motivation and efforts offered, and achieving success in writing. In another way, writers with a high level of confidence in their writing capabilities and processes are more willing to work persistently for satisfying and effective writing. In contrast, those who have less sense of efficacy are unable to resist any failure and tend to avoid what they believe it as a painful experience_ writing.[51] There is a causal relationship between self-efficacy beliefs that the writers hold and the accomplishments that they can achieve in their writing. Accordingly, scholars emphasized that writing self-efficacy beliefs are instrumental for making predictions of crafting outcomes.[49][52][51]

Empirically speaking, there is a study on introductory Composition courses that proved that poor writing is strongly sponsored by the writers' self-doubts of making effective writing rather than their actual writing capabilities.[53] Self-referent thought is a powerful mediator that links one's knowledge and actions.[49] Therefore, even when individuals have the required skills and knowledge, their self-referent may continue in hindering their optimal performance. A 1997 study looked at how self-efficacy could influence the writing ability of 5th graders in the United States. Researchers found that there was a direct correlation between students' self-efficacy and their own writing apprehension, essay performance, and perceived usefulness of writing. As the researchers suggest, this study is important because it showed how important it is for teachers to teach skills and also to build confidence in their students.[54] A more recent study was done that seemed to replicate the findings of the previous study quite nicely. This study found that students' beliefs about their own writing did have an impact on their self-efficacy, apprehension, and performance.[55] This is also evident in a different study on collegiate students that reported the change of knowledge seeking as an outcome of their self-efficacy promotion. Thus, students' self-efficacy is predictive of students' production of effective writing.[56] Therefore, increasing their writing positive beliefs resulted in better performance in their writing.[53] Nurturing the participants' perceived self-efficacy elevated the goals that they used to set up in the writing courses, and this, in turn, promoted their quality of writing and placed more sense of self-satisfaction.[6] Self-regulatory writing is another key determinant associated with writing efficacy and has great influence on writing development. Self-regulation encapsulates the writing dynamism of complexities, time structure, strategies, and whether deficiencies or capabilities. Through self-regulatory efficacy, writers strive toward more self-efficaciousness that effectively impacts their writing attainments[57]

Motivation

One of the factors most commonly associated with self-efficacy in writing studies is motivation. Motivation is often divided into two categories: extrinsic and intrinsic. McLeod suggests that intrinsic motivators tend to be more effective than extrinsic motivators because students then perceive the given task as inherently valuable.[58] Additionally, McCarthy, Meier, and Rinderer explain that writers who are intrinsically motivated tend to be more self-directed, take active control of their writing, and see themselves as more capable of setting and accomplishing goals.[59] Furthermore, writing studies research indicates that self-efficacy influences student choices, effort, persistence, perseverance, thought patterns, and emotional reactions when completing a writing assignment.[50][60][61] Students with a high self-efficacy are more likely to attempt and persist in unfamiliar writing tasks.[59][61]

Performance outcomes

Self-efficacy has often been linked to students' writing performance outcomes. More so than any other element within the cognitive-affective domain, self-efficacy beliefs have proven to be predictive of performance outcomes in writing.[59][50][60][61] In order to assess the relationship between self-efficacy and writing capabilities, several studies have constructed scales to measure students' self-efficacy beliefs.[59][60] The results of these scales are then compared to student writing samples. The studies included other variables, such as writing anxiety, grade goals, depth of processing, and expected outcomes. However, self-efficacy was the only variable that was a statistically significant predictor of writing performance.[50]

Public speaking

A strong negative relationship has been suggested between levels of speech anxiety and self-efficacy.[62][63]

Healthcare

As the focus of healthcare continues to transition from the medical model to health promotion and preventive healthcare, the role of self-efficacy as a potent influence on health behavior and self-care has come under review. According to Luszczynska and Schwarzer,[2] self-efficacy plays a role in influencing the adoption, initiation, and maintenance of healthy behaviors, as well as curbing unhealthy practices.

Healthcare providers can integrate self-efficacy interventions into patient education. One method is to provide examples of other people acting on a health promotion behavior and then work with the patient to encourage their belief in their own ability to change.[64] Furthermore, when nurses followed-up by telephone after hospital discharge, individuals with chronic obstructive pulmonary disease (COPD) were found to have increased self-efficacy in managing breathing difficulties. In this study, the nurses helped reinforce education and reassured patients regarding their self-care management techniques while in their home environment.[65]

Other contexts

At the National Kaohsiung First University of Science and Technology in Taiwan, researchers investigated the correlations between general Internet self-efficacy (GISE), Web-specific self-efficacy (WSE), and e-service usage. Researchers concluded that GISE directly affects the WSE of a consumer, which in turn shows a strong correlation with e-service usage. These findings are significant for future consumer targeting and marketing.[66]

Furthermore, self-efficacy has been included as one of the four factors of core self-evaluation, one's fundamental appraisal of oneself, along with locus of control, neuroticism, and self-esteem.[67] Core self-evaluation has shown to predict job satisfaction and job performance.[67][68][69][70][71]

Researchers have also examined self-efficacy in the context of the work–life interface. Chan et al. (2016) developed and validated a measure "self-efficacy to regulate work and life" and defined it as "the belief one has in one's own ability to achieve a balance between work and non-work responsibilities, and to persist and cope with challenges posed by work and non-work demands" (p. 1758).[72] Specifically, Chan et al. (2016) found that "self-efficacy to regulate work and life" helped to explain the relationship between work–family enrichment, work–life balance, and job satisfaction and family satisfaction.[72] Chan et al. (2017) also found that "self-efficacy to regulate work and life" assists individuals to achieve work–life balance and work engagement despite the presence of family and work demands.[73]

Subclassifications

While self-efficacy is sometimes measured as a whole, as with the General Self-Efficacy Scale,[74] it is also measured in particular functional situations.

Social self-efficacy

Social self-efficacy has been variably defined and measured. According to Smith and Betz, social self-efficacy is "an individual's confidence in her/his ability to engage in the social interactional tasks necessary to initiate and maintain interpersonal relationships." They measured social self-efficacy using an instrument of their own devise called the Scale of Perceived Social Self-Efficacy, which measured six domains: (1) making friends, (2) pursuing romantic relationships, (3) social assertiveness, (4) performance in public situations, (5) groups or parties, and (6) giving or receiving help.[75] More recently, it has been suggested that social self-efficacy can also be operationalised in terms of cognitive (confidence in knowing what to do in social situations) and behavioral (confidence in performing in social situations) social self-efficacy.[76]

Matsushima and Shiomi measured self-efficacy by focusing on self-confidence about social skill in personal relationship, trust in friends, and trust by friends.[77]

Researchers suggest that social self-efficacy is strongly correlated with shyness and social anxiety.

Academic self-efficacy

Academic self-efficacy refers to the belief that one can successfully engage in and complete course-specific academic tasks, such as accomplishing course aims, satisfactorily completing assignments, achieving a passing grade, and meeting the requirements to continue to pursue one's major course of study.[78] Various empirical inquiries have been aimed at measuring academic self-efficacy.[79][80][81]

Positive academic emotions, such as pride, enthusiasm, and enjoyment, are likely to be influenced by the level of self-efficacy an individual holds. This is because self-efficacy has been linked to an individual's belief in their ability to successfully complete tasks. Therefore, as an individual's self-efficacy increases, they may be more likely to experience positive academic emotions.[82]

Eating self-efficacy

Eating self-efficacy refers to an individual's perceived belief that they can resist the impulse to eat.[83]

Other

Other areas of self-efficacy that have been identified for study include teacher self-efficacy[84] and technological self-efficacy.

Clarifications and distinctions

Self-efficacy versus Efficacy
Unlike efficacy, which is the power to produce an effect—in essence, competence—the term self-efficacy is used, by convention, to refer to the belief (accurate or not) that one has the power to produce that effect by completing a given task or activity related to that competency. Self-efficacy is the belief in one's efficacy.
Self-efficacy versus Self-esteem
Self-efficacy is the perception of one's own ability to reach a goal; self-esteem is the sense of self-worth. For example, a person who is a terrible rock climber would probably have poor self-efficacy with regard to rock climbing, but this will not affect self-esteem if the person does not rely on rock climbing to determine self-worth.[52] On the other hand, one might have enormous confidence with regard to rock climbing, yet set such a high standard, and base enough of self-worth on rock-climbing skill, that self-esteem is low.[85] Someone who has high self-efficacy in general but is poor at rock climbing might have misplaced confidence, or believe that improvement is possible.
Self-efficacy versus Confidence
Canadian-American psychologist Albert Bandura describes the difference between self-efficacy and confidence as such:[86]

the construct of self-efficacy differs from the colloquial term 'confidence.' Confidence is a nonspecific term that refers to strength of belief but does not necessarily specify what the certainty is about. I can be supremely confident that I will fail at an endeavor. Perceived self-efficacy refers to belief in one's agentive capabilities, that one can produce given levels of attainment. A self-efficacy belief, therefore, includes both an affirmation of a capability level and the strength of that belief.

Self-efficacy versus Self-concept
Self-efficacy comprises beliefs of personal capability to perform specific actions. Self-concept is measured more generally and includes the evaluation of such competence and the feelings of self-worth associated with the behaviors in question.[52] In an academic situation, a student's confidence in their ability to write an essay is self-efficacy. Self-concept, on the other hand, could be how a student's level of intelligence affects their beliefs regarding their worth as a person.
Self-efficacy as part of core self-evaluations
Timothy A. Judge et al. (2002) has argued that the concepts of locus of control, neuroticism, generalized self-efficacy (which differs from Bandura's theory of self-efficacy) and self-esteem are so strongly correlated and exhibit such a high degree of theoretical overlap that they are actually aspects of the same higher order construct, which he calls core self-evaluations.[87]

See also

References

  1. "Teaching Tip Sheet: Self-Efficacy". https://www.apa.org/pi/aids/resources/education/self-efficacy. 
  2. 2.0 2.1 2.2 2.3 Luszczynska, A.; Schwarzer, R. (2005). "Social cognitive theory". Predicting health behaviour (2nd ed. rev. ed.). Buckingham, England: Open University Press. pp. 127–169. https://archive.org/details/predictinghealth00conn. 
  3. Krishnan, Pramila; Krutikova, Sofya (2013-10-01). "Non-cognitive skill formation in poor neighbourhoods of urban India" (in en). Labour Economics 24: 68–85. doi:10.1016/j.labeco.2013.06.004. ISSN 0927-5371. http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S092753711300078X. 
  4. Wuepper, David; Lybbert, Travis J. (2017-10-05). "Perceived Self-Efficacy, Poverty, and Economic Development" (in en). Annual Review of Resource Economics 9 (1): 383–404. doi:10.1146/annurev-resource-100516-053709. ISSN 1941-1340. 
  5. 5.0 5.1 5.2 5.3 5.4 5.5 Bandura, Albert (2010) (in en), Self-Efficacy, American Cancer Society, pp. 1–3, doi:10.1002/9780470479216.corpsy0836, ISBN 978-0-470-47921-6, https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.1002/9780470479216.corpsy0836, retrieved 2021-03-20 
  6. 6.0 6.1 6.2 Bandura, A (1977). "Self-efficacy: Toward a Unifying Theory of Behavioral Change". Psychological Review 84 (2): 191–215. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.84.2.191. PMID 847061. 
  7. Miller, N. E.; Dollard, J. (1941). Social Learning and Imitation. New Haven: Yale University Press. https://archive.org/details/in.ernet.dli.2015.17786. 
  8. Bandura, A (1988). "Organizational Application of Social Cognitive Theory". Australian Journal of Management 13 (2): 275–302. doi:10.1177/031289628801300210. 
  9. Mischel, W.; Shoda, Y. (1995). "A cognitive-affective system theory of personality: Reconceptualizing situations, dispositions, dynamics, and invariance in personality structure". Psychological Review 102 (2): 246–268. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.102.2.246. PMID 7740090. 
  10. Ormrod, J.E. (1999). Human learning (3rd ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice-Hall. 
  11. McAdam, E. K. (1986). "Cognitive behavior therapy and its application with adolescents". Journal of Adolescence 9 (1): 1–15. doi:10.1016/S0140-1971(86)80024-0. PMID 3700776. 
  12. Heider, Fritz (1958). The Psychology of Interpersonal Relations. New York: John Wiley & Sons. 
  13. Yeh, Yu-chu; Chang, Han-Lin; Chen, Szu-Yu (2019-04-01). "Mindful learning: A mediator of mastery experience during digital creativity game-based learning among elementary school students" (in en). Computers & Education 132: 63–75. doi:10.1016/j.compedu.2019.01.001. ISSN 0360-1315. https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0360131519300016. 
  14. Smirl, Paul (2018-04-17). "How to Build Confidence at Work" (in en-US). https://business.wisc.edu/news/build-your-confidence-achieve-greater-success-at-work/. 
  15. Lenz, Elizabeth R.; Shortridge-Baggett, Lillie M. (2002). Self-Efficacy in Nursing. Springer Publishing Company. pp. 12. ISBN 9780826115591. 
  16. Csikszentmihalyi, M., Finding Flow, 1997
  17. Porter, Lyman W.; Bigley, Gregory A.; Steers, Richard M. (2003). Motivation and Work Behavior (7th ed.). McGraw-Hill Irwin. pp. 131–132. 
  18. Schunk, Dale H. (1990). "Goal Setting and Self-Efficacy During Self-Regulated Learning". Educational Psychologist 25: 71–86. doi:10.1207/s15326985ep2501_6. http://libres.uncg.edu/ir/uncg/f/D_Schunk_Goal_1990.pdf. 
  19. Bandura, Albert (1977). Social Learning Theory. Alexandria, VA: Prentice Hall. p. 247. ISBN 978-0-13-816744-8. https://books.google.com/books?id=IXvuAAAAMAAJ. 
  20. Seifert, Kelvin (2011). Educational Psychology (Third ed.). pp. 119. https://www.dropbox.com/s/z6w0r87qmrz7vg0/Educational%20Psychology.pdf?dl=0#pageContainer116. Retrieved 16 November 2017. 
  21. Stajkovic, A.D., Luthans, F. (1998). Self-efficacy and work-related performance: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin.
  22. Lunenburg, F. C. (2011). Self-efficacy in the workplace: Implications for motivation and performance. International journal of management, business, and administration, 14(1), 1-6.
  23. Stajkovic, & Luthans, (2003) Social cognitive theory and self efficacy: implications for motivation theory and practice. In R. M. Steers, L.W. Porter, & G.A. Bigley (Eds.), Motivation and leadership at work (8th Ed.).
  24. Cenciotti, Roberto; Borgogni, Laura; Consiglio, Chiara; Fedeli, Emiliano; Alessandri, Guido (2020-11-13). "The Work Agentic Capabilities (WAC) Questionnaire: Validation of a New Measure" (in en). Revista de Psicología del Trabajo y de las Organizaciones 36 (3): 195–204. doi:10.5093/jwop2020a19. ISSN 1576-5962. https://journals.copmadrid.org/jwop/art/jwop2020a19. 
  25. Conner, M., ed (2005). Predicting health behaviour (2nd ed. rev. ed.). Buckingham, England: Open University Press. 
  26. Karyn Ainsworth, Fall Quarter Seminar Paper: What is Teaching? / What is Learning?
  27. Diffusion of the Internet within a Graduate School of Education, 2. Conceptual Framework 2.3.3.2 Bandura: Efficacy x Value
  28. Bandura, Albert. Self-efficacy: the exercise of control. New York. ISBN 0-7167-2626-2. OCLC 36074515. https://www.worldcat.org/oclc/36074515. 
  29. The YouTube video "Self Efficacy" can be found at https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=HnACsrdGZAI and summarizes many of the factors affecting self-efficacy that are described above. It additionally provides detailed examples that can clarify any confusion.
  30. 30.0 30.1 Waaktaar, Trine; Torgersen, Svenn (2013). "Self-Efficacy Is Mainly Genetic, Not Learned: A Multiple-Rater Twin Study on the Causal Structure of General Self-Efficacy in Young People". Twin Research and Human Genetics 16 (3): 651–660. doi:10.1017/thg.2013.25. PMID 23601253. 
  31. Scheithauer, Herbert; Petermann, Franz (July 1999). "William Damon und Nancy Eisenberg (Eds.). (1998). Handbook of child psychology. Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development (5th ed.). New York: Wiley, 1208 Seiten, DM 358,-. William Damon, Irving E. Siegel und Anne K. Renninger (Eds.). (1998). Handbook of child psychology. Vol. 4. Child psychology in practice (5th ed.). New York: Wiley, 1188 Seiten, DM 358,-.". Kindheit und Entwicklung 8 (3): 189–193. doi:10.1026//0942-5403.8.3.189. ISSN 0942-5403. https://econtent.hogrefe.com/doi/10.1026//0942-5403.8.3.189. 
  32. Schunk, D. H.; Pajares, F. (2010-01-01), Peterson, Penelope; Baker, Eva; McGaw, Barry, eds. (in en), Self-Efficacy Beliefs, Oxford: Elsevier, pp. 668–672, doi:10.1016/b978-0-08-044894-7.00620-5, ISBN 978-0-08-044894-7, https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/B9780080448947006205, retrieved 2023-07-08 
  33. Dorfman, Bat-Shahar; Fortus, David (October 2019). "Students' self-efficacy for science in different school systems" (in en). Journal of Research in Science Teaching 56 (8): 1037–1059. doi:10.1002/tea.21542. ISSN 0022-4308. https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/tea.21542. 
  34. Klassen, Robert M. (December 2004). "A Cross-Cultural Investigation of the Efficacy Beliefs of South Asian Immigrant and Anglo Canadian Nonimmigrant Early Adolescents." (in en). Journal of Educational Psychology 96 (4): 731–742. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.96.4.731. ISSN 1939-2176. http://doi.apa.org/getdoi.cfm?doi=10.1037/0022-0663.96.4.731. 
  35. Bandura, Albert; Caprara, Gian Vittorio; Barbaranelli, Claudio; Pastorelli, Concetta; Regalia, Camillo (2001). "Sociocognitive self-regulatory mechanisms governing transgressive behavior". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 80 (1): 125–135. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.80.1.125. PMID 11195885. 
  36. Kwak, K., & Bandura, A. (1998). Role of perceived self-efficacy and moral disengagement in antisocial conduct. Manuscript, Osan College, Seoul, Korea.
  37. Bandura, Albert; Vittorio Caprara, Gian; Barbaranelli, Claudio; Gerbino, Maria; Pastorelli, Concetta (2003). "Role of Affective Self-Regulatory Efficacy in Diverse Spheres of Psychosocial Functioning". Child Development 74 (3): 769–782. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00567. PMID 12795389. 
  38. Talsma, Kate; Schüz, Benjamin; Norris, Kimberley (2019-01-01). "Miscalibration of self-efficacy and academic performance: Self-efficacy ≠ self-fulfilling prophecy" (in en). Learning and Individual Differences 69: 182–195. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2018.11.002. ISSN 1041-6080. https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/abs/pii/S104160801830178X. 
  39. Ončevska Ager, Elena; Wyatt, Mark (2019-02-01). "Supporting a pre-service English language teacher's self-determined development" (in en). Teaching and Teacher Education 78: 106–116. doi:10.1016/j.tate.2018.11.013. ISSN 0742-051X. https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/abs/pii/S0742051X18308540. 
  40. Christine Galbreath Jernigan, What do Students Expect to Learn? The Role of Learner Expectancies, Beliefs, and Attributions for Success and Failure in Student Motivation .
  41. Schwarzer, R (2008). "Modeling health behavior change: How to predict and modify the adoption and maintenance of health behaviors". Applied Psychology: An International Review 57 (1): 1–29. doi:10.1111/j.1464-0597.2007.00325.x. 
  42. Gutiérrez-Doña, B.; Lippke, S.; Renner, B.; Kwon, S.; Schwarzer, R. (2009). "How self-efficacy and planning predict dietary behaviors in Costa Rican and South Korean women: A moderated mediation analysis". Applied Psychology: Health and Well-Being 1: 91–104. doi:10.1111/j.1758-0854.2008.01001.x. http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bsz:352-opus-75550. 
  43. Bandura, A.; Barbaranelli, C.; Caprara, G. V.; Pastorelli, C. (1996). "Multifaceted Impact of Self-Efficacy Beliefs on Academic Functioning". Child Development 67 (3): 1206–1222. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1996.tb01791.x. PMID 8706518. 
  44. Maddux, James E.; Meier, Lisa J. (1995). "Self-Efficacy and Depression". Self-Efficacy, Adaptation, and Adjustment. The Plenum Series in Social/Clinical Psychology. Plenum Press. pp. 143–169. doi:10.1007/978-1-4419-6868-5_5. ISBN 978-1-4757-6498-7. 
  45. Graham, S (2011). "Self-efficacy and academic listening". Journal of English for Academic Purposes 10 (2): 113–117. doi:10.1016/j.jeap.2011.04.001. http://centaur.reading.ac.uk/20903/1/20903Graham%2520final%5B1%5D.pdf. 
  46. 46.0 46.1 Pajares, Frank (1996). "Self-Efficacy Beliefs in Academic Settings". Review of Educational Research 66 (4): 543–578. doi:10.3102/00346543066004543. 
  47. Betz, Nancy E.; Hackett, Gail (1986). "Applications of Self-Efficacy Theory to Understanding Career Choice Behavior". Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology 4 (3): 279–289. doi:10.1521/jscp.1986.4.3.279. 
  48. Brauner, Philipp; Leonhardt, Thiemo; Ziefle, Martina; Schroeder, Ulrik (2010). "The Effect of Tangible Artifacts, Gender and Subjective Technical Competence on Teaching Programming to Seventh Graders". Teaching Fundamentals Concepts of Informatics. Lecture Notes in Computer Science. 5941. pp. 61–71. doi:10.1007/978-3-642-11376-5_7. ISBN 978-3-642-11375-8. http://www.comm.rwth-aachen.de/files/tangible_artifacts.pdf. Retrieved 2020-01-15. 
  49. 49.0 49.1 49.2 Bandura, Albert (February 1982). "Self-efficacy mechanism in human agency.". American Psychologist 37 (2): 122–147. doi:10.1037/0003-066x.37.2.122. ISSN 1935-990X. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066x.37.2.122. 
  50. 50.0 50.1 50.2 50.3 Pajares, Frank (2003). "Self-Efficacy Beliefs, Motivation, and Achievement in Writing". Reading and Writing Quarterly 19 (2): 139–158. doi:10.1080/10573560308222. 
  51. 51.0 51.1 Mascle, Deanna DeBrine (2013-03-19). "Writing Self-Efficacy and Written Communication Skills". Business Communication Quarterly 76 (2): 216–225. doi:10.1177/1080569913480234. ISSN 1080-5699. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1080569913480234. 
  52. 52.0 52.1 52.2 Pajares, Frank (2002). "Self-efficacy beliefs in academic contexts: An outline". Emory University. http://www.des.emory.edu:80/mfp/efftalk.html. 
  53. 53.0 53.1 Ryan, Paris. "The Impact of a Student's Self-Efficacy and Self-Apprehensiveness in an Introductory College Composition Course.". Eric v30 (n1). //www.nrmera.org/educational-research-theory-practice/. 
  54. Pajares, Frank; Valiante, Gio (1997-07-01). "Influence of Self-Efficacy on Elementary Students' Writing". The Journal of Educational Research 90 (6): 353–360. doi:10.1080/00220671.1997.10544593. ISSN 0022-0671. https://doi.org/10.1080/00220671.1997.10544593. 
  55. Sanders-Reio, Joanne; Alexander, Patricia A.; Reio, Thomas G.; Newman, Isadore (2014-10-01). "Do students' beliefs about writing relate to their writing self-efficacy, apprehension, and performance?" (in en). Learning and Instruction 33: 1–11. doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2014.02.001. ISSN 0959-4752. https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0959475214000206. 
  56. Zimmerman, Barry J.; Bandura, Albert (December 1994). "Impact of Self-Regulatory Influences on Writing Course Attainment". American Educational Research Journal 31 (4): 845–862. doi:10.3102/00028312031004845. ISSN 0002-8312. http://dx.doi.org/10.3102/00028312031004845. 
  57. Hidi, Suzanne. Boscolo, Pietro (2007). Writing and motivation. Elsevier. ISBN 978-0-08-045325-5. OCLC 1029700826. http://worldcat.org/oclc/1029700826. 
  58. McLeod, Susan (1987). "Some Thoughts about Feelings: The Affective Domain and the Writing Process". College Composition and Communication 38 (4): 426–435. doi:10.2307/357635. 
  59. 59.0 59.1 59.2 59.3 McCarthy, Patricia, Scott Meier, and Regina Rinderer (1985). "Self-Efficacy and Writing: A Different View of Self Evaluation". College Composition and Communication 36 (4): 465–471. doi:10.2307/357865. 
  60. 60.0 60.1 60.2 Pajares, Frank; Johnson, Margaret J. (1994). "Confidence and Competence in Writing: The Role of Self-Efficacy, Outcome Expectancy, and Apprehension". Research in the Teaching of English 28 (3): 313–331. 
  61. 61.0 61.1 61.2 Schunk, Dale K. (2003). "Self-Efficacy for Reading and Writing: Influence of Modeling, Goal-Setting, and Self-Evaluation". Reading and Writing Quarterly 19 (2): 159–172. doi:10.1080/10573560308219. http://libres.uncg.edu/ir/uncg/f/D_Schunk_Self_2003.pdf. 
  62. Hassall, Trevor; Arquero, Jose L.; Joyce, John; Gonzalez, Jose M. (12 July 2013). "Communication apprehension and communication self-efficacy in accounting students". Asian Review of Accounting 21 (2): 160–175. doi:10.1108/ARA-03-2013-0017. http://shura.shu.ac.uk/11679/1/Hassall%20Communication%20apprehension%20communication%20self-efficacy%20accounting%20students.pdf. 
  63. Ireland, Christopher (March 2016). "INTED2016 Proceedings". 10th Annual International Technology, Education and Development Conference. 1. Valencia, Spain. pp. 1474–1483. doi:10.21125/inted.2016.1317. ISBN 978-84-608-5617-7. 
  64. Ball, J., Bindler, R., Cowen, K., & Shaw, M. (2017). Principles of Pediatric Nursing: Caring for Children (7th ed.). Upper Saddle River: Pearson.
  65. Wong, K.W.; Wong, F.K.Y.; Chan, M.F. (2005). "Effects of nurse-initiated telephone follow-up among patients with chronic obstructive pulmonary disease". Journal of Advanced Nursing 49 (2): 210–222. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2648.2004.03280.x. PMID 15641953. 
  66. Hsu, M.H.; Chiu, C.M. (2004). "Internet self-efficacy and electronic service acceptance". Decision Support Systems 38 (3): 369–381. doi:10.1016/j.dss.2003.08.001. 
  67. 67.0 67.1 Judge, T. A.; Locke, E. A.; Durham, C. C. (1997). "The dispositional causes of job satisfaction: A core evaluations approach". Research in Organizational Behavior 19: 151–188. 
  68. Bono, J. E.; Judge, T. A. (2003). "Core self-evaluations: A review of the trait and its role in job satisfaction and job performance". European Journal of Personality 17 (Suppl 1): S5–S18. doi:10.1002/per.481. 
  69. Dormann, C.; Fay, D.; Zapf, D.; Frese, M. (2006). "A state-trait analysis of job satisfaction: On the effect of core self-evaluations". Applied Psychology: An International Review 55 (1): 27–51. doi:10.1111/j.1464-0597.2006.00227.x. 
  70. Judge, T. A.; Locke, E. A.; Durham, C. C.; Kluger, A. N. (1998). "Dispositional effects on job and life satisfaction: The role of core evaluations". Journal of Applied Psychology 83 (1): 17–34. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.83.1.17. PMID 9494439. 
  71. Judge, T. A.; Bono, J. E. (2001). "Relationship of core self-evaluations traits—self-esteem, generalized self-efficacy, locus of control, and emotional stability—with job satisfaction and job performance: A meta-analysis". Journal of Applied Psychology 86 (1): 80–92. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.86.1.80. PMID 11302235. 
  72. 72.0 72.1 Chan, Xi Wen; Kalliath, Thomas; Brough, Paula; Siu, Oi-Ling; O'Driscoll, Michael P.; Timms, Carolyn (2016-08-21). "Work–family enrichment and satisfaction: the mediating role of self-efficacy and work–life balance". The International Journal of Human Resource Management 27 (15): 1755–1776. doi:10.1080/09585192.2015.1075574. ISSN 0958-5192. 
  73. Xi Wen Chan; Thomas Kalliath; Paula Brough; Michael O'Driscoll; Oi-Ling Siu; Carolyn Timms (2017-07-20). "Self-efficacy and work engagement: test of a chain model". International Journal of Manpower 38 (6): 819–834. doi:10.1108/IJM-11-2015-0189. ISSN 0143-7720. 
  74. Schwarzer, R., & Jerusalem, M. (1995). Generalized Self-Efficacy scale. In J. Weinman, S. Wright, & M. Johnston, Measures in health psychology: A user's portfolio. Causal and control beliefs (pp. 35-37). Windsor, UK: NFER-NELSON.
  75. Smith, H. M.; Betz, N. E. (2000). "Development and validation of a scale of perceived social self-efficacy". Journal of Career Assessment 8 (3): 286. doi:10.1177/106907270000800306. 
  76. Grieve, Rachel; Witteveen, Kate; Tolan, G. Anne; Jacobson, Brett (2014-03-01). "Development and validation of a measure of cognitive and behavioural social self-efficacy". Personality and Individual Differences 59: 71–76. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2013.11.008. 
  77. Matsushima, R.; Shiomi, K. (2003). "Social self-efficacy and interpersonal stress in adolescence". Social Behavior and Personality 31 (4): 323–332. doi:10.2224/sbp.2003.31.4.323. 
  78. Jimenez Soffa, S. (2006). Inspiring academic confidence in the classroom: An investigation of features of the classroom experience that contribute to the academic self-efficacy of undergraduate women enrolled in gateway courses. Dissertation completed at the University of Wisconsin-Madison.
  79. Bong, M. (1997). Congruence of measurement specificity on relations between academic self-efficacy, effort, and achievement indexes. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Educational Research Association (Chicago, IL, March 24–28, 1997).
  80. Rushi, P. J. (2007). "Questioning the utility of self-efficacy measurements for Indians". International Journal of Research & Method in Education 30 (2): 193–206. doi:10.1080/17437270701383339. https://digitalcommons.imsa.edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=1016&context=pres_pr. 
  81. Academic self-concept: models, measurement, influences and enhancements.
  82. Chen, Xu; Zhao, Hongxia; Zhang, Dajun (2022-12-11). "Effect of Teacher Support on Adolescents' Positive Academic Emotion in China: Mediating Role of Psychological Suzhi and General Self-Efficacy" (in en). International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 19 (24): 16635. doi:10.3390/ijerph192416635. ISSN 1660-4601. PMID 36554516. 
  83. Lombardo, C.; Cerolini, S.; Alivernini, F.; Ballesio, A.; Violani, C.; Fernandes, M.; Lucidi, F. (2021). "Eating self-efficacy: validation of a new brief scale". Eating and Weight Disorders - Studies on Anorexia, Bulimia and Obesity 26 (1): 295–303. doi:10.1007/s40519-020-00854-2. PMID 32008174. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/32008174/#:~:text=Purpose%3A%20Eating%20self%2Defficacy%20(,body%20mass%20index%20and%20affects.. 
  84. Schwarzer, R.; Hallum, S. (2008). "Perceived teacher self-efficacy as a predictor of job stress and burnout: Mediation analyses". Applied Psychology: An International Review 57: 152–171. doi:10.1111/j.1464-0597.2008.00359.x. 
  85. Prof. Albert Bandura quoted in The Wall Street Journal 29 April 2008: D1
  86. Bandura, Albert; Freeman, W. H.; Lightsey, Richard (January 1999). "Self-Efficacy: The Exercise of Control". Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy 13 (2): 158–166. doi:10.1891/0889-8391.13.2.158. ISSN 0889-8391. http://dx.doi.org/10.1891/0889-8391.13.2.158. 
  87. Judge, Timothy A.; Erez, Amir; Bono, Joyce E.; Thoresen, Carl J. (2002). "Are measures of self-esteem, neuroticism, locus of control, and generalized self-efficacy indicators of a common core construct?". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 83 (3): 693–710. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.83.3.693. PMID 12219863. 
General

Further reading