Social:Research on meditation

From HandWiki
EEG technology has been used for meditation research

For the purpose of this article, research on meditation concerns research into the psychological and physiological effects of meditation using the scientific method. In recent years, these studies have increasingly involved the use of modern scientific techniques and instruments, such as fMRI and EEG which are able to directly observe brain physiology and neural activity in living subjects, either during the act of meditation itself, or before and after a meditation effort, thus allowing linkages to be established between meditative practice and changes in brain structure or function.

Since the 1950s hundreds of studies on meditation have been conducted, but many of the early studies were flawed and thus yielded unreliable results.[1][2] Contemporary studies have attempted to address many of these flaws with the hope of guiding current research into a more fruitful path.[3] In 2013, researchers at Johns Hopkins, publishing in the Journal of the American Medical Association, identified 47 studies that qualify as well-designed and therefore reliable. Based on these studies, they concluded that there is moderate evidence that meditation reduces anxiety, depression, and pain, but no evidence that meditation is more effective than active treatment (drugs, exercise, other behavioral therapies).[4] A 2017 commentary was similarly mixed.[5][6]

The process of meditation, as well as its effects, is a growing subfield of neurological research.[7][8] Modern scientific techniques and instruments, such as fMRI and EEG, have been used to study how regular meditation affects individuals by measuring brain and bodily changes.[7]


Difficulties in the scientific study of meditation

Weaknesses in historic meditation and mindfulness research

A comparison of the effect of various meditation techniques on systolic blood pressure.[1]

In June, 2007 the United States National Center for Complementary and Integrative Health (NCCIH) published an independent, peer-reviewed, meta-analysis of the state of meditation research, conducted by researchers at the University of Alberta Evidence-based Practice Center. The report reviewed 813 studies involving five broad categories of meditation: mantra meditation, mindfulness meditation, yoga, T'ai chi, and Qigong, and included all studies on adults through September 2005, with a particular focus on research pertaining to hypertension, cardiovascular disease, and substance abuse. The report concluded:

Scientific research on meditation practices does not appear to have a common theoretical perspective and is characterized by poor methodological quality. Future research on meditation practices must be more rigorous in the design and execution of studies and in the analysis and reporting of results. (p. 6)

It noted that there is no theoretical explanation of health effects from meditation common to all meditation techniques.[1]

A version of this report subsequently published in the Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine in 2008 stated: "Most clinical trials on meditation practices are generally characterized by poor methodological quality with significant threats to validity in every major quality domain assessed." This was despite a statistically significant increase in quality of all reviewed meditation research, in general, over time between 1956 and 2005. Of the 400 clinical studies, 10% were found to be good quality. A call was made for rigorous study of meditation.[3] These authors also noted that this finding is not unique to the area of meditation research and that the quality of reporting is a frequent problem in other areas of complementary and alternative medicine (CAM) research and related therapy research domains.

Of more than 3,000 scientific studies that were found in a comprehensive search of 17 relevant databases, only about 4% had randomised controlled trials (RCTs), which are designed to exclude the placebo effect.[1]

In a 2013 meta-analysis, Awasthi argued that meditation is defined poorly and despite the research studies showing clinical efficacy, exact mechanisms of action remain unclear.[9] A 2017 commentary was similarly mixed,[5][6] with concerns including the particular characteristics of individuals who tend to participate in mindfulness and meditation research.[10]

Position statements

A 2013 statement from the American Heart Association (AHA) evaluated the evidence for the effectiveness of TM as a treatment for hypertension as "unknown/unclear/uncertain or not well-established", and stated: "Because of many negative studies or mixed results and a paucity of available trials... other meditation techniques are not recommended in clinical practice to lower BP at this time."[11] According to the AHA, while there are promising results about the impact of meditation in reducing blood pressure and managing insomnia, depression and anxiety, it is not a replacement for healthy lifestyle changes and is not a substitute for effective medication.[12]

Methodological obstacles

The term meditation encompasses a wide range of practices and interventions rooted in different traditions, but research literature has sometimes failed to adequately specify the nature of the particular meditation practice(s) being studied.[13] Different forms of meditation practice may yield different results depending on the factors being studied.[13]

The presence of a number of intertwined factors including the effects of meditation, the theoretical orientation of how meditation practices are taught, the cultural background of meditators, and generic group effects complicates the task of isolating the effects of meditation:[14]

Numerous studies have demonstrated the beneficial effects of a variety of meditation practices. It has been unclear to what extent these practices share neural correlates. Interestingly, a recent study compared electroencephalogram activity during a focused-attention and open monitoring meditation practice from practitioners of two Buddhist traditions. The researchers found that the differences between the two meditation traditions were more pronounced than the differences between the two types of meditation. These data are consistent with our findings that theoretical orientation of how a practice is taught strongly influences neural activity during these practices. However, the study used long-term practitioners from different cultures, which may have confounded the results.

Research on mindfulness

A previous study commissioned by the US Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality found that meditation interventions reduce multiple negative dimensions of psychological stress.[4] Other systematic reviews and meta-analyses show that mindfulness meditation has several mental health benefits such as bringing about reductions in depression symptoms,[15][16][17] improvements in mood[18], stress-resilience[19], and attentional control[20]. Mindfulness interventions also appear to be a promising intervention for managing depression in youth.[21][22] Mindfulness meditation is useful for managing stress,[16][23][24][25] anxiety,[15][16][24] and also appears to be effective in treating substance use disorders.[26][27][28] A recent meta analysis by Hilton et al. (2016) including 30 randomized controlled trials found high quality evidence for improvement in depressive symptoms.[29] Other review studies have shown that mindfulness meditation can enhance the psychological functioning of breast cancer survivors,[16] is effective for people with eating disorders,[30][31] and may also be effective in treating psychosis.[32][33][34]

Studies have also shown that rumination and worry contribute to mental illnesses such as depression and anxiety,[35] and mindfulness-based interventions are effective in the reduction of worry.[35][36]

Some studies suggest that mindfulness meditation contributes to a more coherent and healthy sense of self and identity, when considering aspects such as sense of responsibility, authenticity, compassion, self-acceptance and character.[37][38]

Brain mechanisms

Main page: Biology:Mechanisms of mindfulness meditation

In 2011, National Center for Complementary and Integrative Health (NCCIH) released findings from a study in which magnetic resonance images were taken of the brains of 16 participants 2 weeks before and after the participants joined the mindfulness meditation (MM) program by researchers from Massachusetts General Hospital, Bender Institute of Neuroimaging in Germany, and the University of Massachusetts Medical School. The researchers concluded:

... these findings may represent an underlying brain mechanism associated with mindfulness-based improvements in mental health.[39]

The analgesic effect of MM involves multiple brain mechanisms including the activation of the anterior cingulate cortex and the ventromedial prefrontal cortex.[40] In addition, brief periods of MM training increases the amount of grey matter in the hippocampus and parietal lobe.[41] Other neural changes resulting from MM may increase the efficiency of attentional control.[42]

Participation in MBSR programmes has been found to correlate with decreases in right basolateral amygdala gray matter density,[43] and increases in gray matter concentration within the left hippocampus.[44]

Changes in the brain

Mindfulness meditation also appears to bring about favorable structural changes in the brain, though more research needs to be done because most of these studies are small and have weak methodology.[45] One recent study found a significant cortical thickness increase in individuals who underwent a brief -8 weeks- MBSR training program and that this increase was coupled with a significant reduction of several psychological indices related to worry, state anxiety, depression.[46] Another study describes how mindfulness based interventions target neurocognitive mechanisms of addiction at the attention-appraisal-emotion interface.[27] A meta-analysis by Fox et al. (2014) using results from 21 brain imaging studies found consistent differences in the region of the prefrontal cortex and other brain regions associated with body awareness. In terms of effect size the mean effect was rated as moderate. (Cohen's d = 0.46) However the results should be interpreted with caution because funnel plots indicate that publication bias is an issue in meditation research.[45] A follow up by Fox et al. (2016) using 78 functional neuro-imaging studies suggests that different meditation styles are reliably associated with different brain activity. Activations in some brain regions are usually accompanied by deactivation in others. This finding suggests that meditation research must put emphasis on comparing practices from the same style of meditation, for example results from studies investigating focused attention methods cannot be compared to results from open monitoring approaches.[47]

Attention and mindfulness

Attention networks and mindfulness meditation

Psychological and Buddhist conceptualisations of mindfulness both highlight awareness and attention training as key components, in which levels of mindfulness can be cultivated with practise of mindfulness meditation.[48][49] Focused attention meditation (FAM) and open monitoring meditation (OMM) are distinct types of mindfulness meditation; FAM refers to the practice of intently maintaining focus on one object, whereas OMM is the progression of general awareness of one's surroundings while regulating thoughts.[50][51]

Focused attention meditation is typically practiced first to increase the ability to enhance attentional stability, and awareness of mental states with the goal being to transition to open monitoring meditation practise that emphasizes the ability to monitor moment-by-moment changes in experience, without a focus of attention to maintain. Mindfulness meditation may lead to greater cognitive flexibility.[52]

In an active randomized controlled study completed in 2019, participants who practiced mindfulness meditation demonstrated a greater improvement in awareness and attention that participants in the active control condition.[53]

Evidence for improvements in three areas of attention

Sustained attention
  • Tasks of sustained attention relate to vigilance and the preparedness that aids completing a particular task goal. Psychological research into the relationship between mindfulness meditation and the sustained attention network have revealed the following:
  • Mindfulness meditators have demonstrated superior performance when the stimulus to be detected in a task was unexpected, relative to when it was expected. This suggests that attention resources were more readily available in order to perform well in the task. This was despite not receiving a visual cue to aid performance. (Valentine & Sweet, 1999).
  • In a continuous performance task[54] an association was found between higher dispositional mindfulness and more stable maintenance of sustained attention.
  • In an EEG study, the Attentional blink effect was reduced, and P3b ERP amplitude decreased in a group of participants who completed a mindfulness retreat.[55] The incidence of reduced attentional blink effect relates to an increase in detectability of a second target. This may have been due to a greater ability to allocate attentional resources for detecting the second target, reflected in a reduced P3b amplitude.
  • A greater degree of attentional resources may also be reflected in faster response times in task performance, as was found for participants with higher levels of mindfulness experience.[56]
Selective attention
  • Selective attention as linked with the orientation network, is involved in selecting the relevant stimuli to attend to.
  • Performance in the ability to limit attention to potentially sensory inputs (i.e. selective attention) was found to be higher following the completion of an 8-week MBSR course, compared to a one-month retreat and control group (with no mindfulness training).[56] The ANT task is a general applicable task designed to test the three attention networks, in which participants are required to determine the direction of a central arrow on a computer screen.[57] Efficiency in orienting that represent the capacity to selectively attend to stimuli was calculated by examining changes in the reaction time that accompanied cues indicating where the target occurred relative to the aid of no cues.
  • Meditation experience was found to correlate negatively with reaction times on an Eriksen flanker task measuring responses to global and local figures. Similar findings have been observed for correlations between mindfulness experience in an orienting score of response times taken from Attention Network Task performance.[58]
  • Participants who engaged in the Meditation Breath Attention Score exercise performed better on anagram tasks and reported greater focused attention on this task compared to those who did not undergo this exercise.[59]
Executive control attention
  • Executive control attention include functions of inhibiting the conscious processing of distracting information. In the context of mindful meditation, distracting information relates to attention grabbing mental events such as thoughts related to the future or past.[51]
  • More than one study have reported findings of a reduced Stroop effect following mindfulness meditation training.[52][60][61] The Stroop effect indexes interference created by having words printed in colour that differ to the read semantic meaning e.g. green printed in red. However findings for this task are not consistently found.[62][63] For instance the MBSR may differ to how mindful one becomes relative to a person who is already high in trait mindfulness.[42]
  • Using the Attention Network Task (a version of Eriksen flanker task [57]) it was found that error scores that indicate executive control performance were reduced in experienced meditators [56] and following a brief 5 session mindfulness training program.[60]
  • A neuroimaging study supports behavioural research findings that higher levels of mindfulness are associated with greater proficiency to inhibit distracting information. As greater activation of the rostral anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) was shown for mindfulness meditators than matched controls.
  • Participants with at least 6 years of experience meditating performed better on the Stroop Test compared to participants who had not had experience meditating.[64] The group of meditators also had lower reaction times during this test than the group of non-meditators.[64]
  • Following a Stroop test, reduced amplitude of the P3 ERP component was found for a meditation group relative to control participants. This was taken to signify that mindfulness meditation improves executive control functions of attention. An increased amplitude in the N2 ERP component was also observed in the mindfulness meditation group, thought to reflect more efficient perceptual discrimination in earlier stages of perceptual processing.[65]

Emotion regulation and mindfulness

Research shows meditation practices lead to greater emotional regulation abilities. Mindfulness can help people become more aware of thoughts in the present moment, and this increased self-awareness leads to better processing and control over one's responses to surroundings or circumstances.[66][67]

Positive effects of this heightened awareness include a greater sense of empathy for others, an increase in positive patterns of thinking, and a reduction in anxiety.[67][66] Reductions in rumination also have been found following mindfulness meditation practice, contributing to the development of positive thinking and emotional well-being.

Evidence of mindfulness and emotion regulation outcomes

Emotional reactivity can be measured and reflected in brain regions related to the production of emotions.[68] It can also be reflected in tests of attentional performance, indexed in poorer performance in attention related tasks. The regulation of emotional reactivity as initiated by attentional control capacities can be taxing to performance, as attentional resources are limited.[69]

  • Patients with social anxiety disorder (SAD) exhibited reduced amygdala activation in response to negative self-beliefs following an MBSR intervention program that involves mindfulness meditation practice.[70]
  • The LPP ERP component indexes arousal and is larger in amplitude for emotionally salient stimuli relative to neutral.[71][72][73] Individuals higher in trait mindfulness showed lower LPP responses to high arousal unpleasant images. These findings suggest that individuals with higher trait mindfulness were better able to regulate emotional reactivity to emotionally evocative stimuli.[74]
  • Participants who completed a 7-week mindfulness training program demonstrated a reduction in a measure of emotional interference (measured as slower responses times following the presentation of emotional relative to neutral pictures). This suggests a reduction in emotional interference.[75]
  • Following a MBSR intervention, decreases in social anxiety symptom severity were found, as well as increases in bilateral parietal cortex neural correlates. This is thought to reflect the increased employment of inhibitory attentional control capacities to regulate emotions.[76][77]
  • Participants who engaged in emotion-focus meditation and breathing meditation exhibited delayed emotional response to negatively valanced film stimuli compared to participants who did not engage in any type of meditation.[78]

Controversies in mindful emotion regulation

It is debated as to whether top-down executive control regions such as the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC),[79] are required[77] or not[70] to inhibit reactivity of the amygdala activation related to the production of evoked emotional responses. Arguably an initial increase in activation of executive control regions developed during mindfulness training may lessen with increasing mindfulness expertise.[80]

Stress reduction

Research has shown stress reduction benefits from mindfulness.[81][82][83] A 2019 study tested the effects of meditation on the psychological well-being, work stress, and blood pressure of employees working in the United Kingdom. One group of participants were instructed to meditate once a day using a mindfulness app on their smartphones, while the control group did not engage in meditation. Measurements of well-being, stress, and perceived workplace support were taken for both groups before the intervention and then again after 4 months. Based on self-report questionnaires, the participants who engaged in meditation showed a significant increase in psychological well-being and perceived workplace support. The meditators also reported a significant decrease in anxiety and stress levels.[83]

Other research shows decreased stress levels in people who engage in meditation after shorter periods of time as well. Evidence of significant stress reduction was found after only three weeks of meditation intervention.[84] Brief, daily meditation sessions can alter one's behavioral response to stressors, improving coping mechanisms and decreasing the adverse impact caused be stress.[85][86] A study from 2016 examined anxiety and emotional states of naive meditators before and after a 7-day meditation retreat in Thailand. Results displayed a significant reduction in perceived stress after this traditional Buddhist meditation retreat.[86]

Future directions

A large part of mindfulness research is dependent on technology. As new technology continues to be developed, new imaging techniques will become useful in this field. Real-time fMRI might give immediate feedback and guide participants through the programs. It could also be used to more easily train and evaluate mental states during meditation itself.[87] The new technology in the upcoming years offers many new opportunities for the continued research.

Research on other types of meditation

Cortical Areas Thicker in Meditators .jpg

Insight (Vipassana) meditation

Vipassana meditation is a component of Buddhist practice. Phra Taweepong Inwongsakul and Sampath Kumar from the University of Mysore have been studying the effects of this meditation on 120 students by measuring the associated increase of cortical thickness in the brain. The results of this study are inconclusive.[88][89] Vipassana meditation leads to more than just mindfulness, but has been found to reduce stress, increase well-being and self-kindness.[90] These effects were found to be most powerful short-term, but still had a relatively significant impact 6 months later. In a study conducted by Szekeres and Wertheim (2014), they found stress to be the category that seemed to have the most regression, but the others contained higher prevalence when compared to the participants’ original scores that were given from before embarking on Vipassana meditation. Overall, according to self-reports, Vipassana can have short and long-term effects on an individual.

An essential component to the Vipassana mediation approach is the focus on awareness, referring to bodily sensations and psychological status. In a study conducted by Zeng et al. (2013), awareness was described as the acknowledgement of consciousness which is monitoring all aspects of the environment.[91] This definition differentiates the concept of awareness from mindfulness. The emphasis on awareness, and the way it assists in monitoring emotion, is unique to this meditative practice.

Sahaja yoga and mental silence

Sahaja yoga meditation is regarded as a mental silence meditation, and has been shown to correlate with particular brain[92][93] and brain wave[94][95][96] characteristics. One study has led to suggestions that Sahaja meditation involves 'switching off' irrelevant brain networks for the maintenance of focused internalized attention and inhibition of inappropriate information.[97] Sahaja meditators appear to benefit from lower depression[98] and scored above control group for emotional well-being and mental health measures on SF-36 ratings.[99][100][101]

A study comparing practitioners of Sahaja Yoga meditation with a group of non meditators doing a simple relaxation exercise, measured a drop in skin temperature in the meditators compared to a rise in skin temperature in the non meditators as they relaxed. The researchers noted that all other meditation studies that have observed skin temperature have recorded increases and none have recorded a decrease in skin temperature. This suggests that Sahaja Yoga meditation, being a mental silence approach, may differ both experientially and physiologically from simple relaxation.[96]

Kundalini yoga

Kundalini yoga has proved to increase the prevention of cognitive decline and evaluate the response of biomarkers to treatment, thereby shedding light on the underlying mechanisms of the link between Kundalini Yoga and cognitive impairment. For the study, 81 participants aged 55 and older who had subjective memory complaints and met criteria for mild cognitive impairment, indicated by a total score of 0.5 on the Clinical Dementia Rating Scale. The results showed that at 12 weeks, both the yoga group showed significant improvements in recall memory and visual memory and showed significant sustained improvement in memory up to the 24-week follow-up, the yoga group showed significant improvement in verbal fluency and sustained significant improvements in executive functioning at week 24. In addition, the yoga cohort showed significant improvement in depressive symptoms, apathy, and resilience from emotional stress. This research was provided by Helen Lavretsky, M.D. and colleagues.[102] In another study, Kundalini Yoga did not show significant effectiveness in treating obsessive-compulsive disorders compared with Relaxation/Meditation.[103]

Transcendental Meditation

The first Transcendental Meditation (TM) research studies were conducted at UCLA and Harvard University and published in Science and the American Journal of Physiology in 1970 and 1971.[104] However, much research has been of poor quality,[1][103][105] including a high risk for bias due to the connection of researchers to the TM organization and the selection of subjects with a favorable opinion of TM.[106][107][108] Independent systematic reviews have not found health benefits for TM exceeding those of relaxation and health education.[1][103][107] A 2013 statement from the American Heart Association described the evidence supporting TM as a treatment for hypertension as Level IIB, meaning that TM "may be considered in clinical practice" but that its effectiveness is "unknown/unclear/uncertain or not well-established".[This quote needs a citation] In another study, TM proved comparable with other kinds of relaxation therapies in reducing anxiety.[103]

Research on unspecified or multiple types of meditation

Brain activity

Main page: Biology:Brain activity and meditation

The medial prefrontal and posterior cingulate cortices have been found to be relatively deactivated during meditation (experienced meditators using concentration, lovingkindness and choiceless awareness meditation). In addition experienced meditators were found to have stronger coupling between the posterior cingulate, dorsal anterior cingulate, and dorsolateral prefrontal cortices both when meditating and when not meditating.[109] Over time meditation can actually increase the integrity of both gray and white matter. The added amount of gray matter found in the brain stem after meditation improves communication between the cortex and all other areas within the brain.[110][111] Meditation often stimulates a large network of cortical regions including the frontal and parietal regions, lateral occipital lobe, the insular cortex, thalamic nuclei, basal ganglia, and the cerebellum region in the brain. These parts of the brain are connected with attention and the default network of the brain which is associated to day dreaming.[112]

In addition, both meditation and yoga have been found to have impacts on the brain, specifically the caudate. Here, there is greater connectivity to the caudate, as well as the interaction of basal ganglia cortico-thalamic feedback loops within meditation and yoga practice.[113] Further studies need to be conducted to show a stronger relationship between these areas of brain involvement with common results of yoga and meditation, (improved mental health and well-being).

One reason that mindfulness-based approaches could have the impact of decreased anxiety is a result of a decrease in activity with the amygdala, the area of the brain that is involved with the “fight or flight” response.[114] Another reason this treatment assists anxious individuals is its ability to reduce cortisol levels. This occurs as levels of glucocorticoids remain elevated and act as a force of resistance against cortisol.[114]

Biological Impact

Mindfulness-based stress reduction has been found to interact and regulate physiological factors such immune, metabolic, cardiovascular, and endocrine systems.[114]

Changes in the brain and neuroplasticity

Meditation has been shown to change grey matter concentrations and the precuneus.[115][44][116][45][43]

An eight-week MBSR course induced changes in gray matter concentrations.[44] Exploratory whole brain analyses identified significant increases in gray matter concentration in the PCC, TPJ, and the cerebellum. These results suggest that participation in MBSR is associated with changes in gray matter concentration in brain regions involved in learning and memory processes, emotion regulation, self-referential processing, and perspective taking. Studies have found that regular meditation practice increases the protein BDNF in the brain and improves cellular health, reduces the rate of aging within cells, and a reduction of grey matter decay in the brain making it possible for higher neuroplasticity over longer periods of time.[117] Meditation has been linked to higher levels of cognitive flexibility and a greater ability to recognize cognitive evaluations and functions.[118] Recent studies suggest that meditation can increase the amount of gray matter in the medulla oblongata located in the brain stem leading to decreased arousal when dealing with unpleasant situations and higher levels of activity in the higher cortical regions of the brain and insula.[110] Meditation has shown to increase activity in anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and enhance communication from the anterior cingulate cortex to the rest of the brain resulting in a better ability to evaluate and resolve cognitive tasks.[119] Studies have shown that meditation actually increases the amount of white matter within the parts of the brain that assist in self regulation and behavior.[119]

Attention/Mind wandering

Meditation has been found to decrease mind wandering and allows the brain to fully focus on challenging tasks for longer periods of time without getting distracted. This is due to an increased ability to reduce activity in the default mode network when focusing on a particular task.[120] Non directive forms of meditation where the meditator lets their mind wander freely can actually produce higher levels of activity in the default mode network when compared to a resting state or having the brain in a neutral place.[120][121] These Non directive forms of meditation allows the meditators to have better control over thoughts during everyday activities or when focusing on specific task due to a reduced frustration at the brains mind wandering process.[121] When given a specific task, meditation can allow quicker response to changing environmental stimuli. Meditation can allow the brain to decrease attention to unwanted responses of irrelevant environmental stimuli and a reduces the Stroop effect. Those who meditate have regularly demonstrated more control on what they focus their attention on while maintaining a mindful awareness on what is around them.[122]  Experienced meditators have been shown to have an increased ability when it comes to conflict monitoring[123] and find it easier to switch between competing stimuli.[124] Those who practice meditation experience an increase of attentional resources in the brain and steady meditation practice can lead to the reduction of the attentional blink due to a decreased mental exertion when identifying important stimuli.[124]

Perception

Studies have shown that meditation has both short-term and long-term effects on various perceptual faculties. In 1984 a study showed that meditators have a significantly lower detection threshold for light stimuli of short duration.[125] In 2000 a study of the perception of visual illusions by zen masters, novice meditators, and non-meditators showed statistically significant effects found for the Poggendorff Illusion but not for the Müller-Lyer Illusion. The zen masters experienced a statistically significant reduction in initial illusion (measured as error in millimeters) and a lower decrement in illusion for subsequent trials.[126] Tloczynski has described the theory of mechanism behind the changes in perception that accompany mindfulness meditation thus: "A person who meditates consequently perceives objects more as directly experienced stimuli and less as concepts… With the removal or minimization of cognitive stimuli and generally increasing awareness, meditation can therefore influence both the quality (accuracy) and quantity (detection) of perception."[126] Brown also points to this as a possible explanation of the phenomenon: "[the higher rate of detection of single light flashes] involves quieting some of the higher mental processes which normally obstruct the perception of subtle events."[This quote needs a citation] In other words, the practice may temporarily or permanently alter some of the top-down processing involved in filtering subtle events usually deemed noise by the perceptual filters.[citation needed]

Memory

Meditation enhances memory capacity specifically in the working memory and increases executive functioning by helping participants better understand what is happening moment for moment. Those who meditate regularly have demonstrated the ability to better process and distinguish important information from the working memory and store it into long term memory with more accuracy than those who do not practice meditation techniques.[111] Meditation may be able to expand the amount of information that can be held with in working memory and by so doing is able to improve IQ scores and increase individual intelligence.[120] The encoding process for both audio and visual information has been shown to be more accurate and detailed when meditation is used.[124] Though there are limited studies on meditation's effects on long-term memory because of meditations ability to increase attentional awareness episodic long-term memory is believed to be more vivid and accurate for those who meditate regularly. Meditation has also shown to decrease memory complaints from those who suffer with Alzheimers disease which also suggests the benefits meditation could have on episodic long-term memory which is linked to Alzheimers.[127]

Calming and relaxation

According to an article in Psychological Bulletin, EEG activity slows as a result of meditation.[128] The National Institutes of Health (NIH) has written, "It is thought that some types of meditation might work by reducing activity in the sympathetic nervous system and increasing activity in the parasympathetic nervous system,"[This quote needs a citation] or equivalently, that meditation produces a reduction in arousal and increase in relaxation.[citation needed]

Herbert Benson, founder of the Mind-Body Medical Institute, which is affiliated with Harvard University and several Boston hospitals, reports that meditation induces a host of biochemical and physical changes in the body collectively referred to as the "relaxation response".[129] The relaxation response includes changes in metabolism, heart rate, respiration, blood pressure and brain chemistry. Benson and his team have also done clinical studies at Buddhist monasteries in the Himalaya n Mountains.[130] Benson wrote The Relaxation Response to document the benefits of meditation, which in 1975 were not yet widely known.[131]

Arousing effects

Although the most common modern characterization of Buddhist meditation is a ‘relaxation’ technique, both scientific studies and Buddhist textual sources proves meditation’s arousing or wake-promoting effects.[132] Meditations aiming at improving meta-cognitive skills and compassion (e.g. loving-kindness meditation) are associated with physiological arousal, compared to breathing meditation.[133] Theravada (i.e.Vipassana) styles of meditation induce relaxation responses, while Vajrayana styles of meditation induce arousal responses.[134] Short term meditation training enables the voluntary activation of the Sympathetic Nervous System (SNS) results in epinephrine release.[135] When the SNS is activated, human body is turning into ‘fight or flight’ mode, whereas the PNS is termed the ‘rest and digest’ mode.[136] For example, when SNS is activated, heart rate, blood pressure, and respiration will be increased, and catecholamines will be produced, while heart rate variability and galvanic skin resistance will be decreased.[136] Therefore, Relaxing meditation seems to correspond to PNS dominance, and arousing meditation seems to correspond to SNS dominance.

Slowing aging

Aging is a process accompanied by a decrease in brain weight and volume. This phenomenon can be explained by structural changes in the brain, namely, a loss of grey matter. Some studies over the last decade have implicated meditation as a protective factor against normal age-related brain atrophy.[137] The first direct evidence for this link emerged from a study investigating changes in the cortical thickness of meditators. The researchers found that regular meditation practice was able to reduce age-related thinning of the frontal cortex, though these findings were restricted to particular regions of the brain.[138] A similar study looked to further expand on this finding by including a behavioural component. Consistent with the previous study, meditators did not show the expected negative correlation between grey matter volume and age. In addition, the results for meditators on the behavioural test, measuring attentional performance, were comparable across all age groups.[139] This implies that meditation can potentially protect against age-related grey matter loss and age-related cognitive decline. Since then, more research has supported the notion that meditation serves as a neuroprotective factor that slows age-related brain atrophy.[137][140] Still, all studies have been cross sectional in design. Furthermore, these results merely describe associations and do not make causal inferences.[141] Further work using longitudinal and experimental designs may help solidify the causal link between meditation and grey matter loss. Since few studies have investigated this direct link, however insightful they may be, there is not sufficient evidence for a conclusive answer.

Research has also been conducted on the malleable determinants of cellular aging in an effort to understand human longevity. Researchers have stated, "We have reviewed data linking stress arousal and oxidative stress to telomere shortness. Meditative practices appear to improve the endocrine balance toward positive arousal (high DHEA, lower cortisol) and decrease oxidative stress. Thus, meditation practices may promote mitotic cell longevity both through decreasing stress hormones and oxidative stress and increasing hormones that may protect the telomere."[142][143]

Happiness and emotional well-being

Studies have shown meditators to have higher happiness than control groups, although this may be due to non-specific factors such as meditators having better general self-care.[144][145][99][98]

Positive relationships have been found between the volume of gray matter in the right precuneus area of the brain and both meditation and the subject's subjective happiness score.[146][115][44][116][45][43] A recent study found that participants who engaged in a body-scan meditation for about 20 minutes self-reported higher levels of happiness and decrease in anxiety compared to participants who just rested during the 20 minute time-span. These results suggest that an increase in awareness of one's body through meditation causes a state of selflessness and a feeling of connectedness. This result then leads to reports of positive emotions.[147]

A technique known as Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction (MBSR) displays significant benefits for mental health and coping behaviors. Participants who had no prior experience with MBSR reported a significant increase in happiness after 8 weeks of MBSR practice. Focus on the present moment and increased awareness of one's thoughts can help monitor and reduce judgment or negative thoughts, causing a report of higher emotional well-being.[148] The MBSR program and evidence for its effectiveness is described in Jon Kabat-Zinn's book Full Catastrophe Living.[149]

Potential adverse effects and limits of meditation

The following is an official statement from the US government-run National Center for Complementary and Integrative Health:

"Meditation is considered to be safe for healthy people. There have been rare reports that meditation could cause or worsen symptoms in people who have certain psychiatric problems, but this question has not been fully researched. People with physical limitations may not be able to participate in certain meditative practices involving physical movement. Individuals with existing mental or physical health conditions should speak with their health care providers prior to starting a meditative practice and make their meditation instructor aware of their condition."[150]

Adverse effects have been reported,[151][152] and may, in some cases, be the result of "improper use of meditation".[153] The NIH advises prospective meditators to "ask about the training and experience of the meditation instructor… [they] are considering."[150]

As with any practice, meditation may also be used to avoid facing ongoing problems or emerging crises in the meditator's life. In such situations, it may instead be helpful to apply mindful attitudes acquired in meditation while actively engaging with current problems.[154][155] According to the NIH, meditation should not be used as a replacement for conventional health care or as a reason to postpone seeing a doctor.[150]

Pain

Meditation has been show to reduce pain perception.[156] An intervention known as mindfulness-based pain management (MBPM) has been subject to a range of studies demonstrating its effectiveness.[157][158]

See also

References

  1. 1.0 1.1 1.2 1.3 1.4 1.5 "Meditation practices for health: state of the research". Evidence Report/Technology Assessment (155): 1–263. June 2007. PMID 17764203. PMC 4780968. http://www.ahrq.gov/downloads/pub/evidence/pdf/meditation/medit.pdf. 
  2. Lutz, Antoine; Dunne, John D.; Davidson, Richard J. (2007). "Meditation and the Neuroscience of Consciousness: An Introduction". in Zelazo, Philip David; Moscovitch, Morris; Thompson, Evan. The Cambridge Handbook of Consciousness. Cambridge Handbooks in Psychology. Cambridge University Press. pp. 499–552. doi:10.1017/CBO9780511816789.020. ISBN 978-0-511-81678-9. https://semanticscholar.org/paper/175754d83888a4164b84f7310dc8d23274403976. 
  3. 3.0 3.1 "Clinical trials of meditation practices in health care: characteristics and quality". Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine 14 (10): 1199–213. December 2008. doi:10.1089/acm.2008.0307. PMID 19123875. https://semanticscholar.org/paper/50410f7dadaadfd9c34e7d0138a627032154ef4e. 
  4. 4.0 4.1 "Meditation programs for psychological stress and well-being: a systematic review and meta-analysis". JAMA Internal Medicine 174 (3): 357–68. March 2014. doi:10.1001/jamainternmed.2013.13018. PMID 24395196. 
  5. 5.0 5.1 "Mind the Hype: A Critical Evaluation and Prescriptive Agenda for Research on Mindfulness and Meditation". Perspectives on Psychological Science 13 (1): 36–61. January 2018. doi:10.1177/1745691617709589. PMID 29016274. 
  6. 6.0 6.1 Stetka, Bret (October 2017). "Where's the Proof That Mindfulness Meditation Works?". Scientific American. doi:10.1038/scientificamericanmind0118-20. https://www.scientificamerican.com/article/wheres-the-proof-that-mindfulness-meditation-works1/. 
  7. 7.0 7.1 "Special issue on mindfulness neuroscience". Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 8 (1): 1–3. January 2013. doi:10.1093/scan/nss104. PMID 22956677. 
  8. "Foreword to Advances in Meditation Research: neuroscience and clinical applications". Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1307 (1): v–vi. January 2014. doi:10.1111/nyas.12305. PMID 24571183. Bibcode2014NYASA1307D...5S. 
  9. Awasthi B (2013). "Issues and perspectives in meditation research: in search for a definition". Frontiers in Psychology 3: 613. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00613. PMID 23335908. 
  10. "Reiterated Concerns and Further Challenges for Mindfulness and Meditation Research: A Reply to Davidson and Dahl". Perspectives on Psychological Science 13 (1): 66–69. January 2018. doi:10.1177/1745691617727529. PMID 29016240. 
  11. "Beyond Medications and Diet: Alternative Approaches to Lowering Blood Pressure: A Scientific Statement from the American Heart Association". Hypertension 61 (6): 1360–83. June 2013. doi:10.1161/HYP.0b013e318293645f. PMID 23608661. 
  12. "Meditation to Boost Health and Well-Being". https://www.heart.org/en/healthy-living/healthy-lifestyle/mental-health-and-wellbeing/meditation-to-boost-health-and-wellbeing. 
  13. 13.0 13.1 Davidson, Richard J.; Kaszniak, Alfred W. (October 2015). "Conceptual and Methodological Issues in Research on Mindfulness and Meditation". American Psychologist 70 (7): 581–592. doi:10.1037/a0039512. PMID 26436310. 
  14. "Common and Dissociable Neural Activity After Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction and Relaxation Response Programs". Psychosomatic Medicine 80 (5): 439–451. June 2018. doi:10.1097/PSY.0000000000000590. PMID 29642115. 
  15. 15.0 15.1 "Mindfulness-based interventions for people diagnosed with a current episode of an anxiety or depressive disorder: a meta-analysis of randomised controlled trials". PLOS ONE 9 (4): e96110. Apr 2014. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0096110. PMID 24763812. Bibcode2014PLoSO...996110S. 
  16. 16.0 16.1 16.2 16.3 "Mindfulness-based stress reduction for healthy individuals: A meta-analysis". Journal of Psychosomatic Research 78 (6): 519–28. June 2015. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychores.2015.03.009. PMID 25818837. 
  17. "Critical analysis of the efficacy of meditation therapies for acute and subacute phase treatment of depressive disorders: a systematic review". Psychosomatics 56 (2): 140–52. 2014. doi:10.1016/j.psym.2014.10.007. PMID 25591492. PMC 4383597. http://www.escholarship.org/uc/item/0372c9xp. 
  18. Walsh, Kathleen Marie; Saab, Bechara J; Farb, Norman AS (2019-01-08). "Effects of a Mindfulness Meditation App on Subjective Well-Being: Active Randomized Controlled Trial and Experience Sampling Study" (in en). JMIR Mental Health 6 (1): e10844. doi:10.2196/10844. ISSN 2368-7959. PMID 30622094. 
  19. Walsh, Kathleen Marie; Saab, Bechara J; Farb, Norman AS (2019-01-08). "Effects of a Mindfulness Meditation App on Subjective Well-Being: Active Randomized Controlled Trial and Experience Sampling Study" (in en). JMIR Mental Health 6 (1): e10844. doi:10.2196/10844. ISSN 2368-7959. PMID 30622094. 
  20. Walsh, Kathleen Marie; Saab, Bechara J; Farb, Norman AS (2019-01-08). "Effects of a Mindfulness Meditation App on Subjective Well-Being: Active Randomized Controlled Trial and Experience Sampling Study" (in en). JMIR Mental Health 6 (1): e10844. doi:10.2196/10844. ISSN 2368-7959. PMID 30622094. 
  21. "Meditation and mindfulness in clinical practice". Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America 23 (3): 487–534. July 2014. doi:10.1016/j.chc.2014.03.002. PMID 24975623. 
  22. "Mindfulness Interventions with Youth: A Meta-Analysis". Mindfulness 59 (4): 297–302. Jan 2014. doi:10.1093/sw/swu030. PMID 25365830. https://semanticscholar.org/paper/d2cfe8c682e240cc1f6a6e00d5a0a96610b1379a. 
  23. "Mindfulness-based stress reduction as a stress management intervention for healthy individuals: a systematic review". Journal of Evidence-Based Complementary & Alternative Medicine 19 (4): 271–86. October 2014. doi:10.1177/2156587214543143. PMID 25053754. 
  24. 24.0 24.1 "The effect of mindfulness-based therapy on anxiety and depression: A meta-analytic review". Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 78 (2): 169–83. April 2010. doi:10.1037/a0018555. PMID 20350028. 
  25. Walsh, Kathleen Marie; Saab, Bechara J; Farb, Norman AS (2019-01-08). "Effects of a Mindfulness Meditation App on Subjective Well-Being: Active Randomized Controlled Trial and Experience Sampling Study" (in en). JMIR Mental Health 6 (1): e10844. doi:10.2196/10844. ISSN 2368-7959. PMID 30622094. 
  26. "Are mindfulness-based interventions effective for substance use disorders? A systematic review of the evidence". Substance Use & Misuse 49 (5): 492–512. April 2014. doi:10.3109/10826084.2013.770027. PMID 23461667. 
  27. 27.0 27.1 "Mindfulness training targets neurocognitive mechanisms of addiction at the attention-appraisal-emotion interface". Frontiers in Psychiatry 4 (173): 173. January 2014. doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2013.00173. PMID 24454293. 
  28. "Mindfulness-based interventions: an antidote to suffering in the context of substance use, misuse, and addiction". Substance Use & Misuse 49 (5): 487–91. April 2014. doi:10.3109/10826084.2014.860749. PMID 24611846. 
  29. "Mindfulness Meditation for Chronic Pain: Systematic Review and Meta-analysis". Annals of Behavioral Medicine 51 (2): 199–213. April 2017. doi:10.1007/s12160-016-9844-2. PMID 27658913. 
  30. "Mindfulness-based interventions for binge eating: a systematic review and meta-analysis". Journal of Behavioral Medicine 38 (2): 348–62. April 2015. doi:10.1007/s10865-014-9610-5. PMID 25417199. 
  31. "Mindfulness and weight loss: a systematic review". Psychosomatic Medicine 77 (1): 59–67. January 2015. doi:10.1097/PSY.0000000000000127. PMID 25490697. 
  32. "Do mindfulness-based therapies have a role in the treatment of psychosis?". The Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry 48 (2): 124–7. February 2014. doi:10.1177/0004867413512688. PMID 24220133. http://irep.ntu.ac.uk/id/eprint/26548/1/PubSub3164_Griffiths.pdf. 
  33. "Mindfulness for psychosis". The British Journal of Psychiatry 204 (5): 333–4. May 2014. doi:10.1192/bjp.bp.113.136044. PMID 24785766. 
  34. "Mindfulness interventions for psychosis: a meta-analysis". Schizophrenia Research 150 (1): 176–84. October 2013. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2013.07.055. PMID 23954146. 
  35. 35.0 35.1 "Assessing treatments used to reduce rumination and/or worry: a systematic review". Clinical Psychology Review 33 (8): 996–1009. December 2013. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2013.08.004. PMID 24036088. http://epubs.surrey.ac.uk/805216/6/Querstret%26Cropley_2013.pdf. 
  36. "How do mindfulness-based cognitive therapy and mindfulness-based stress reduction improve mental health and wellbeing? A systematic review and meta-analysis of meditation studies". Clinical Psychology Review 37: 1–12. April 2015. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2015.01.006. PMID 25689576. 
  37. "Mindfulness meditation and explicit and implicit indicators of personality and self-concept changes". Frontiers in Psychology 6: 44. 2015. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00044. PMID 25688222. 
  38. "Improving personality/character traits in individuals with alcohol dependence: the influence of mindfulness-oriented meditation". Journal of Addictive Diseases 34 (1): 75–87. 2015. doi:10.1080/10550887.2014.991657. PMID 25585050. 
  39. "Research Spotlight: Mindfulness Meditation Is Associated With Structural Changes in the Brain". NCCIH. January 30, 2011. http://nccih.nih.gov/research/results/spotlight/012311.htm. 
  40. "Mindfulness meditation-related pain relief: evidence for unique brain mechanisms in the regulation of pain". Neuroscience Letters 520 (2): 165–73. June 2012. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2012.03.082. PMID 22487846. 
  41. "Neuromodulatory treatments for chronic pain: efficacy and mechanisms". Nature Reviews. Neurology 10 (3): 167–78. March 2014. doi:10.1038/nrneurol.2014.12. PMID 24535464. 
  42. 42.0 42.1 "Neural mechanisms of attentional control in mindfulness meditation". Frontiers in Neuroscience 7: 8. 2013. doi:10.3389/fnins.2013.00008. PMID 23382709. 
  43. 43.0 43.1 43.2 "Stress reduction correlates with structural changes in the amygdala". Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 5 (1): 11–7. March 2010. doi:10.1093/scan/nsp034. PMID 19776221. 
  44. 44.0 44.1 44.2 44.3 "Mindfulness practice leads to increases in regional brain gray matter density". Psychiatry Research 191 (1): 36–43. January 2011. doi:10.1016/j.pscychresns.2010.08.006. PMID 21071182. 
  45. 45.0 45.1 45.2 45.3 "Is meditation associated with altered brain structure? A systematic review and meta-analysis of morphometric neuroimaging in meditation practitioners". Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 43: 48–73. June 2014. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.03.016. PMID 24705269. 
  46. "Interaction between neuroanatomical and psychological changes after mindfulness-based training". PLOS ONE 9 (10): e108359. Oct 2014. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0108359. PMID 25330321. Bibcode2014PLoSO...9j8359S. 
  47. "Functional neuroanatomy of meditation: A review and meta-analysis of 78 functional neuroimaging investigations". Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 65: 208–28. June 2016. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.03.021. PMID 27032724. Bibcode2016arXiv160306342F. 
  48. "Mindfulness‐based interventions in context: past, present, and future". Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice 10 (2): 144–156. 2003. doi:10.1093/clipsy/bpg016. 
  49. Walsh, Kathleen Marie; Saab, Bechara J; Farb, Norman AS (2019-01-08). "Effects of a Mindfulness Meditation App on Subjective Well-Being: Active Randomized Controlled Trial and Experience Sampling Study" (in en). JMIR Mental Health 6 (1): e10844. doi:10.2196/10844. ISSN 2368-7959. PMID 30622094. 
  50. "Focused attention, open monitoring and loving kindness meditation: effects on attention, conflict monitoring, and creativity - A review". Frontiers in Psychology 5: 1083. 2014. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01083. PMID 25295025. 
  51. 51.0 51.1 "Attention regulation and monitoring in meditation". Trends in Cognitive Sciences 12 (4): 163–9. April 2008. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2008.01.005. PMID 18329323. 
  52. 52.0 52.1 "Meditation, mindfulness and cognitive flexibility". Consciousness and Cognition 18 (1): 176–86. March 2009. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2008.12.008. PMID 19181542. 
  53. Walsh, Kathleen Marie; Saab, Bechara J; Farb, Norman AS (2019-01-08). "Effects of a Mindfulness Meditation App on Subjective Well-Being: Active Randomized Controlled Trial and Experience Sampling Study" (in en). JMIR Mental Health 6 (1): e10844. doi:10.2196/10844. ISSN 2368-7959. PMID 30622094. 
  54. "The relation between self-report mindfulness and performance on tasks of sustained attention". Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment 31 (1): 60–66. 2009. doi:10.1007/s10862-008-9086-0. 
  55. "Mental training affects distribution of limited brain resources". PLOS Biology 5 (6): e138. June 2007. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.0050138. PMID 17488185. 
  56. 56.0 56.1 56.2 "Mindfulness training modifies subsystems of attention". Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience 7 (2): 109–19. June 2007. doi:10.3758/cabn.7.2.109. PMID 17672382. 
  57. 57.0 57.1 "Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 14 (3): 340–7. April 2002. doi:10.1162/089892902317361886. PMID 11970796. 
  58. "Greater efficiency in attentional processing related to mindfulness meditation". Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 63 (6): 1168–80. June 2010. doi:10.1080/17470210903249365. PMID 20509209. 
  59. Green, Joseph P.; Black, Katharine N. (2017). "Meditation-focused attention with the MBAS and solving anagrams.". Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice 4 (4): 348–366. doi:10.1037/cns0000113. ISSN 2326-5531. 
  60. 60.0 60.1 "Short-term meditation training improves attention and self-regulation". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 104 (43): 17152–6. October 2007. doi:10.1073/pnas.0707678104. PMID 17940025. Bibcode2007PNAS..10417152T. 
  61. "Effects of level of meditation experience on attentional focus: is the efficiency of executive or orientation networks improved?". Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine 13 (6): 651–7. 2007. doi:10.1089/acm.2007.7022. PMID 17718648. https://semanticscholar.org/paper/17ed959ece2bc6ee416f214bb4aaf534d27da133. 
  62. "Mindfulness-based stress reduction and attentional control". Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy 14 (6): 449–463. 2007. doi:10.1002/cpp.544. 
  63. "How Does Mindfulness Meditation Work? Proposing Mechanisms of Action From a Conceptual and Neural Perspective". Perspectives on Psychological Science 6 (6): 537–59. November 2011. doi:10.1177/1745691611419671. PMID 26168376. https://semanticscholar.org/paper/73b8a5091dc9a8a7ea60b81641689bb5ccc5aea1. 
  64. 64.0 64.1 "Long-term meditation: the relationship between cognitive processes, thinking styles and mindfulness". Cognitive Processing 19 (1): 73–85. February 2018. doi:10.1007/s10339-017-0844-3. PMID 29110263. 
  65. "Regular, brief mindfulness meditation practice improves electrophysiological markers of attentional control". Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 6: 18. 2012. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2012.00018. PMID 22363278. 
  66. 66.0 66.1 Chawla, Neharika; Marlatt, G. Alan (2010). "Mindlessness-Mindfulness". The Corsini Encyclopedia of Psychology. American Cancer Society. pp. 1–2. doi:10.1002/9780470479216.corpsy0549. ISBN 9780470479216. 
  67. 67.0 67.1 Baer, Ruth A. (2003). "Mindfulness Training as a Clinical Intervention: A Conceptual and Empirical Review". Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice 10 (2): 125–143. doi:10.1093/clipsy.bpg015. 
  68. "The cognitive control of emotion". Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9 (5): 242–9. May 2005. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2005.03.010. PMID 15866151. 
  69. "Research on attention networks as a model for the integration of psychological science". Annual Review of Psychology 58: 1–23. 2007. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085516. PMID 17029565. https://semanticscholar.org/paper/084ab2c74d9244e562de3730c433cd9e4043a68f. 
  70. 70.0 70.1 "Effects of mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR) on emotion regulation in social anxiety disorder". Emotion 10 (1): 83–91. February 2010. doi:10.1037/a0018441. PMID 20141305. 
  71. "Brain potentials in affective picture processing: covariation with autonomic arousal and affective report". Biological Psychology 52 (2): 95–111. March 2000. doi:10.1016/s0301-0511(99)00044-7. PMID 10699350. http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bsz:352-opus-21021. 
  72. "Affective picture processing: the late positive potential is modulated by motivational relevance". Psychophysiology 37 (2): 257–61. March 2000. doi:10.1111/1469-8986.3720257. PMID 10731776. http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bsz:352-opus-21050. 
  73. "Attention and emotion: an ERP analysis of facilitated emotional stimulus processing". NeuroReport 14 (8): 1107–10. June 2003. doi:10.1097/00001756-200306110-00002. PMID 12821791. 
  74. "Dispositional mindfulness and the attenuation of neural responses to emotional stimuli". Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 8 (1): 93–9. January 2013. doi:10.1093/scan/nss004. PMID 22253259. 
  75. "Mindfulness meditation and reduced emotional interference on a cognitive task". Motivation and Emotion 31 (4): 271–283. 2007. doi:10.1007/s11031-007-9076-7. 
  76. "MBSR vs aerobic exercise in social anxiety: fMRI of emotion regulation of negative self-beliefs". Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 8 (1): 65–72. January 2013. doi:10.1093/scan/nss054. PMID 22586252. 
  77. 77.0 77.1 "Attending to the present: mindfulness meditation reveals distinct neural modes of self-reference". Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 2 (4): 313–22. December 2007. doi:10.1093/scan/nsm030. PMID 18985137. 
  78. "Breath Versus Emotions: The Impact of Different Foci of Attention During Mindfulness Meditation on the Experience of Negative and Positive Emotions". Behavior Therapy 49 (5): 702–714. September 2018. doi:10.1016/j.beth.2017.12.006. PMID 30146138. 
  79. "Prefrontal involvement in the regulation of emotion: convergence of rat and human studies". Current Opinion in Neurobiology 16 (6): 723–7. December 2006. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2006.07.004. PMID 17084617. 
  80. "Does mindfulness training improve cognitive abilities? A systematic review of neuropsychological findings". Clinical Psychology Review 31 (3): 449–64. April 2011. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2010.11.003. PMID 21183265. 
  81. "Common and Dissociable Neural Activity After Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction and Relaxation Response Programs". Psychosomatic Medicine 80 (5): 439–451. June 2018. doi:10.1097/PSY.0000000000000590. PMID 29642115. 
  82. "Mindfulness, Meditation, Relaxation Response Have Different Effects on Brain Function". 2018-06-13. https://www.laboratoryequipment.com/news/2018/06/mindfulness-meditation-relaxation-response-have-different-effects-brain-function. 
  83. 83.0 83.1 Bostock, Sophie; Crosswell, Alexandra D.; Prather, Aric A.; Steptoe, Andrew (2019). "Mindfulness on-the-go: Effects of a mindfulness meditation app on work stress and well-being." (in en). Journal of Occupational Health Psychology 24 (1): 127–138. doi:10.1037/ocp0000118. ISSN 1939-1307. PMID 29723001. 
  84. Walsh, Kathleen Marie; Saab, Bechara J; Farb, Norman AS (2019-01-08). "Effects of a Mindfulness Meditation App on Subjective Well-Being: Active Randomized Controlled Trial and Experience Sampling Study" (in en). JMIR Mental Health 6 (1): e10844. doi:10.2196/10844. ISSN 2368-7959. PMID 30622094. 
  85. Basso, Julia C.; McHale, Alexandra; Ende, Victoria; Oberlin, Douglas J.; Suzuki, Wendy A. (2019). "Brief, daily meditation enhances attention, memory, mood, and emotional regulation in non-experienced meditators". Behavioural Brain Research 356: 208–220. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2018.08.023. ISSN 0166-4328. PMID 30153464. 
  86. 86.0 86.1 Surinrut, Piyawan; Auamnoy, Titinun; Sangwatanaroj, Somkiat (2016). "Enhanced happiness and stress alleviation upon insight meditation retreat: mindfulness, a part of traditional Buddhist meditation". Mental Health, Religion & Culture 19 (7): 648–659. doi:10.1080/13674676.2016.1207618. ISSN 1367-4676. 
  87. "Tools of the trade: theory and method in mindfulness neuroscience". Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 8 (1): 118–20. January 2013. doi:10.1093/scan/nss112. PMID 23081977. 
  88. Inwongsakul PT (September 2015). Impact of vipassana meditation on life satisfaction and quality of life (Ph.D. thesis). University of Mysore.
  89. Dargah M (April 2017). The Impact of Vipassana Meditation on Quality of Life (Ph.D. thesis). The Chicago School of Professional Psychology.
  90. Szekeres, Roberta A.; Wertheim, Eleanor H. (December 2015). "Evaluation of Vipassana Meditation Course Effects on Subjective Stress, Well-being, Self-kindness and Mindfulness in a Community Sample: Post-course and 6-month Outcomes: Vipassana , Stress, Mindfulness and Well-being" (in en). Stress and Health 31 (5): 373–381. doi:10.1002/smi.2562. PMID 24515781. 
  91. Zeng, Xianglong; Oei, Tian P. S.; Liu, Xiangping (December 2014). "Monitoring Emotion Through Body Sensation: A Review of Awareness in Goenka's Vipassana" (in en). Journal of Religion and Health 53 (6): 1693–1705. doi:10.1007/s10943-013-9754-6. ISSN 0022-4197. PMID 23846450. 
  92. "Monitoring the neural activity of the state of mental silence while practicing Sahaja yoga meditation". Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine 21 (3): 175–9. March 2015. doi:10.1089/acm.2013.0450. PMID 25671603. 
  93. "Gray Matter and Functional Connectivity in Anterior Cingulate Cortex are Associated with the State of Mental Silence During Sahaja Yoga Meditation". Neuroscience 371: 395–406. February 2018. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2017.12.017. PMID 29275207. 
  94. "Human anterior and frontal midline theta and lower alpha reflect emotionally positive state and internalized attention: high-resolution EEG investigation of meditation". Neuroscience Letters 310 (1): 57–60. September 2001. doi:10.1016/S0304-3940(01)02094-8. PMID 11524157. 
  95. "Impact of regular meditation practice on EEG activity at rest and during evoked negative emotions". The International Journal of Neuroscience 115 (6): 893–909. June 2005. doi:10.1080/00207450590897969. PMID 16019582. 
  96. 96.0 96.1 Manocha, Ramesh; Black, Deborah; Spiro, David; Ryan, Jake; Stough, Con (March 2010). "Changing Definitions of Meditation – Is there a Physiological Corollary? Skin temperature changes of a mental silence orientated form of meditation compared to rest". Journal of the International Society of Life Sciences 28 (1): 23–31. http://www.researchingmeditation.org/meditation_research/skintemp.pdf. 
  97. "Non-linear dynamic complexity of the human EEG during meditation". Neuroscience Letters 330 (2): 143–6. September 2002. doi:10.1016/S0304-3940(02)00745-0. PMID 12231432. 
  98. 98.0 98.1 "The effects of Sahaja Yoga meditation on mental health: a systematic review". Journal of Complementary & Integrative Medicine 15 (3). May 2018. doi:10.1515/jcim-2016-0163. PMID 29847314. 
  99. 99.0 99.1 "Quality of life and functional health status of long-term meditators". Evidence-Based Complementary and Alternative Medicine 2012: 1–9. 2012. doi:10.1155/2012/350674. PMID 22611427. 
  100. Manocha, Ramesh (2014). "Meditation, mindfulness and mind-emptiness". Acta Neuropsychiatrica 23: 46–7. doi:10.1111/j.1601-5215.2010.00519.x. 
  101. Morgon A. Sahaja Yoga: an Ancient Path to Modern Mental Health? (Doctor of Clinical Psychology thesis). University of Plymouth.
  102. Watts, Vabren (2016). "Kundalini Yoga Found to Enhance Cognitive Functioning in Older Adults". Psychiatric News 51 (9): 1. doi:10.1176/appi.pn.2016.4b11. 
  103. 103.0 103.1 103.2 103.3 "Meditation therapy for anxiety disorders". The Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews (1): CD004998. January 2006. doi:10.1002/14651858.CD004998.pub2. PMID 16437509. 
  104. Lyn Freeman, Mosby’s Complementary & Alternative Medicine: A Research-Based Approach, Mosby Elsevier, 2009, p. 163
  105. Chapter 51: Complementary and Alternative Approaches to Management of Patients with Heart Disease (9th ed.). Saunders. 2011. ISBN 978-1-4377-2708-1. "A systematic review of six RCTs of transcendental meditation failed to generate convincing evidence that meditation is an effective treatment for hypertension"  (References the same 2004 systematic review by Canter and Ernst on TM and hypertension that is separately referenced in this article)
  106. "Insufficient evidence to conclude whether or not Transcendental Meditation decreases blood pressure: results of a systematic review of randomized clinical trials". Journal of Hypertension 22 (11): 2049–54. November 2004. doi:10.1097/00004872-200411000-00002. PMID 15480084. 
  107. 107.0 107.1 "Meditation therapies for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)". The Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews (6): CD006507. June 2010. doi:10.1002/14651858.CD006507.pub2. PMID 20556767. 
  108. "The cumulative effects of Transcendental Meditation on cognitive function--a systematic review of randomised controlled trials". Wiener Klinische Wochenschrift 115 (21–22): 758–66. November 2003. doi:10.1007/BF03040500. PMID 14743579. 
  109. "Meditation experience is associated with differences in default mode network activity and connectivity". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 108 (50): 20254–9. December 2011. doi:10.1073/pnas.1112029108. PMID 22114193. Bibcode2011PNAS..10820254B. 
  110. 110.0 110.1 Baer, Ruth (2010-05-01) (in en). Assessing Mindfulness and Acceptance Processes in Clients: Illuminating the Theory and Practice of Change. New Harbinger Publications. ISBN 978-1-60882-263-8. https://books.google.com/books?id=DlnH-qadA08C&pg=PA185&dq=meditation+and+neuroplasticity#v=onepage. 
  111. 111.0 111.1 Chambers, Richard; Lo, Barbara Chuen Yee; Allen, Nicholas B. (2008-06-01). "The Impact of Intensive Mindfulness Training on Attentional Control, Cognitive Style, and Affect" (in en). Cognitive Therapy and Research 32 (3): 303–322. doi:10.1007/s10608-007-9119-0. ISSN 1573-2819. 
  112. Brefczynski-Lewis, J. A.; Lutz, A.; Schaefer, H. S.; Levinson, D. B.; Davidson, R. J. (2007-07-03). "Neural correlates of attentional expertise in long-term meditation practitioners" (in en). Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 104 (27): 11483–11488. doi:10.1073/pnas.0606552104. ISSN 0027-8424. PMID 17596341. Bibcode2007PNAS..10411483B. 
  113. Gard, Tim; Taquet, Maxime; Dixit, Rohan; Hölzel, Britta K.; Dickerson, Bradford C.; Lazar, Sara W. (2015-03-16). "Greater widespread functional connectivity of the caudate in older adults who practice kripalu yoga and vipassana meditation than in controls". Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 9: 137. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2015.00137. ISSN 1662-5161. PMID 25852521. 
  114. 114.0 114.1 114.2 Reive, Carol (July 2019). "The Biological Measurements of Mindfulness-based Stress Reduction: A Systematic Review" (in en). Explore 15 (4): 295–307. doi:10.1016/j.explore.2019.01.001. PMID 30765300. 
  115. 115.0 115.1 "Brain Gray Matter Changes Associated with Mindfulness Meditation in Older Adults: An Exploratory Pilot Study using Voxel-based Morphometry". Neuro 1 (1): 23–26. 2014. doi:10.17140/NOJ-1-106. PMID 25632405. 
  116. 116.0 116.1 "Shifting brain asymmetry: the link between meditation and structural lateralization". Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 10 (1): 55–61. January 2015. doi:10.1093/scan/nsu029. PMID 24643652. 
  117. Tolahunase, Madhuri R.; Sagar, Rajesh; Faiq, Muneeb; Dada, Rima (2018-01-01). "Yoga- and meditation-based lifestyle intervention increases neuroplasticity and reduces severity of major depressive disorder: A randomized controlled trial" (in en). Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience 36 (3): 423–442. doi:10.3233/RNN-170810. ISSN 0922-6028. PMID 29614706. https://content.iospress.com/articles/restorative-neurology-and-neuroscience/rnn170810. 
  118. Moore, Adam; Malinowski, Peter (2009-03-01). "Meditation, mindfulness and cognitive flexibility" (in en). Consciousness and Cognition 18 (1): 176–186. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2008.12.008. ISSN 1053-8100. PMID 19181542. http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1053810008001967. 
  119. 119.0 119.1 Tang, Yi-Yuan; Lu, Qilin; Fan, Ming; Yang, Yihong; Posner, Michael I. (2012-06-26). "Mechanisms of white matter changes induced by meditation" (in en). Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 109 (26): 10570–10574. doi:10.1073/pnas.1207817109. ISSN 0027-8424. PMID 22689998. Bibcode2012PNAS..10910570T. 
  120. 120.0 120.1 120.2 Mrazek, Michael D.; Franklin, Michael S.; Phillips, Dawa Tarchin; Baird, Benjamin; Schooler, Jonathan W. (2013-03-28). "Mindfulness Training Improves Working Memory Capacity and GRE Performance While Reducing Mind Wandering" (in en-US). Psychological Science 24 (5): 776–781. doi:10.1177/0956797612459659. ISSN 0956-7976. PMID 23538911. 
  121. 121.0 121.1 Xu, Jian; Vik, Alexandra; Groote, Inge Rasmus; Lagopoulos, Jim; Holen, Are; Ellingsen, Øyvind; Davanger, Svend (2014). "Nondirective meditation activates default mode network and areas associated with memory retrieval and emotional processing" (in English). Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 8: 86. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2014.00086. ISSN 1662-5161. PMID 24616684. 
  122. Semple, Randye J. (2010-06-01). "Does Mindfulness Meditation Enhance Attention? A Randomized Controlled Trial" (in en). Mindfulness 1 (2): 121–130. doi:10.1007/s12671-010-0017-2. ISSN 1868-8535. 
  123. Walsh, Kathleen Marie; Saab, Bechara J; Farb, Norman AS (2019-01-08). "Effects of a Mindfulness Meditation App on Subjective Well-Being: Active Randomized Controlled Trial and Experience Sampling Study" (in en). JMIR Mental Health 6 (1): e10844. doi:10.2196/10844. ISSN 2368-7959. PMID 30622094. 
  124. 124.0 124.1 124.2 Brown, Kirk Warren; Creswell, J. David; Ryan, Richard M. (2015-11-17) (in en). Handbook of Mindfulness: Theory, Research, and Practice. Guilford Publications. ISBN 978-1-4625-2593-5. https://books.google.com/books?id=ZF-uCgAAQBAJ&pg=PA190&dq=meditation+and+episodic+memory#v=onepage. 
  125. "Differences in visual sensitivity among mindfulness meditators and non-meditators". Perceptual and Motor Skills 58 (3): 727–33. June 1984. doi:10.2466/pms.1984.58.3.727. PMID 6382144. 
  126. 126.0 126.1 "Perception of visual illusions by novice and longer-term meditators". Perceptual and Motor Skills 91 (3 Pt 1): 1021–6. December 2000. doi:10.2466/pms.2000.91.3.1021. PMID 11153836. 
  127. Thompson, Lynn C. (2004). "A Pilot Study of a Yoga and Meditation Intervention for Dementia Caregiver Stress". Journal of Clinical Psychology 60 (6): 677–687. doi:10.1002/jclp.10259. PMID 15141399. 
  128. "Meditation states and traits: EEG, ERP, and neuroimaging studies". Psychological Bulletin 132 (2): 180–211. March 2006. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.132.2.180. PMID 16536641. 
  129. "The relaxation response: therapeutic effect". Science 278 (5344): 1694–5. December 1997. doi:10.1126/science.278.5344.1693b. PMID 9411784. Bibcode1997Sci...278.1693B. 
  130. Cromie, William J. (18 April 2002). "Meditation changes temperatures: Mind controls body in extreme experiments". Harvard University Gazette. http://news.harvard.edu/gazette/2002/04.18/09-tummo.html. 
  131. Benson, Herbert (2001). The Relaxation Response. HarperCollins. pp. 61–3. ISBN 978-0-380-81595-1. https://archive.org/details/relaxationrespon00bens_0/page/61. [non-primary source needed]
  132. "Awakening is not a metaphor: The effects of buddhist meditation practices on basic wakefulness.". Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 13071 (1): 64–81. 2014. doi:10.1111/nyas.12279. PMID 24372471. Bibcode2014NYASA1307...64B. 
  133. "Is meditation always relaxing? investigating heart rate, heart rate variability, experienced effort and likeability during training of three types of meditation.". International Journal of Psychophysiology 97 (1): 38–45. 2015. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2015.04.017. PMID 25937346. 
  134. "Arousal vs. relaxation: A comparison of the neurophysiological and cognitive correlates of vajrayana and theravada meditative practices.". PLOS ONE 9 (7): e102990. 2014. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0102990. PMID 25051268. Bibcode2014PLoSO...9j2990A. 
  135. "Voluntary activation of the sympathetic nervous system and attenuation of the innate immune response in humans.". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 111 (20): 7379–7384. 2014. doi:10.1073/pnas.1322174111. PMID 24799686. Bibcode2014PNAS..111.7379K. 
  136. 136.0 136.1 Brodal, Per (2004). The Central Nervous System: Structure and Function (3 ed.). Oxford University Press US. pp. 369–396. ISBN 978-0-19-516560-9. https://archive.org/details/centralnervoussy0003brod. 
  137. 137.0 137.1 "Forever Young(er): potential age-defying effects of long-term meditation on gray matter atrophy". Frontiers in Psychology 5: 1551. 2015. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01551. PMID 25653628. 
  138. "Meditation experience is associated with increased cortical thickness". NeuroReport 16 (17): 1893–7. November 2005. doi:10.1097/01.wnr.0000186598.66243.19. PMID 16272874. 
  139. "Age effects on gray matter volume and attentional performance in Zen meditation". Neurobiology of Aging 28 (10): 1623–7. October 2007. doi:10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2007.06.008. PMID 17655980. 
  140. "Reduced age-related degeneration of the hippocampal subiculum in long-term meditators". Psychiatry Research 232 (3): 214–8. June 2015. doi:10.1016/j.pscychresns.2015.03.008. PMID 25907419. 
  141. "Exploring age-related brain degeneration in meditation practitioners". Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1307 (1): 82–88. January 2014. doi:10.1111/nyas.12217. PMID 23924195. Bibcode2014NYASA1307...82L. 
  142. "Can meditation slow rate of cellular aging? Cognitive stress, mindfulness, and telomeres". Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1172 (1): 34–53. August 2009. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04414.x. PMID 19735238. Bibcode2009NYASA1172...34E. 
  143. "Effect of comprehensive lifestyle changes on telomerase activity and telomere length in men with biopsy-proven low-risk prostate cancer: 5-year follow-up of a descriptive pilot study". The Lancet. Oncology 14 (11): 1112–1120. October 2013. doi:10.1016/S1470-2045(13)70366-8. PMID 24051140. 
  144. "Efficacy of rajayoga meditation on positive thinking: an index for self-satisfaction and happiness in life". Journal of Clinical and Diagnostic Research 7 (10): 2265–7. October 2013. doi:10.7860/JCDR/2013/5889.3488. PMID 24298493. 
  145. Campos, Daniel; Cebolla, Ausiàs; Quero, Soledad; Bretón-López, Juana; Botella, Cristina; Soler, Joaquim; García-Campayo, Javier; Demarzo, Marcelo et al. (2016). "Meditation and happiness: Mindfulness and self-compassion may mediate the meditation–happiness relationship". Personality and Individual Differences 93: 80–85. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2015.08.040. 
  146. "The structural neural substrate of subjective happiness". Scientific Reports 5: 16891. November 2015. doi:10.1038/srep16891. PMID 26586449. Bibcode2015NatSR...516891S. 
  147. "When the dissolution of perceived body boundaries elicits happiness: The effect of selflessness induced by a body scan meditation". Consciousness and Cognition 46: 89–98. November 2016. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2016.09.013. PMID 27684609. 
  148. "Mindfulness-based stress reduction and attentional control". Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy 14 (6): 449–463. 2007. doi:10.1002/cpp.544. 
  149. Kabat-Zinn, Jon (2013) (in en). Full Catastrophe Living: Using the Wisdom of Your Body and Mind to Face Stress, Pain, and Illness (2nd ed.). Random House Publishing Group. ISBN 978-0-345-53972-4. https://books.google.com/books?id=fIuNDtnb2ZkC&newbks=0&hl=en. 
  150. 150.0 150.1 150.2 "Meditation: An Introduction". National Center for Complementary and Integrative Health. June 2010. http://nccih.nih.gov/health/meditation/overview.htm. 
  151. Perez-De-Albeniz, Alberto; Holmes, Jeremy (2000). "Meditation: Concepts, effects and uses in therapy". International Journal of Psychotherapy 5 (1): 49–58. doi:10.1080/13569080050020263. 
  152. Rocha, Tomas (25 June 2014). "The Dark Knight of the Soul". https://www.theatlantic.com/health/archive/2014/06/the-dark-knight-of-the-souls/372766/?single_page=true. 
  153. "Religious or spiritual problem. A culturally sensitive diagnostic category in the DSM-IV". The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 183 (7): 435–44. July 1995. doi:10.1097/00005053-199507000-00003. PMID 7623015. 
  154. Hayes, Steven C.; Strosahl, Kirk D.; Wilson, Kelly G. (1999). "3". Acceptance and Commitment Therapy: An Experiential Approach to Behavior Change. New York: Guilford. ISBN 978-1-57230-481-9. 
  155. Metzner, Ralph (2005). "Psychedelic, Psychoactive and Addictive Drugs and States of Consciousness". in Earlywine, Mitch. Mind-Altering Drugs. Oxford: Oxford University Press. pp. 25–48. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195165319.003.0002. ISBN 978-0-19-516531-9. 
  156. "Meditation reduces pain-related neural activity in the anterior cingulate cortex, insula, secondary somatosensory cortex, and thalamus". Frontiers in Psychology 5: 1489. 2014. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01489. PMID 25566158. 
  157. "A literature review of Breathworks and mindfulness intervention". British Journal of Healthcare Management 24 (5): 235–241. 2018. doi:10.12968/bjhc.2018.24.5.235. 
  158. "Psychobiological correlates of improved mental health in patients with musculoskeletal pain after a mindfulness-based pain management program". The Clinical Journal of Pain 29 (3): 233–44. March 2013. doi:10.1097/AJP.0b013e31824c5d9f. PMID 22874090. 

External links