Biology:Hippocampus
Hippocampus | |
---|---|
Humans have two hippocampi, one in each hemisphere of the brain. They are located in the medial temporal lobes of the cerebrum. In this lateral view of the human brain, the frontal lobe is at the left, the occipital lobe at the right, and the temporal and parietal lobes have largely been removed to reveal one of the hippocampi underneath. | |
Hippocampus (lowest pink bulb) as part of the limbic system | |
Details | |
Part of | Temporal lobe |
Identifiers | |
Latin | hippocampus |
Anatomical terms of neuroanatomy |
The hippocampus (pl.: hippocampi; via Latin from Greek ἱππόκαμπος, 'seahorse') is a major component of the brain of humans and other vertebrates. Humans and other mammals have two hippocampi, one in each side of the brain. The hippocampus is part of the limbic system, and plays important roles in the consolidation of information from short-term memory to long-term memory, and in spatial memory that enables navigation. The hippocampus is located in the allocortex, with neural projections into the neocortex, in humans[1][2][3] as well as other primates.[4] The hippocampus, as the medial pallium, is a structure found in all vertebrates.[5] In humans, it contains two main interlocking parts: the hippocampus proper (also called Ammon's horn), and the dentate gyrus.[6][7]
In Alzheimer's disease (and other forms of dementia), the hippocampus is one of the first regions of the brain to suffer damage;[8] short-term memory loss and disorientation are included among the early symptoms. Damage to the hippocampus can also result from oxygen starvation (hypoxia), encephalitis, or medial temporal lobe epilepsy. People with extensive, bilateral hippocampal damage may experience anterograde amnesia: the inability to form and retain new memories.
Since different neuronal cell types are neatly organized into layers in the hippocampus, it has frequently been used as a model system for studying neurophysiology. The form of neural plasticity known as long-term potentiation (LTP) was initially discovered to occur in the hippocampus and has often been studied in this structure. LTP is widely believed to be one of the main neural mechanisms by which memories are stored in the brain.
In rodents as model organisms, the hippocampus has been studied extensively as part of a brain system responsible for spatial memory and navigation. Many neurons in the rat and mouse hippocampus respond as place cells: that is, they fire bursts of action potentials when the animal passes through a specific part of its environment. Hippocampal place cells interact extensively with head direction cells, whose activity acts as an inertial compass, and conjecturally with grid cells in the neighboring entorhinal cortex.
Name
The earliest description of the ridge running along the floor of the temporal horn of the lateral ventricle comes from the Venetian anatomist Julius Caesar Aranzi (1587), who likened it first to a silkworm and then to a seahorse (Latin hippocampus, from Greek ἱππόκαμπος, from ἵππος, 'horse' + κάμπος, 'sea monster'). The German anatomist Duvernoy (1729), the first to illustrate the structure, also wavered between "seahorse" and "silkworm". "Ram's horn" was proposed by the Danish anatomist Jacob Winsløw in 1732; and a decade later his fellow Parisian, the surgeon de Garengeot, used cornu Ammonis – horn of Amun,[10] the ancient Egyptian god who was often represented as having a ram's head.[11]
Another reference appeared with the term pes hippocampi, which may date back to Diemerbroeck in 1672, introducing a comparison with the shape of the folded back forelimbs and webbed feet of the mythological hippocampus, a sea monster with a horse's forequarters and a fish's tail. The hippocampus was then described as pes hippocampi major, with an adjacent bulge in the occipital horn, described as the pes hippocampi minor and later renamed as the calcar avis.[10][12] The renaming of the hippocampus as hippocampus major, and the calcar avis as hippocampus minor, has been attributed to Félix Vicq-d'Azyr systematizing nomenclature of parts of the brain in 1786. Mayer mistakenly used the term hippopotamus in 1779, and was followed by some other authors until Karl Friedrich Burdach resolved this error in 1829. In 1861 the hippocampus minor became the center of a dispute over human evolution between Thomas Henry Huxley and Richard Owen, satirized as the Great Hippocampus Question. The term hippocampus minor fell from use in anatomy textbooks and was officially removed in the Nomina Anatomica of 1895.[13] Today, the structure is just called the hippocampus,[10] with the term cornu Ammonis (that is, 'Ammon's horn') surviving in the names of the hippocampal subfields CA1-CA4.[14][6]
Relation to limbic system
The term limbic system was introduced in 1952 by Paul MacLean[15] to describe the set of structures that line the deep edge of the cortex (Latin limbus meaning border): These include the hippocampus, cingulate cortex, olfactory cortex, and amygdala. Paul MacLean later suggested that the limbic structures comprise the neural basis of emotion. The hippocampus is anatomically connected to parts of the brain that are involved with emotional behavior – the septum, the hypothalamic mammillary body, and the anterior nuclear complex in the thalamus, and is generally accepted to be part of the limbic system.[16]
Anatomy
The hippocampus can be seen as a ridge of gray matter tissue, elevating from the floor of each lateral ventricle in the region of the inferior or temporal horn.[17][18] This ridge can also be seen as an inward fold of the archicortex into the medial temporal lobe.[19] The hippocampus can only be seen in dissections as it is concealed by the parahippocampal gyrus.[19][20] The cortex thins from six layers to the three or four layers that make up the hippocampus.[21]
The term hippocampal formation is used to refer to the hippocampus proper and its related parts. However, there is no consensus as to what parts are included. Sometimes the hippocampus is said to include the dentate gyrus and the subiculum. Some references include the dentate gyrus and the subiculum in the hippocampal formation,[1] and others also include the presubiculum, parasubiculum, and entorhinal cortex.[2] The neural layout and pathways within the hippocampal formation are very similar in all mammals.[3]
The hippocampus, including the dentate gyrus, has the shape of a curved tube, which has been compared to a seahorse, and to a horn of a ram, which after the ancient Egyptian god often portrayed as such takes the name cornu Ammonis. Its abbreviation CA is used in naming the hippocampal subfields CA1, CA2, CA3, and CA4.[20] It can be distinguished as an area where the cortex narrows into a single layer of densely packed pyramidal neurons, which curl into a tight U shape. One edge of the "U," – CA4, is embedded into the backward-facing, flexed dentate gyrus. The hippocampus is described as having an anterior and posterior part (in primates) or a ventral and dorsal part in other animals. Both parts are of similar composition but belong to different neural circuits.[22] In the rat, the two hippocampi resemble a pair of bananas, joined at the stems by the commissure of fornix (also called the hippocampal commissure). In primates, the part of the hippocampus at the bottom, near the base of the temporal lobe, is much broader than the part at the top. This means that in cross-section the hippocampus can show a number of different shapes, depending on the angle and location of the cut.[citation needed]
In a cross-section of the hippocampus, including the dentate gyrus, several layers will be shown. The dentate gyrus has three layers of cells (or four if the hilus is included). The layers are from the outer in – the molecular layer, the inner molecular layer, the granular layer, and the hilus. The CA3 in the hippocampus proper has the following cell layers known as strata: lacunosum-moleculare, radiatum, lucidum, pyramidal, and oriens. CA2 and CA1 also have these layers except the lucidum stratum.[23][24]
The input to the hippocampus (from varying cortical and subcortical structures) comes from the entorhinal cortex via the perforant path. The entorhinal cortex (EC) is strongly and reciprocally connected with many cortical and subcortical structures as well as with the brainstem. Different thalamic nuclei, (from the anterior and midline groups), the medial septal nucleus, the supramammillary nucleus of the hypothalamus, and the raphe nuclei and locus coeruleus of the brainstem all send axons to the EC, so that it serves as the interface between the neocortex and the other connections, and the hippocampus.[citation needed]
The EC is located in the parahippocampal gyrus,[2] a cortical region adjacent to the hippocampus.[25] This gyrus conceals the hippocampus. The parahippocampal gyrus is adjacent to the perirhinal cortex, which plays an important role in the visual recognition of complex objects. There is also substantial evidence that it makes a contribution to memory, which can be distinguished from the contribution of the hippocampus. It is apparent that complete amnesia occurs only when both the hippocampus and the parahippocampus are damaged.[25]
Circuitry
The major input to the hippocampus is through the entorhinal cortex (EC), whereas its major output is via CA1 to the subiculum.[26] Information reaches CA1 via two main pathways, direct and indirect. Axons from the EC that originate in layer III are the origin of the direct perforant pathway and form synapses on the very distal apical dendrites of CA1 neurons. Conversely, axons originating from layer II are the origin of the indirect pathway, and information reaches CA1 via the trisynaptic circuit. In the initial part of this pathway, the axons project through the perforant pathway to the granule cells of the dentate gyrus (first synapse). From then, the information follows via the mossy fibres to CA3 (second synapse). From there, CA3 axons called Schaffer collaterals leave the deep part of the cell body and loop up to the apical dendrites and then extend to CA1 (third synapse).[26] Axons from CA1 then project back to the entorhinal cortex, completing the circuit.[27]
Basket cells in CA3 receive excitatory input from the pyramidal cells and then give an inhibitory feedback to the pyramidal cells. This recurrent inhibition is a simple feedback circuit that can dampen excitatory responses in the hippocampus. The pyramidal cells gives a recurrent excitation which is an important mechanism found in some memory processing microcircuits.[28]
Several other connections play important roles in hippocampal function.[20] Beyond the output to the EC, additional output pathways go to other cortical areas including the prefrontal cortex. A major output goes via the fornix to the lateral septal area and to the mammillary body of the hypothalamus (which the fornix interconnects with the hippocampus).[19] The hippocampus receives modulatory input from the serotonin, norepinephrine, and dopamine systems, and from the nucleus reuniens of the thalamus to field CA1. A very important projection comes from the medial septal nucleus, which sends cholinergic, and gamma amino butyric acid (GABA) stimulating fibers (GABAergic fibers) to all parts of the hippocampus. The inputs from the medial septal nucleus play a key role in controlling the physiological state of the hippocampus; destruction of this nucleus abolishes the hippocampal theta rhythm and severely impairs certain types of memory.[29]
Regions
Areas of the hippocampus are shown to be functionally and anatomically distinct. The dorsal hippocampus (DH), ventral hippocampus (VH) and intermediate hippocampus serve different functions, project with differing pathways, and have varying degrees of place cells.[30] The dorsal hippocampus serves for spatial memory, verbal memory, and learning of conceptual information. Using the radial arm maze, lesions in the DH were shown to cause spatial memory impairment while VH lesions did not. Its projecting pathways include the medial septal nucleus and supramammillary nucleus.[31] The dorsal hippocampus also has more place cells than both the ventral and intermediate hippocampal regions.[32]
The intermediate hippocampus has overlapping characteristics with both the ventral and dorsal hippocampus.[30] Using anterograde tracing methods, Cenquizca and Swanson (2007) located the moderate projections to two primary olfactory cortical areas and prelimbic areas of the medial prefrontal cortex. This region has the smallest number of place cells. The ventral hippocampus functions in fear conditioning and affective processes.[33] Anagnostaras et al. (2002) showed that alterations to the ventral hippocampus reduced the amount of information sent to the amygdala by the dorsal and ventral hippocampus, consequently altering fear conditioning in rats.[34] Historically, the earliest widely held hypothesis was that the hippocampus is involved in olfaction.[35] This idea was cast into doubt by a series of anatomical studies that did not find any direct projections to the hippocampus from the olfactory bulb.[36] However, later work did confirm that the olfactory bulb does project into the ventral part of the lateral entorhinal cortex, and field CA1 in the ventral hippocampus sends axons to the main olfactory bulb,[37] the anterior olfactory nucleus, and to the primary olfactory cortex. There continues to be some interest in hippocampal olfactory responses, in particular, the role of the hippocampus in memory for odors, but few specialists today believe that olfaction is its primary function.[38][39]
Function
Theories of hippocampal functions
Over the years, three main ideas of hippocampal function have dominated the literature: response inhibition, episodic memory, and spatial cognition. The behavioral inhibition theory (caricatured by John O'Keefe and Lynn Nadel as "slam on the brakes!")[40] was very popular up to the 1960s. It derived much of its justification from two observations: first, that animals with hippocampal damage tend to be hyperactive; second, that animals with hippocampal damage often have difficulty learning to inhibit responses that they have previously been taught, especially if the response requires remaining quiet as in a passive avoidance test. British psychologist Jeffrey Gray developed this line of thought into a full-fledged theory of the role of the hippocampus in anxiety.[41] The inhibition theory is currently the least popular of the three.[42]
The second major line of thought relates the hippocampus to memory. Although it had historical precursors, this idea derived its main impetus from a famous report by American neurosurgeon William Beecher Scoville and British-Canadian neuropsychologist Brenda Milner[43] describing the results of surgical destruction of the hippocampi when trying to relieve epileptic seizures in an American man Henry Molaison,[44] known until his death in 2008 as "Patient H.M." The unexpected outcome of the surgery was severe anterograde and partial retrograde amnesia; Molaison was unable to form new episodic memories after his surgery and could not remember any events that occurred just before his surgery, but he did retain memories of events that occurred many years earlier extending back into his childhood. This case attracted such widespread professional interest that Molaison became the most intensively studied subject in medical history.[45] In the ensuing years, other patients with similar levels of hippocampal damage and amnesia (caused by accident or disease) have also been studied, and thousands of experiments have studied the physiology of activity-driven changes in synaptic connections in the hippocampus. There is now universal agreement that the hippocampi play some sort of important role in memory; however, the precise nature of this role remains widely debated.[46][47] A recent theory proposed – without questioning its role in spatial cognition – that the hippocampus encodes new episodic memories by associating representations in the newborn granule cells of the dentate gyrus and arranging those representations sequentially in the CA3 by relying on the phase precession generated in the entorhinal cortex[48]
The third important theory of hippocampal function relates the hippocampus to space. The spatial theory was originally championed by O'Keefe and Nadel, who were influenced by American psychologist E.C. Tolman's theories about "cognitive maps" in humans and animals. O'Keefe and his student Dostrovsky in 1971 discovered neurons in the rat hippocampus that appeared to them to show activity related to the rat's location within its environment.[49] Despite skepticism from other investigators, O'Keefe and his co-workers, especially Lynn Nadel, continued to investigate this question, in a line of work that eventually led to their very influential 1978 book The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map.[50] There is now almost universal agreement that hippocampal function plays an important role in spatial coding, but the details are widely debated.[51]
Later research has focused on trying to bridge the disconnect between the two main views of hippocampal function as being split between memory and spatial cognition. In some studies, these areas have been expanded to the point of near convergence. In an attempt to reconcile the two disparate views, it is suggested that a broader view of the hippocampal function is taken and seen to have a role that encompasses both the organisation of experience (mental mapping, as per Tolman's original concept in 1948) and the directional behaviour seen as being involved in all areas of cognition, so that the function of the hippocampus can be viewed as a broader system that incorporates both the memory and the spatial perspectives in its role that involves the use of a wide scope of cognitive maps.[52] This relates to the purposive behaviorism born of Tolman's original goal of identifying the complex cognitive mechanisms and purposes that guided behaviour.[53]
It has also been proposed that the spiking activity of hippocampal neurons is associated spatially, and it was suggested that the mechanisms of memory and planning both evolved from mechanisms of navigation and that their neuronal algorithms were basically the same.[54]
Many studies have made use of neuroimaging techniques such as functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), and a functional role in approach-avoidance conflict has been noted. The anterior hippocampus is seen to be involved in decision-making under approach-avoidance conflict processing. It is suggested that the memory, spatial cognition, and conflict processing functions may be seen as working together and not mutually exclusive.[55]
Role in memory
Psychologists and neuroscientists generally agree that the hippocampus plays an important role in the formation of new memories about experienced events (episodic or autobiographical memory).[47][56] Part of this function is hippocampal involvement in the detection of new events, places and stimuli.[57] Some researchers regard the hippocampus as part of a larger medial temporal lobe memory system responsible for general declarative memory (memories that can be explicitly verbalized – these would include, for example, memory for facts in addition to episodic memory).[46] The hippocampus also encodes emotional context from the amygdala. This is partly why returning to a location where an emotional event occurred may evoke that emotion. There is a deep emotional connection between episodic memories and places.[58]
Due to bilateral symmetry the brain has a hippocampus in each cerebral hemisphere. If damage to the hippocampus occurs in only one hemisphere, leaving the structure intact in the other hemisphere, the brain can retain near-normal memory functioning.[59] Severe damage to the hippocampi in both hemispheres results in profound difficulties in forming new memories (anterograde amnesia) and often also affects memories formed before the damage occurred (retrograde amnesia). Although the retrograde effect normally extends many years back before the brain damage, in some cases older memories remain. This retention of older memories leads to the idea that consolidation over time involves the transfer of memories out of the hippocampus to other parts of the brain.[60] Experiments using intrahippocampal transplantation of hippocampal cells in primates with neurotoxic lesions of the hippocampus have shown that the hippocampus is required for the formation and recall, but not the storage, of memories.[61] It has been shown that a decrease in the volume of various parts of the hippocampus in people leads to specific memory impairments. In particular, efficiency of verbal memory retention is related to the anterior parts of the right and left hippocampus. The right head of the hippocampus is more involved in executive functions and regulation during verbal memory recall. The tail of the left hippocampus tends to be closely related to verbal memory capacity.[62]
Damage to the hippocampus does not affect some types of memory, such as the ability to learn new skills (playing a musical instrument or solving certain types of puzzles, for example). This fact suggests that such abilities depend on different types of memory (procedural memory) and different brain regions. Furthermore, amnesic patients frequently show "implicit" memory for experiences even in the absence of conscious knowledge. For example, patients asked to guess which of two faces they have seen most recently may give the correct answer most of the time in spite of stating that they have never seen either of the faces before. Some researchers distinguish between conscious recollection, which depends on the hippocampus, and familiarity, which depends on portions of the medial temporal lobe.[63]
When rats are exposed to an intense learning event, they may retain a life-long memory of the event even after a single training session. The memory of such an event appears to be first stored in the hippocampus, but this storage is transient. Much of the long-term storage of the memory seems to take place in the anterior cingulate cortex.[64] When such an intense learning event was experimentally applied, more than 5,000 differently methylated DNA regions appeared in the hippocampus neuronal genome of the rats at one hour and at 24 hours after training.[65] These alterations in methylation pattern occurred at many genes that were down-regulated, often due to the formation of new 5-methylcytosine sites in CpG rich regions of the genome. Furthermore, many other genes were upregulated, likely often due to the removal of methyl groups from previously existing 5-methylcytosines (5mCs) in DNA. Demethylation of 5mC can be carried out by several proteins acting in concert, including TET enzymes as well as enzymes of the DNA base excision repair pathway (see Epigenetics in learning and memory).
Studies on freely moving rats and mice have shown many hippocampal neurons to act as place cells that cluster in place fields, and these fire bursts of action potentials when the animal passes through a particular location. This place-related neural activity in the hippocampus has also been reported in monkeys that were moved around a room whilst in a restraint chair.[66] However, the place cells may have fired in relation to where the monkey was looking rather than to its actual location in the room.[67] Over many years, many studies have been carried out on place-responses in rodents, which have given a large amount of information.[51] Place cell responses are shown by pyramidal cells in the hippocampus and by granule cells in the dentate gyrus. Other cells in smaller proportion are inhibitory interneurons, and these often show place-related variations in their firing rate that are much weaker. There is little, if any, spatial topography in the representation; in general, cells lying next to each other in the hippocampus have uncorrelated spatial firing patterns. Place cells are typically almost silent when a rat is moving around outside the place field but reach sustained rates as high as 40 Hz when the rat is near the center. Neural activity sampled from 30 to 40 randomly chosen place cells carries enough information to allow a rat's location to be reconstructed with high confidence. The size of place fields varies in a gradient along the length of the hippocampus, with cells at the dorsal end showing the smallest fields, cells near the center showing larger fields, and cells at the ventral tip showing fields that cover the entire environment.[51] In some cases, the firing rate of hippocampal cells depends not only on place but also the direction a rat is moving, the destination toward which it is traveling, or other task-related variables.[68] The firing of place cells is timed in relation to local theta waves, a process termed phase precession.[69]
In humans, cells with location-specific firing patterns have been reported during a study of patients with drug-resistant epilepsy. They were undergoing an invasive procedure to localize the source of their seizures, with a view to surgical resection. The patients had diagnostic electrodes implanted in their hippocampus and then used a computer to move around in a virtual reality town.[70] Similar brain imaging studies in navigation have shown the hippocampus to be active.[71] A study was carried out on taxi drivers. London's black cab drivers need to learn the locations of a large number of places and the fastest routes between them in order to pass a strict test known as The Knowledge in order to gain a license to operate. A study showed that the posterior part of the hippocampus is larger in these drivers than in the general public, and that a positive correlation exists between the length of time served as a driver and the increase in the volume of this part. It was also found the total volume of the hippocampus was unchanged, as the increase seen in the posterior part was made at the expense of the anterior part, which showed a relative decrease in size. There have been no reported adverse effects from this disparity in hippocampal proportions.[72] Another study showed opposite findings in blind individuals. The anterior part of the right hippocampus was larger and the posterior part was smaller, compared with sighted individuals.[73]
There are several navigational cells in the brain that are either in the hippocampus itself or are strongly connected to it, such as the speed cells present in the medial entorhinal cortex. Together these cells form a network that serves as spatial memory. The first of such cells discovered in the 1970s were the place cells, which led to the idea of the hippocampus acting to give a neural representation of the environment in a cognitive map.[74] When the hippocampus is dysfunctional, orientation is affected; people may have difficulty in remembering how they arrived at a location and how to proceed further. Getting lost is a common symptom of amnesia.[75] Studies with animals have shown that an intact hippocampus is required for initial learning and long-term retention of some spatial memory tasks, in particular ones that require finding the way to a hidden goal.[76][77][78][79] Other cells have been discovered since the finding of the place cells in the rodent brain that are either in the hippocampus or the entorhinal cortex. These have been assigned as head direction cells, grid cells and boundary cells.[51][80] Speed cells are thought to provide input to the hippocampal grid cells.
Role in approach-avoidance conflict processing
Approach-avoidance conflict happens when a situation is presented that can either be rewarding or punishing, and the ensuing decision-making has been associated with anxiety.[81] fMRI findings from studies in approach-avoidance decision-making found evidence for a functional role that is not explained by either long-term memory or spatial cognition. Overall findings showed that the anterior hippocampus is sensitive to conflict, and that it may be part of a larger cortical and subcortical network seen to be important in decision-making in uncertain conditions.[81]
A review makes reference to a number of studies that show the involvement of the hippocampus in conflict tasks. The authors suggest that one challenge is to understand how conflict processing relates to the functions of spatial navigation and memory and how all of these functions need not be mutually exclusive.[55]
Electroencephalography
The hippocampus shows two major "modes" of activity, each associated with a distinct pattern of neural population activity and waves of electrical activity as measured by an electroencephalogram (EEG). These modes are named after the EEG patterns associated with them: theta and large irregular activity (LIA). The main characteristics described below are for the rat, which is the animal most extensively studied.[82]
The theta mode appears during states of active, alert behavior (especially locomotion), and also during REM (dreaming) sleep.[83] In the theta mode, the EEG is dominated by large regular waves with a frequency range of 6 to 9 Hz, and the main groups of hippocampal neurons (pyramidal cells and granule cells) show sparse population activity, which means that in any short time interval, the great majority of cells are silent, while the small remaining fraction fire at relatively high rates, up to 50 spikes in one second for the most active of them. An active cell typically stays active for half a second to a few seconds. As the rat behaves, the active cells fall silent and new cells become active, but the overall percentage of active cells remains more or less constant. In many situations, cell activity is determined largely by the spatial location of the animal, but other behavioral variables also clearly influence it.
The LIA mode appears during slow-wave (non-dreaming) sleep, and also during states of waking immobility such as resting or eating.[83] In the LIA mode, the EEG is dominated by sharp waves that are randomly timed large deflections of the EEG signal lasting for 25–50 milliseconds. Sharp waves are frequently generated in sets, with sets containing up to 5 or more individual sharp waves and lasting up to 500 ms. The spiking activity of neurons within the hippocampus is highly correlated with sharp wave activity. Most neurons decrease their firing rate between sharp waves; however, during a sharp wave, there is a dramatic increase in firing rate in up to 10% of the hippocampal population
These two hippocampal activity modes can be seen in primates as well as rats, with the exception that it has been difficult to see robust theta rhythmicity in the primate hippocampus. There are, however, qualitatively similar sharp waves and similar state-dependent changes in neural population activity.[84]
Theta rhythm
The underlying currents producing the theta wave are generated mainly by densely packed neural layers of the entorhinal cortex, CA3, and the dendrites of pyramidal cells. The theta wave is one of the largest signals seen on EEG, and is known as the hippocampal theta rhythm.[85] In some situations the EEG is dominated by regular waves at 3 to 10 Hz, often continuing for many seconds. These reflect subthreshold membrane potentials and strongly modulate the spiking of hippocampal neurons and synchronise across the hippocampus in a travelling wave pattern.[86] The trisynaptic circuit is a relay of neurotransmission in the hippocampus that interacts with many brain regions. From rodent studies it has been proposed that the trisynaptic circuit generates the hippocampal theta rhythm.[87]
Theta rhythmicity is very obvious in rabbits and rodents and also clearly present in cats and dogs. Whether theta can be seen in primates is not yet clear.[88] In rats (the animals that have been the most extensively studied), theta is seen mainly in two conditions: first, when an animal is walking or in some other way actively interacting with its surroundings; second, during REM sleep.[89] The function of theta has not yet been convincingly explained although numerous theories have been proposed.[82] The most popular hypothesis has been to relate it to learning and memory. An example would be the phase with which theta rhythms, at the time of stimulation of a neuron, shape the effect of that stimulation upon its synapses. What is meant here is that theta rhythms may affect those aspects of learning and memory that are dependent upon synaptic plasticity.[90] It is well established that lesions of the medial septum – the central node of the theta system – cause severe disruptions of memory.[91] However, the medial septum is more than just the controller of theta; it is also the main source of cholinergic projections to the hippocampus.[20] It has not been established that septal lesions exert their effects specifically by eliminating the theta rhythm.[92]
Sharp waves
During sleep or during resting, when an animal is not engaged with its surroundings, the hippocampal EEG shows a pattern of irregular slow waves, somewhat larger in amplitude than theta waves. This pattern is occasionally interrupted by large surges called sharp waves.[93] These events are associated with bursts of spike activity lasting 50 to 100 milliseconds in pyramidal cells of CA3 and CA1. They are also associated with short-lived high-frequency EEG oscillations called "ripples", with frequencies in the range 150 to 200 Hz in rats, and together they are known as sharp waves and ripples. Sharp waves are most frequent during sleep when they occur at an average rate of around 1 per second (in rats) but in a very irregular temporal pattern. Sharp waves are less frequent during inactive waking states and are usually smaller. Sharp waves have also been observed in humans and monkeys. In macaques, sharp waves are robust but do not occur as frequently as in rats.[84]
One of the most interesting aspects of sharp waves is that they appear to be associated with memory. Wilson and McNaughton 1994,[94] and numerous later studies, reported that when hippocampal place cells have overlapping spatial firing fields (and therefore often fire in near-simultaneity), they tend to show correlated activity during sleep following the behavioral session. This enhancement of correlation, commonly known as reactivation, has been found to occur mainly during sharp waves.[95] It has been proposed that sharp waves are, in fact, reactivations of neural activity patterns that were memorized during behavior, driven by strengthening of synaptic connections within the hippocampus.[96] This idea forms a key component of the "two-stage memory" theory,[97] advocated by Buzsáki and others, which proposes that memories are stored within the hippocampus during behavior and then later transferred to the neocortex during sleep. Sharp waves in Hebbian theory are seen as persistently repeated stimulations by presynaptic cells, of postsynaptic cells that are suggested to drive synaptic changes in the cortical targets of hippocampal output pathways.[98] Suppression of sharp waves and ripples in sleep or during immobility can interfere with memories expressed at the level of the behavior,[99][100] nonetheless, the newly formed CA1 place cell code can re-emerge even after a sleep with abolished sharp waves and ripples, in spatially non-demanding tasks.[101]
Long-term potentiation
Since at least the time of Ramon y Cajal (1852–1934), psychologists have speculated that the brain stores memory by altering the strength of connections between neurons that are simultaneously active.[102] This idea was formalized by Donald Hebb in 1949,[103] but for many years remained unexplained. In 1973, Tim Bliss and Terje Lømo described a phenomenon in the rabbit hippocampus that appeared to meet Hebb's specifications: a change in synaptic responsiveness induced by brief strong activation and lasting for hours or days or longer.[104] This phenomenon was soon referred to as long-term potentiation (LTP). As a candidate mechanism for long-term memory, LTP has since been studied intensively, and a great deal has been learned about it. However, the complexity and variety of the intracellular signalling cascades that can trigger LTP is acknowledged as preventing a more complete understanding.[105]
The hippocampus is a particularly favorable site for studying LTP because of its densely packed and sharply defined layers of neurons, but similar types of activity-dependent synaptic change have also been observed in many other brain areas.[106] The best-studied form of LTP has been seen in CA1 of the hippocampus and occurs at synapses that terminate on dendritic spines and use the neurotransmitter glutamate.[105] The synaptic changes depend on a special type of glutamate receptor, the N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptor, a cell surface receptor which has the special property of allowing calcium to enter the postsynaptic spine only when presynaptic activation and postsynaptic depolarization occur at the same time.[107] Drugs that interfere with NMDA receptors block LTP and have major effects on some types of memory, especially spatial memory. Genetically modified mice that are modified to disable the LTP mechanism, also generally show severe memory deficits.[107]
Disorders
Aging
Age-related conditions such as Alzheimer's disease and other forms of dementia (for which hippocampal disruption is one of the earliest signs[108]) have a severe impact on many types of cognition including memory. Even normal aging is associated with a gradual decline in some types of memory, including episodic memory and working memory (or short-term memory). Because the hippocampus is thought to play a central role in memory, there has been considerable interest in the possibility that age-related declines could be caused by hippocampal deterioration.[109] Some early studies reported substantial loss of neurons in the hippocampus of elderly people, but later studies using more precise techniques found only minimal differences.[109] Similarly, some MRI studies have reported shrinkage of the hippocampus in elderly people, but other studies have failed to reproduce this finding. There is, however, a reliable relationship between the size of the hippocampus and memory performance; so that where there is age-related shrinkage, memory performance will be impaired.[110] There are also reports that memory tasks tend to produce less hippocampal activation in the elderly than in the young.[110] Furthermore, a randomized control trial published in 2011 found that aerobic exercise could increase the size of the hippocampus in adults aged 55 to 80 and also improve spatial memory.[111]
Stress
The hippocampus contains high levels of glucocorticoid receptors, which make it more vulnerable to long-term stress than most other brain areas.[112] There is evidence that humans having experienced severe, long-lasting traumatic stress show atrophy of the hippocampus more than of other parts of the brain.[113] These effects show up in post-traumatic stress disorder,[114] and they may contribute to the hippocampal atrophy reported in schizophrenia[115] and severe depression.[116] Anterior hippocampal volume in children is positively correlated with parental family income and this correlation is thought to be mediated by income related stress.[117] A recent study has also revealed atrophy as a result of depression, but this can be stopped with anti-depressants even if they are not effective in relieving other symptoms.[118]
Chronic stress resulting in elevated levels of glucocorticoids, notably of cortisol, is seen to be a cause of neuronal atrophy in the hippocampus. This atrophy results in a smaller hippocampal volume which is also seen in Cushing's syndrome. The higher levels of cortisol in Cushing's syndrome is usually the result of medications taken for other conditions.[119][120] Neuronal loss also occurs as a result of impaired neurogenesis. Another factor that contributes to a smaller hippocampal volume is that of dendritic retraction where dendrites are shortened in length and reduced in number, in response to increased glucocorticoids. This dendritic retraction is reversible.[120] After treatment with medication to reduce cortisol in Cushing's syndrome, the hippocampal volume is seen to be restored by as much as 10%.[119] This change is seen to be due to the reforming of the dendrites.[120] This dendritic restoration can also happen when stress is removed. There is, however, evidence derived mainly from studies using rats that stress occurring shortly after birth can affect hippocampal function in ways that persist throughout life.[121]
Sex-specific responses to stress have also been demonstrated in the rat to have an effect on the hippocampus. Chronic stress in the male rat showed dendritic retraction and cell loss in the CA3 region but this was not shown in the female. This was thought to be due to neuroprotective ovarian hormones.[122][123] In rats, DNA damage increases in the hippocampus under conditions of stress.[124]
Epilepsy
The hippocampus is one of the few brain regions where new neurons are generated. This process of neurogenesis is confined to the dentate gyrus.[125] The production of new neurons can be positively affected by exercise or negatively affected by epileptic seizures.[125]
Seizures in temporal lobe epilepsy can affect the normal development of new neurons and can cause tissue damage. Hippocampal sclerosis including Ammon's horn sclerosis that is specific to the mesial temporal lobe, is the most common type of such tissue damage.[126][127] It is not yet clear, however, whether the epilepsy is usually caused by hippocampal abnormalities or whether the hippocampus is damaged by cumulative effects of seizures.[128] However, in experimental settings where repetitive seizures are artificially induced in animals, hippocampal damage is a frequent result. This may be a consequence of the concentration of excitable glutamate receptors in the hippocampus. Hyperexcitability can lead to cytotoxicity and cell death.[120] It may also have something to do with the hippocampus being a site where new neurons continue to be created throughout life,[125] and to abnormalities in this process.[120]
Schizophrenia
The causes of schizophrenia are not well understood, but numerous abnormalities of brain structure have been reported. The most thoroughly investigated alterations involve the cerebral cortex, but effects on the hippocampus have also been described. Many reports have found reductions in the size of the hippocampus in people with schizophrenia.[129][130] The left hippocampus seems to be affected more than the right.[129] The changes noted have largely been accepted to be the result of abnormal development. It is unclear whether hippocampal alterations play any role in causing the psychotic symptoms that are the most important feature of schizophrenia. It has been suggested that on the basis of experimental work using animals, hippocampal dysfunction might produce an alteration of dopamine release in the basal ganglia, thereby indirectly affecting the integration of information in the prefrontal cortex.[131] It has also been suggested that hippocampal dysfunction might account for the disturbances in long-term memory frequently observed.[132]
MRI studies have found a smaller brain volume and larger ventricles in people with schizophrenia – however researchers do not know if the shrinkage is from the schizophrenia or from the medication.[133][134] The hippocampus and thalamus have been shown to be reduced in volume; and the volume of the globus pallidus is increased. Cortical patterns are altered, and a reduction in the volume and thickness of the cortex particularly in the frontal and temporal lobes has been noted. It has further been proposed that many of the changes seen are present at the start of the disorder which gives weight to the theory that there is abnormal neurodevelopment.[135]
The hippocampus has been seen as central to the pathology of schizophrenia, both in the neural and physiological effects.[129] It has been generally accepted that there is an abnormal synaptic connectivity underlying schizophrenia. Several lines of evidence implicate changes in the synaptic organization and connectivity, in and from the hippocampus[129] Many studies have found dysfunction in the synaptic circuitry within the hippocampus and its activity on the prefrontal cortex. The glutamatergic pathways have been seen to be largely affected. The subfield CA1 is seen to be the least involved of the other subfields,[129][136] and CA4 and the subiculum have been reported elsewhere as being the most implicated areas.[136] The review concluded that the pathology could be due to genetics, faulty neurodevelopment or abnormal neural plasticity. It was further concluded that schizophrenia is not due to any known neurodegenerative disorder.[129] Oxidative DNA damage is substantially increased in the hippocampus of elderly patients with chronic schizophrenia.[137]
Transient global amnesia
Transient global amnesia is a dramatic, sudden, temporary, near-total loss of short-term memory. Various causes have been hypothesized including ischemia, epilepsy, migraine[138] and disturbance of cerebral venous blood flow,[139] leading to ischemia of structures such as the hippocampus that are involved in memory.[140]
There has been no scientific proof of any cause. However, diffusion weighted MRI studies taken from 12 to 24 hours following an episode has shown there to be small dot-like lesions in the hippocampus. These findings have suggested a possible implication of CA1 neurons made vulnerable by metabolic stress.[138]
PTSD
Some studies shows correlation of reduced hippocampus volume and post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD).[141][142][143] A study of Vietnam War combat veterans with PTSD showed a 20% reduction in the volume of their hippocampus compared with veterans having suffered no such symptoms.[144] This finding was not replicated in chronic PTSD patients traumatized at an air show plane crash in 1988 (Ramstein, Germany).[145] It is also the case that non-combat twin brothers of Vietnam veterans with PTSD also had smaller hippocampi than other controls, raising questions about the nature of the correlation.[146] A 2016 study strengthened theory that a smaller hippocampus increases the risk for post-traumatic stress disorder, and a larger hippocampus increases the likelihood of efficacious treatment.[147]
Microcephaly
Hippocampus atrophy has been characterized in those with microcephaly,[148] and mouse models with WDR62 mutations which recapitulate human point mutations shown a deficiency in hippocampal development and neurogenesis.[149]
Other animals
Other mammals
The hippocampus has a generally similar appearance across the range of mammals, from monotremes such as the echidna to primates such as humans.[150] The hippocampal-size-to-body-size ratio broadly increases, being about twice as large for primates as for the echidna. It does not, however, increase at anywhere close to the rate of the neocortex-to-body-size ratio. Therefore, the hippocampus takes up a much larger fraction of the cortical mantle in rodents than in primates. In adult humans the volume of the hippocampus on each side of the brain is about 3.0 to 3.5 cm3 as compared to 320 to 420 cm3 for the volume of the neocortex.[151]
There is also a general relationship between the size of the hippocampus and spatial memory. When comparisons are made between similar species, those that have a greater capacity for spatial memory tend to have larger hippocampal volumes.[152] This relationship also extends to sex differences; in species where males and females show strong differences in spatial memory ability they also tend to show corresponding differences in hippocampal volume.[153]
Other vertebrates
Non-mammalian species do not have a brain structure that looks like the mammalian hippocampus, but they have one that is considered homologous to it. The hippocampus, as pointed out above, is in essence part of the allocortex. Only mammals have a fully developed cortex, but the structure it evolved from, called the pallium, is present in all vertebrates, even the most primitive ones such as the lamprey or hagfish.[154] The pallium is usually divided into three zones: medial, lateral and dorsal. The medial pallium forms the precursor of the hippocampus. It does not resemble the hippocampus visually because the layers are not warped into an S shape or enfolded by the dentate gyrus, but the homology is indicated by strong chemical and functional affinities. There is now evidence that these hippocampal-like structures are involved in spatial cognition in birds, reptiles, and fish.[155]
Birds
In birds, the correspondence is sufficiently well established that most anatomists refer to the medial pallial zone as the "avian hippocampus".[156] Numerous species of birds have strong spatial skills, in particular those that cache food. There is evidence that food-caching birds have a larger hippocampus than other types of birds and that damage to the hippocampus causes impairments in spatial memory.[157]
Fish
The story for fish is more complex. In teleost fish (which make up the great majority of existing species), the forebrain is distorted in comparison to other types of vertebrates: most neuroanatomists believe that the teleost forebrain is in essence everted, like a sock turned inside-out, so that structures that lie in the interior, next to the ventricles, for most vertebrates, are found on the outside in teleost fish, and vice versa.[158] One of the consequences of this is that the medial pallium ("hippocampal" zone) of a typical vertebrate is thought to correspond to the lateral pallium of a typical fish. Several types of fish (particularly goldfish) have been shown experimentally to have strong spatial memory abilities, even forming "cognitive maps" of the areas they inhabit.[152] There is evidence that damage to the lateral pallium impairs spatial memory.[159][160] It is not yet known whether the medial pallium plays a similar role in even more primitive vertebrates, such as sharks and rays, or even lampreys and hagfish.[161]
Insects and molluscs
Some types of insects, and molluscs such as the octopus, also have strong spatial learning and navigation abilities, but these appear to work differently from the mammalian spatial system, so there is as yet no good reason to think that they have a common evolutionary origin; nor is there sufficient similarity in brain structure to enable anything resembling a "hippocampus" to be identified in these species. Some have proposed, however, that the insect's mushroom bodies may have a function similar to that of the hippocampus.[162]
Computational models
Thorough research of hippocampus in different organisms, a comprehensive database about morphology, connectivity, physiology and computational models was collected.[163]
Additional images
Notes
- ↑ 1.0 1.1 Martin, JH (2003). "Lymbic system and cerebral circuits for emotions, learning, and memory". Neuroanatomy: text and atlas (third ed.). McGraw-Hill Companies. p. 382. ISBN 978-0071212373. https://books.google.com/books?id=OUC4igr3O4sC&pg=PA382. Retrieved 2016-12-16.
- ↑ 2.0 2.1 2.2 "Hippocampal neuroanatomy". The hippocampus book (first ed.). New York: Oxford University Press. 2007. p. 37. ISBN 978-0195100273. https://books.google.com/books?id=zg6oyF1DziQC&pg=PA37. Retrieved 2016-12-15.
- ↑ 3.0 3.1 "The hippocampal formation". The hippocampus book (first ed.). New York: Oxford University Press. 2007. p. 3. ISBN 978-0195100273. https://books.google.com/books?id=zg6oyF1DziQC&pg=PA3. Retrieved 2016-12-15.
- ↑ Bachevalier, Jocelyne (December 26, 2019). "Nonhuman primate models of hippocampal development and dysfunction". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 116 (52): 26210–26216. doi:10.1073/pnas.1902278116. PMID 31871159. Bibcode: 2019PNAS..11626210B.
- ↑ Bingman V.P., Salas C., Rodriguez F. (2009) Evolution of the Hippocampus. In: Binder M.D., Hirokawa N., Windhorst U. (eds) Encyclopedia of Neuroscience. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-540-29678-2_3158 [1]
- ↑ 6.0 6.1 "Search Results for ammon's horn". https://www.oxfordreference.com/search?q=ammon%27s+horn&searchBtn=Search&isQuickSearch=true.
- ↑ Colman, Andrew M. (21 May 2015). "dentate gyrus" (in en). Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/acref/9780199657681.001.0001. https://www.oxfordreference.com/view/10.1093/acref/9780199657681.001.0001/acref-9780199657681-e-2184?rskey=auIwwd&result=10.
- ↑ "Preclinical Alzheimer's disease: Definition, natural history, and diagnostic criteria". Alzheimer's & Dementia 12 (3): 292–323. March 2016. doi:10.1016/j.jalz.2016.02.002. PMID 27012484.
- ↑ preparation by László Seress in 1980.
- ↑ 10.0 10.1 10.2 Duvernoy, 2005
- ↑ "cornu ammonis". http://medical-dictionary.thefreedictionary.com/cornu+ammonis.
- ↑ "Hippocampus minor, calcar avis, and the Huxley-Owen debate". Neurosurgery 65 (6): 1098–1104; discussion 1104–1105. December 2009. doi:10.1227/01.neu.0000359535.84445.0b. PMID 19934969.
- ↑ Gross, 1993
- ↑ Pang, CC; Kiecker, C; O'Brien, JT; Noble, W; Chang, RC (April 2019). "Ammon's Horn 2 (CA2) of the Hippocampus: A Long-Known Region with a New Potential Role in Neurodegeneration.". The Neuroscientist 25 (2): 167–180. doi:10.1177/1073858418778747. PMID 29865938. https://kclpure.kcl.ac.uk/portal/en/publications/ammons-horn-2-ca2-of-the-hippocampus(0547351a-ef50-44ac-b15d-e192706f446b).html.
- ↑ "The limbic system conception and its historical evolution". TheScientificWorldJournal 11: 2428–2441. 2011. doi:10.1100/2011/157150. PMID 22194673.
- ↑ "Chapter 9: Limbic System". https://www.dartmouth.edu/~rswenson/NeuroSci/chapter_9.html.
- ↑ Andersen, Per; Morris, Richard; Amaral, David; Bliss, Tim; O'Keefe, John (2006). The Hippocampus Book. Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0199880133. https://books.google.com/books?id=lc2xMroaWDUC&q=pes+hippocampus&pg=PT137. Retrieved 25 October 2020.
- ↑ Alberts, Daniel Albert (2012). Dorland's illustrated medical dictionary. (32nd ed.). Philadelphia, PA: Saunders/Elsevier. p. 860. ISBN 978-1416062578.
- ↑ 19.0 19.1 19.2 Purves, Dale (2011). Neuroscience (5th ed.). Sunderland, MA: Sinauer. pp. 730–735. ISBN 978-0878936953.
- ↑ 20.0 20.1 20.2 20.3 Amaral and Lavenex, 2006
- ↑ Purves, Dale (2011). Neuroscience (5. ed.). Sunderland, Mass.: Sinauer. p. 590. ISBN 978-0878936953.
- ↑ Moser and Moser, 1998
- ↑ Murakami, Gen; Tsurugizawa, Tomokazu; Hatanaka, Yusuke; Komatsuzaki, Yoshimasa; Tanabe, Nobuaki; Mukai, Hideo; Hojo, Yasushi; Kominami, Shiro et al. (December 2006). "Comparison between basal and apical dendritic spines in estrogen-induced rapid spinogenesis of CA1 principal neurons in the adult hippocampus". Biochemical and Biophysical Research Communications 351 (2): 553–558. doi:10.1016/j.bbrc.2006.10.066. PMID 17070772. "CA1 neurons consist of four regions, i.e., the stratum oriens, the cell body, the stratum radiatum, and the stratum lacunosum-moleculare".
- ↑ Rissman, Robert A.; Nocera, Romy; Fuller, Laura M.; Kordower, Jeffrey H.; Armstrong, David M. (February 2006). "Age-related alterations in GABAA receptor subunits in the nonhuman primate hippocampus". Brain Research 1073-1074: 120–130. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2005.12.036. PMID 16430870.
- ↑ 25.0 25.1 Eichenbaum et al., 2007
- ↑ 26.0 26.1 Kandel, 2012
- ↑ Purves, Dale (2011). Neuroscience (5th ed.). Sunderland, Mass.: Sinauer. p. 171. ISBN 978-0878936953.
- ↑ "Introduction to Neurons and Neuronal Networks | Section 1, Intro Chapter | Neuroscience Online: An Electronic Textbook for the Neurosciences | Department of Neurobiology and Anatomy – The University of Texas Medical School at Houston". http://neuroscience.uth.tmc.edu/s1/introduction.html.
- ↑ Winson, 1978
- ↑ 30.0 30.1 Fanselow, 2010
- ↑ Pothuizen et al., 2004
- ↑ Jung et al., 1994
- ↑ Cenquizca et al., 2007
- ↑ Anagnostaras et al., 2002
- ↑ Finger, S (2001). "Defining and controlling the circuits of emotion". Origins of neuroscience: a history of explorations into brain function. Oxford/New York: Oxford University Press. p. 286. ISBN 978-0195065039.
- ↑ Finger, p. 183
- ↑ Van Groen, Thomas; Wyss, J. Michael (1990). "Extrinsic projections from area CA1 of the rat hippocampus: olfactory, cortical, subcortical, and bilateral hippocampal formation projections". Journal of Comparative Neurology 302 (3): 515–528. doi:10.1002/cne.903020308. PMID 1702115.
- ↑ Eichenbaum et al., 1991
- ↑ Vanderwolf, 2001
- ↑ Nadel et al., 1975
- ↑ Gray and McNaughton, 2000
- ↑ Best & White, 1999
- ↑ Scoville and Milner, 1957
- ↑ New York Times, 12-06-2008
- ↑ Squire, 2009
- ↑ 46.0 46.1 Squire, 1992
- ↑ 47.0 47.1 Eichenbaum and Cohen, 1993
- ↑ "Episodic Memories: How do the Hippocampus and the Entorhinal Ring Attractors Cooperate to Create Them?". Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience 14: 68. September 2020. doi:10.3389/fnsys.2020.559186. PMID 33013334.
- ↑ O'Keefe and Dostrovsky, 1971
- ↑ O'Keefe and Nadel, 1978
- ↑ 51.0 51.1 51.2 51.3 Moser et al., 2008
- ↑ "Memory and Space: Towards an Understanding of the Cognitive Map". The Journal of Neuroscience 35 (41): 13904–13911. October 2015. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2618-15.2015. PMID 26468191.
- ↑ "The hippocampus and declarative memory: Cognitive mechanisms and neural codes". Behavioural Brain Research 127 (1): 199–207. 2001. doi:10.1016/s0166-4328(01)00365-5. PMID 11718892.
- ↑ "Memory, navigation and theta rhythm in the hippocampal-entorhinal system". Nature Neuroscience 16 (2): 130–138. February 2013. doi:10.1038/nn.3304. PMID 23354386.
- ↑ 55.0 55.1 "The role of the hippocampus in approach-avoidance conflict decision-making: Evidence from rodent and human studies". Behavioural Brain Research 313: 345–357. October 2016. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2016.07.039. PMID 27457133.
- ↑ Squire and Schacter, 2002
- ↑ VanElzakker et al., 2008
- ↑ Gluck, Mark; Mercado, Eduardo; Myers, Catherine (2014). Learning and Memory From Brain to Behavior Second Edition. New York: Kevin Feyen. pp. 416. ISBN 978-1429240147.
- ↑ "Severe amnesia following bilateral medial temporal lobe damage occurring on two distinct occasions". Neurological Sciences 27 (2): 129–133. Jun 2006. doi:10.1007/s10072-006-0614-y. PMID 16816912.
- ↑ Squire and Schacter, 2002, Ch. 1
- ↑ "Primary CA1 and conditionally immortal MHP36 cell grafts restore conditional discrimination learning and recall in marmosets after excitotoxic lesions of the hippocampal CA1 field". Brain: A Journal of Neurology 122 (12): 2321–2335. December 1999. doi:10.1093/brain/122.12.2321. PMID 10581225.
- ↑ "The role of hippocampal parts in verbal memory and activation processes". International Journal of Psychophysiology 69 (3): 312. September 2008. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2008.05.328.
- ↑ Diana et al., 2007
- ↑ "The involvement of the anterior cingulate cortex in remote contextual fear memory". Science 304 (5672): 881–883. May 2004. doi:10.1126/science.1094804. PMID 15131309. Bibcode: 2004Sci...304..881F.
- ↑ "Experience-dependent epigenomic reorganization in the hippocampus". Learning & Memory 24 (7): 278–288. July 2017. doi:10.1101/lm.045112.117. PMID 28620075.
- ↑ Matsumura et al., 1999
- ↑ Rolls and Xiang, 2006
- ↑ Smith and Mizumori, 2006
- ↑ "Phase relationship between hippocampal place units and the EEG theta rhythm". Hippocampus 3 (3): 317–330. July 1993. doi:10.1002/hipo.450030307. PMID 8353611.
- ↑ Ekstrom et al., 2003
- ↑ "Anterior/posterior competitive deactivation/activation dichotomy in the human hippocampus as revealed by a 3D navigation task". PLOS ONE 9 (1): e86213. 2014-01-27. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0086213. PMID 24475088. Bibcode: 2014PLoSO...986213D.
- ↑ Maguire et al., 2000
- ↑ "Pattern of hippocampal shape and volume differences in blind subjects". NeuroImage 46 (4): 949–957. July 2009. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.01.071. PMID 19285559.
- ↑ O'Keefe and Nadel
- ↑ Chiu et al., 2004
- ↑ Morris et al., 1982
- ↑ Sutherland et al., 1982
- ↑ Sutherland et al., 2001
- ↑ Clark et al., 2005
- ↑ Solstad et al., 2008
- ↑ 81.0 81.1 "Examining the Role of the Human Hippocampus in Approach-Avoidance Decision Making Using a Novel Conflict Paradigm and Multivariate Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging". The Journal of Neuroscience 35 (45): 15039–15049. November 2015. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.1915-15.2015. PMID 26558775.
- ↑ 82.0 82.1 Buzsáki, 2006
- ↑ 83.0 83.1 Buzsáki et al., 1990
- ↑ 84.0 84.1 Skaggs et al., 2007
- ↑ Buzsáki, 2002
- ↑ Lubenov & Siapas, 2009
- ↑ Komisaruk, B. R. (1970). "Synchrony between limbic system theta activity and rhythmical behavior in rats". Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 70 (3): 482–492. doi:10.1037/h0028709. PMID 5418472.
- ↑ Cantero et al., 2003
- ↑ Vanderwolf, 1969
- ↑ Huerta & Lisman, 1993
- ↑ Numan, 1995
- ↑ Kahana et al., 2001
- ↑ Buzsáki, 1986
- ↑ Wilson & McNaughton, 1994
- ↑ Jackson et al., 2006
- ↑ Sutherland & McNaughton, 2000
- ↑ Buzsáki, G. (January 1989). "Two-stage model of memory trace formation: A role for "noisy" brain states". Neuroscience 31 (3): 551–570. doi:10.1016/0306-4522(89)90423-5. PMID 2687720.
- ↑ Buzsáki, 1989
- ↑ "Selective suppression of hippocampal ripples impairs spatial memory". Nature Neuroscience 12 (10): 1222–1223. October 2009. doi:10.1038/nn.2384. PMID 19749750.
- ↑ "Disruption of ripple-associated hippocampal activity during rest impairs spatial learning in the rat". Hippocampus 20 (1): 1–10. January 2010. doi:10.1002/hipo.20707. PMID 19816984.
- ↑ "Optogenetically Blocking Sharp Wave Ripple Events in Sleep Does Not Interfere with the Formation of Stable Spatial Representation in the CA1 Area of the Hippocampus". PLOS ONE 11 (10): e0164675. 19 Nov 2016. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0164675. PMID 27760158. Bibcode: 2016PLoSO..1164675K.
- ↑ Ramon y Cajal, 1894
- ↑ Hebb, 1949
- ↑ Bliss & Lømo, 1973
- ↑ 105.0 105.1 Malenka & Bear, 2004
- ↑ Cooke & Bliss, 2006
- ↑ 107.0 107.1 Nakazawa et al., 2004
- ↑ Hampel et al., 2008
- ↑ 109.0 109.1 Prull et al., 2000, p. 105
- ↑ 110.0 110.1 Prull et al., 2000, p. 107
- ↑ Erickson et al., 2011
- ↑ Joels, 2008
- ↑ Fu et al., 2010
- ↑ "A meta-analysis of structural brain abnormalities in PTSD". Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 30 (7): 1004–1031. 2006. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.03.004. PMID 16730374.
- ↑ "Meta-analysis of regional brain volumes in schizophrenia". The American Journal of Psychiatry 157 (1): 16–25. January 2000. doi:10.1176/ajp.157.1.16. PMID 10618008.
- ↑ "Structural neuroimaging studies in major depressive disorder. Meta-analysis and comparison with bipolar disorder". Archives of General Psychiatry 68 (7): 675–690. Jul 2011. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.60. PMID 21727252. see also MRI database at www.depressiondatabase.org
- ↑ Decker, Alexandra L.; Duncan, Katherine; Finn, Amy S.; Mabbott, Donald J. (2020-08-12). "Children's family income is associated with cognitive function and volume of anterior not posterior hippocampus" (in en). Nature Communications 11 (1): 4040. doi:10.1038/s41467-020-17854-6. ISSN 2041-1723. PMID 32788583. Bibcode: 2020NatCo..11.4040D.
- ↑ Campbell & MacQueen, 2004
- ↑ 119.0 119.1 "Decrease in cortisol reverses human hippocampal atrophy following treatment of Cushing's disease". Biological Psychiatry 46 (12): 1595–1602. December 1999. doi:10.1016/s0006-3223(99)00203-6. PMID 10624540.
- ↑ 120.0 120.1 120.2 120.3 120.4 Disorders, Institute of Medicine (US) Forum on Neuroscience and Nervous System (2011). Overview of the Glutamatergic System. National Academies Press (US). https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK62187/. Retrieved 5 February 2017.
- ↑ Garcia-Segura, pp. 170–171
- ↑ "Chronic stress-induced hippocampal vulnerability: the glucocorticoid vulnerability hypothesis". Reviews in the Neurosciences 19 (6): 395–411. 2008. doi:10.1515/revneuro.2008.19.6.395. PMID 19317179.
- ↑ "Cholesterol and perhaps estradiol protect against corticosterone-induced hippocampal CA3 dendritic retraction in gonadectomized female and male rats". Neuroscience 246: 409–421. August 2013. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.04.027. PMID 23618757.
- ↑ "DNA brain damage after stress in rats". Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 34 (4): 652–656. May 2010. doi:10.1016/j.pnpbp.2010.03.004. PMID 20226828.
- ↑ 125.0 125.1 125.2 Kuruba et al., 2009
- ↑ "Review: Hippocampal sclerosis in epilepsy: a neuropathology review". Neuropathol Appl Neurobiol 40 (5): 520–543. August 2014. doi:10.1111/nan.12150. PMID 24762203.
- ↑ Chang and Lowenstein, 2003
- ↑ Sloviter, 2005
- ↑ 129.0 129.1 129.2 129.3 129.4 129.5 Harrison, 2004
- ↑ "Verbal learning and hippocampal dysfunction in schizophrenia: A meta-analysis". Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 86: 166–175. March 2018. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.12.001. PMID 29223768. PMC 5818020. http://discovery.ucl.ac.uk/10045938/1/Antoniades_Verbal%20learning%20and%20hippocampal.pdf. Retrieved 2018-11-23.
- ↑ Goto & Grace, 2008
- ↑ Boyer et al., 2007
- ↑ "Long-term antipsychotic treatment and brain volumes: a longitudinal study of first-episode schizophrenia". Archives of General Psychiatry 68 (2): 128–137. February 2011. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.199. PMID 21300943.
- ↑ "Progressive brain changes in schizophrenia related to antipsychotic treatment? A meta-analysis of longitudinal MRI studies". Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 37 (8): 1680–1691. September 2013. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.06.001. PMID 23769814.
- ↑ "Schizophrenia – what does structural MRI show?". Tidsskrift for den Norske Laegeforening 133 (8): 850–853. April 2013. doi:10.4045/tidsskr.12.1084. PMID 23612107.
- ↑ 136.0 136.1 "Neuropathological studies of synaptic connectivity in the hippocampal formation in schizophrenia". Hippocampus 11 (5): 508–519. 2001. doi:10.1002/hipo.1067. PMID 11732704.
- ↑ "Evidence for oxidative DNA damage in the hippocampus of elderly patients with chronic schizophrenia". Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 12 (2): 167–175. 2004. doi:10.1097/00019442-200403000-00008. PMID 15010346.
- ↑ 138.0 138.1 "Transient global amnesia". The Hippocampus in Clinical Neuroscience. Frontiers of Neurology and Neuroscience. 34. 2014. pp. 143–149. doi:10.1159/000356431. ISBN 978-3-318-02567-5. http://www.bmj.com/cgi/content/short/4/5633/723-a. Retrieved 2018-08-15.
- ↑ "Aetiology of transient global amnesia". Lancet 352 (9125): 397–399. August 1998. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(98)01442-1. PMID 9717945.
- ↑ "Detection of intracranial venous reflux in patients of transient global amnesia". Neurology 66 (12): 1873–1877. June 2006. doi:10.1212/01.wnl.0000219620.69618.9d. PMID 16801653.
- ↑ "Reduced posterior hippocampal volume in posttraumatic stress disorder". The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 69 (7): 1087–1091. July 2008. doi:10.4088/jcp.v69n0707. PMID 18572983.
- ↑ "Hippocampal volume differences in Gulf War veterans with current versus lifetime posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms". Biological Psychiatry 69 (6): 541–548. March 2011. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.09.044. PMID 21094937. PMC 3259803. http://www.biologicalpsychiatryjournal.com/article/S0006-3223(10)01013-9/fulltext. Retrieved 2017-08-14.
- ↑ "Hippocampal volume and resilience in posttramatic stress disorder". 23 March 2011. https://www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2011/03/110322105257.htm.
- ↑ Carlson, Neil R. (2014). Physiology of Behavior (11 ed.). Pearson Education. p. 624. ISBN 978-1-292-02320-5.
- ↑ "Hippocampal volume in chronic posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD): MRI study using two different evaluation methods". Journal of Affective Disorders 94 (1–3): 121–126. August 2006. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2006.03.010. PMID 16701903. http://dbm.neuro.uni-jena.de/pdf-files/Jatzko-JAD06.pdf. Retrieved 2017-08-14.
- ↑ Stern, Robert (September–October 2019). "The New Phrenology". Skeptical Inquirer (Center for Inquiry) 43 (5): 52–56. https://skepticalinquirer.org/2019/09/the-new-phrenology/. Retrieved 2020-03-20.
- ↑ "Greater hippocampal volume is associated with PTSD treatment response". Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging 252: 36–39. June 2016. doi:10.1016/j.pscychresns.2016.05.001. PMID 27179314.
- ↑ "Whole-exome sequencing identifies recessive WDR62 mutations in severe brain malformations". Nature 467 (7312): 207–210. September 2010. doi:10.1038/nature09327. PMID 20729831. Bibcode: 2010Natur.467..207B.
- ↑ "The Spindle-Associated Microcephaly Protein, WDR62, Is Required for Neurogenesis and Development of the Hippocampus". Front Cell Dev Biol 8 (549353): 85–98. September 2020. doi:10.3389/fcell.2020.549353. PMID 3042990.
- ↑ West, 1990
- ↑ Suzuki et al., 2005
- ↑ 152.0 152.1 Jacobs, 2003
- ↑ Jacobs et al., 1990
- ↑ Aboitiz et al., 2003
- ↑ Rodríguez et al., 2002
- ↑ Colombo and Broadbent, 2000
- ↑ Shettleworth, 2003
- ↑ Nieuwenhuys, 1982
- ↑ Portavella et al., 2002
- ↑ Vargas et al., 2006
- ↑ Docampo-Seara2018
- ↑ Mizunami et al., 1998
- ↑ "The Hippocampome". https://hippocampome.org/.
References
- "The evolutionary origin of the mammalian isocortex: towards an integrated developmental and functional approach". The Behavioral and Brain Sciences 26 (5): 535–552. Oct 2003. doi:10.1017/S0140525X03000128. PMID 15179935.
- "Ch 3. Hippocampal Neuroanatomy". The Hippocampus Book. Oxford University Press. 2006. ISBN 978-0-19-510027-3.
- "The hippocampus and Pavlovian fear conditioning: reply to Bast et al". Hippocampus 12 (4): 561–565. 2002. doi:10.1002/hipo.10071. PMID 12201641. http://homepage.mac.com/sanagnos/19bastreply2002.pdf.
- "Placing hippocampal single-unit studies in a historical context". Hippocampus 9 (4): 346–351. 1999. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(1999)9:4<346::AID-HIPO2>3.0.CO;2-3. PMID 10495017.
- "Long-lasting potentiation of synaptic transmission in the dentate area of the anaesthetized rabbit following stimulation of the perforant path". The Journal of Physiology 232 (2): 331–356. Jul 1973. doi:10.1113/jphysiol.1973.sp010273. PMID 4727084.
- "Hippocampal abnormalities and memory deficits: new evidence of a strong pathophysiological link in schizophrenia". Brain Research Reviews 54 (1): 92–112. Apr 2007. doi:10.1016/j.brainresrev.2006.12.008. PMID 17306884.
- "Hallmarks of a common forebrain vertebrate plan: specialized pallial areas for spatial, temporal and emotional memory in actinopterygian fish". Brain Research Bulletin 66 (4–6): 397–399. Sep 2005. doi:10.1016/j.brainresbull.2005.03.021. PMID 16144602.
- "Neural plasticity in the ageing brain". Nature Reviews. Neuroscience 7 (1): 30–40. Jan 2006. doi:10.1038/nrn1809. PMID 16371948.
- "Hippocampal sharp waves: their origin and significance". Brain Research 398 (2): 242–252. Nov 1986. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(86)91483-6. PMID 3026567.
- "Two-stage model of memory trace formation: a role for "noisy" brain states". Neuroscience 31 (3): 551–570. 1989. doi:10.1016/0306-4522(89)90423-5. PMID 2687720.
- "Chapter 19 Chapter Spatial organization of physiological activity in the hippocampal region: Relevance to memory formation". Spatial organization of physiological activity in the hippocampal region: relevance to memory formation. Progress in Brain Research. 83. 1990. pp. 257–268. doi:10.1016/S0079-6123(08)61255-8. ISBN 9780444811493.
- "Theta oscillations in the hippocampus". Neuron 33 (3): 325–340. Jan 2002. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00586-X. PMID 11832222.
- Buzsáki, G (2006). Rhythms of the Brain. Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-530106-9.
- Ramón y Cajal S (1894). "The Croonian Lecture: La Fine Structure des Centres Nerveux". Proceedings of the Royal Society 55 (331–335): 444–468. doi:10.1098/rspl.1894.0063. Bibcode: 1894RSPS...55..444C.
- "The role of the hippocampus in the pathophysiology of major depression". Journal of Psychiatry & Neuroscience 29 (6): 417–426. Nov 2004. PMID 15644983.
- "Sleep-dependent theta oscillations in the human hippocampus and neocortex". The Journal of Neuroscience 23 (34): 10897–10903. Nov 2003. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-34-10897.2003. PMID 14645485.
- Carey, B (2008-12-04). "H. M., an Unforgettable Amnesiac, Dies at 82". The New York Times. https://www.nytimes.com/2008/12/05/us/05hm.html.
- "Getting lost: directed attention and executive functions in early Alzheimer's disease patients". Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders 17 (3): 174–180. 2004. doi:10.1159/000076353. PMID 14739541.
- "Epilepsy". The New England Journal of Medicine 349 (13): 1257–1266. Sep 2003. doi:10.1056/NEJMra022308. PMID 14507951.
- "Ch 22. The neurobiology of schizophrenia". Neurobiology of Mental Illness. Oxford University Press US. 2005. ISBN 978-0-19-518980-3.
- "Spatial organization of direct hippocampal field CA1 axonal projections to the rest of the cerebral cortex". Brain Research Reviews 56 (1): 1–26. Nov 2007. doi:10.1016/j.brainresrev.2007.05.002. PMID 17559940.
- "Hippocampus and remote spatial memory in rats". Hippocampus 15 (2): 260–272. 2005. doi:10.1002/hipo.20056. PMID 15523608.
- "Is the avian hippocampus a functional homologue of the mammalian hippocampus?". Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 24 (4): 465–484. Jun 2000. doi:10.1016/S0149-7634(00)00016-6. PMID 10817844.
- "Plasticity in the human central nervous system". Brain 129 (Pt 7): 1659–1673. Jul 2006. doi:10.1093/brain/awl082. PMID 16672292.
- "The afferent connections of the main and the accessory olfactory bulb formations in the rat: an experimental HRP-study". The Journal of Comparative Neurology 181 (2): 213–244. Sep 1978. doi:10.1002/cne.901810202. PMID 690266.
- "Imaging recollection and familiarity in the medial temporal lobe: a three-component model". Trends in Cognitive Sciences 11 (9): 379–386. Sep 2007. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2007.08.001. PMID 17707683.
- Duvernoy, HM (2005). "Introduction". The Human Hippocampus (3rd ed.). Berlin: Springer-Verlag. p. 1. ISBN 978-3-540-23191-2. https://books.google.com/books?id=5GkpPjk5z1IC&pg=PP1. Retrieved 2016-03-05.
- "Ch 7. Building a model of the hippocampus in olfaction and memory". Olfaction. MIT Press. 1991. ISBN 978-0-262-04124-9.
- Memory, Amnesia, and the Hippocampal System. MIT Press. 1993.
- "The medial temporal lobe and recognition memory". Annual Review of Neuroscience 30: 123–252. 2007. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.30.051606.094328. PMID 17417939.
- "Cellular networks underlying human spatial navigation" (PDF). Nature 425 (6954): 184–188. Sep 2003. doi:10.1038/nature01964. PMID 12968182. Bibcode: 2003Natur.425..184E. http://memory.psych.upenn.edu/Publications#EkstEtal03. Retrieved 2013-01-24.
- "Exercise training increases size of hippocampus and improves memory". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 108 (7): 3017–3022. Feb 2011. doi:10.1073/pnas.1015950108. PMID 21282661. Bibcode: 2011PNAS..108.3017E.
- "Are the dorsal and ventral hippocampus functionally distinct structures?". Neuron 65 (1): 7–19. Jan 2010. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2009.11.031. PMID 20152109.
- Finger, S (2001). Origins of Neuroscience: A History of Explorations Into Brain Function. Oxford University Press US. ISBN 978-0-19-514694-3.
- Garcia-Segura LM (2009). Hormones and Brain Plasticity. Oxford University Press US. ISBN 978-0-19-532661-1.
- "Hippocampal volume deficits associated with exposure to psychological trauma and posttraumatic stress disorder in adults: a meta-analysis". Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry 34 (7): 1181–1188. Oct 2010. doi:10.1016/j.pnpbp.2010.06.016. PMID 20600466.
- "Toxic effects of depression on brain function: impairment of delayed recall and the cumulative length of depressive disorder in a large sample of depressed outpatients". The American Journal of Psychiatry 165 (6): 731–739. Jun 2008. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.07040574. PMID 18381906.
- "Limbic and cortical information processing in the nucleus accumbens". Trends in Neurosciences 31 (11): 552–558. Nov 2008. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2008.08.002. PMID 18786735.
- The Neuropsychology of Anxiety: An Enquiry into the Functions of the Septo-Hippocampal System. Oxford University Press. 2000.
- "Hippocampus minor and man's place in nature: a case study in the social construction of neuroanatomy". Hippocampus 3 (4): 403–416. Oct 1993. doi:10.1002/hipo.450030403. PMID 8269033.
- "Core candidate neurochemical and imaging biomarkers of Alzheimer's disease". Alzheimer's & Dementia 4 (1): 38–48. Jan 2008. doi:10.1016/j.jalz.2007.08.006. PMID 18631949.
- "The hippocampus in schizophrenia: a review of the neuropathological evidence and its pathophysiological implications". Psychopharmacology 174 (1): 151–162. Jun 2004. doi:10.1007/s00213-003-1761-y. PMID 15205886.
- Hebb DO (1949). Organization of Behavior: a Neuropsychological Theory. New York: John Wiley. ISBN 978-0-471-36727-7.
- "Heightened synaptic plasticity of hippocampal CA1 neurons during a cholinergically induced rhythmic state". Nature 364 (6439): 723–725. Aug 1993. doi:10.1038/364723a0. PMID 8355787. Bibcode: 1993Natur.364..723H.
- "Hippocampal sharp waves and reactivation during awake states depend on repeated sequential experience". The Journal of Neuroscience 26 (48): 12415–22426. Nov 2006. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4118-06.2006. PMID 17135403.
- "Evolution of spatial cognition: sex-specific patterns of spatial behavior predict hippocampal size". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 87 (16): 6349–6352. Aug 1990. doi:10.1073/pnas.87.16.6349. PMID 2201026. Bibcode: 1990PNAS...87.6349J.
- "The evolution of the cognitive map". Brain, Behavior and Evolution 62 (2): 128–139. 2003. doi:10.1159/000072443. PMID 12937351.
- "Comparison of spatial firing characteristics of units in dorsal and ventral hippocampus of the rat". The Journal of Neuroscience 14 (12): 7347–7356. Dec 1994. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.14-12-07347.1994. PMID 7996180.
- "Theta returns". Current Opinion in Neurobiology 11 (6): 739–744. Dec 2001. doi:10.1016/S0959-4388(01)00278-1. PMID 11741027.
- Principles of Neural Science (5th ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill Medical. 2012. pp. 1490–1491. ISBN 9780071390118. OCLC 820110349.
- "Useless or helpful? The "limbic system" concept". Reviews in the Neurosciences 8 (2): 139–145. 1997. doi:10.1515/REVNEURO.1997.8.2.139. PMID 9344183.
- "Functional actions of corticosteroids in the hippocampus". European Journal of Pharmacology 583 (2–3): 312–321. Apr 2008. doi:10.1016/j.ejphar.2007.11.064. PMID 18275953.
- "Hippocampal neurogenesis and neural stem cells in temporal lobe epilepsy". Epilepsy & Behavior 14 (Suppl 1): 65–73. Jan 2009. doi:10.1016/j.yebeh.2008.08.020. PMID 18796338.
- "Hippocampal theta oscillations are travelling waves". Nature 459 (7246): 534–539. May 2009. doi:10.1038/nature08010. PMID 19489117. Bibcode: 2009Natur.459..534L. https://authors.library.caltech.edu/14755/2/Lubenov2009p4508Nature_supp.pdf. Retrieved 2019-07-13.
- "Knowing where and getting there: a human navigation network". Science 280 (5365): 921–924. May 1998. doi:10.1126/science.280.5365.921. PMID 9572740. Bibcode: 1998Sci...280..921M.
- "Navigation-related structural change in the hippocampi of taxi drivers". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 97 (8): 4398–4403. Apr 2000. doi:10.1073/pnas.070039597. PMID 10716738. Bibcode: 2000PNAS...97.4398M.
- "LTP and LTD: an embarrassment of riches". Neuron 44 (1): 5–21. Sep 2004. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2004.09.012. PMID 15450156.
- "Spatial- and task-dependent neuronal responses during real and virtual translocation in the monkey hippocampal formation". The Journal of Neuroscience 19 (6): 2381–2393. Mar 1999. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-06-02381.1999. PMID 10066288.
- "Path integration and the neural basis of the 'cognitive map'". Nature Reviews. Neuroscience 7 (8): 663–678. Aug 2006. doi:10.1038/nrn1932. PMID 16858394.
- "Mushroom bodies of the cockroach: their participation in place memory". The Journal of Comparative Neurology 402 (4): 520–537. Dec 1998. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(19981228)402:4<520::AID-CNE6>3.0.CO;2-K. PMID 9862324.
- "Place navigation impaired in rats with hippocampal lesions". Nature 297 (5868): 681–683. Jun 1982. doi:10.1038/297681a0. PMID 7088155. Bibcode: 1982Natur.297..681M.
- "Functional differentiation in the hippocampus". Hippocampus 8 (6): 608–619. 1998. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(1998)8:6<608::AID-HIPO3>3.0.CO;2-7. PMID 9882018.
- "Place cells, grid cells, and the brain's spatial representation system". Annual Review of Neuroscience 31: 69–89. 2008. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.31.061307.090723. PMID 18284371.
- "Slam on the brakes: a critique of Altman, Brunner, and Bayer's response-inhibition model of hippocampal function". Behavioral Biology 14 (2): 151–162. Jun 1975. doi:10.1016/S0091-6773(75)90148-0. PMID 1137539.
- "NMDA receptors, place cells and hippocampal spatial memory". Nature Reviews. Neuroscience 5 (5): 361–372. May 2004. doi:10.1038/nrn1385. PMID 15100719.
- Nieuwenhuys, R (1982). "An Overview of the Organization of the Brain of Actinopterygian Fishes". Am. Zool. 22 (2): 287–310. doi:10.1093/icb/22.2.287.
- "Effects of medial septal lesions on an operant go/no-go delayed response alternation task in rats.". Physiology & Behavior 58 (6): 1263–1271. 1995. doi:10.1016/0031-9384(95)02044-6. PMID 8623030. https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/abs/pii/0031938495020446. Retrieved 2020-03-09.
- "Evidence for semantic learning in profound amnesia: an investigation with patient H.M". Hippocampus 14 (4): 417–425. 2004. doi:10.1002/hipo.20005. PMID 15224979.
- "The hippocampus as a spatial map. Preliminary evidence from unit activity in the freely-moving rat". Brain Research 34 (1): 171–175. Nov 1971. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(71)90358-1. PMID 5124915.
- The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map. Oxford University Press. 1978. http://www.cognitivemap.net/HCMpdf/HCMChapters.html. Retrieved 2008-10-23.
- "The effects of telencephalic pallial lesions on spatial, temporal, and emotional learning in goldfish". Brain Research Bulletin 57 (3–4): 397–399. 2002. doi:10.1016/S0361-9230(01)00699-2. PMID 11922997.
- "Ammon's horn and the hippocampus". Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 71 (3): 351. Sep 2001. doi:10.1136/jnnp.71.3.351. PMID 11511709.
- "Dissociation of function between the dorsal and the ventral hippocampus in spatial learning abilities of the rat: a within-subject, within-task comparison of reference and working spatial memory". The European Journal of Neuroscience 19 (3): 705–712. Feb 2004. doi:10.1111/j.0953-816X.2004.03170.x. PMID 14984421.
- "Ch 2. Age-related changes in memory: A cognitive neuroscience perspective". The handbook of aging and cognition. Erlbaum. 2000. ISBN 978-0-8058-2966-2.
- "Spatial memory and hippocampal pallium through vertebrate evolution: insights from reptiles and teleost fish". Brain Research Bulletin 57 (3–4): 499–503. 2002. doi:10.1016/S0361-9230(01)00682-7. PMID 11923018.
- "Spatial view cells in the primate hippocampus and memory recall". Reviews in the Neurosciences 17 (1–2): 175–200. 2006. doi:10.1515/REVNEURO.2006.17.1-2.175. PMID 16703951.
- "Impact of aging on hippocampal function: plasticity, network dynamics, and cognition". Progress in Neurobiology 69 (3): 143–179. Feb 2003. doi:10.1016/S0301-0082(02)00126-0. PMID 12758108.
- "Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocampal lesions". Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 20 (1): 11–21. Feb 1957. doi:10.1136/jnnp.20.1.11. PMID 13406589.
- "Memory and hippocampal specialization in food-storing birds: challenges for research on comparative cognition". Brain, Behavior and Evolution 62 (2): 108–216. 2003. doi:10.1159/000072441. PMID 12937349.
- "Theta phase precession in hippocampal neuronal populations and the compression of temporal sequences". Hippocampus 6 (2): 149–176. 1996. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(1996)6:2<149::AID-HIPO6>3.0.CO;2-K. PMID 8797016.
- "EEG sharp waves and sparse ensemble unit activity in the macaque hippocampus". Journal of Neurophysiology 98 (2): 898–910. Aug 2007. doi:10.1152/jn.00401.2007. PMID 17522177.
- "The neurobiology of temporal lobe epilepsy: too much information, not enough knowledge". Comptes Rendus Biologies 328 (2): 143–153. Feb 2005. doi:10.1016/j.crvi.2004.10.010. PMID 15771000. https://comptes-rendus.academie-sciences.fr/biologies/articles/10.1016/j.crvi.2004.10.010/.
- "Hippocampal place cells, context, and episodic memory". Hippocampus 16 (9): 716–729. 2006. doi:10.1002/hipo.20208. PMID 16897724.
- "Representation of geometric borders in the entorhinal cortex". Science 322 (5909): 1865–1868. Dec 2008. doi:10.1126/science.1166466. PMID 19095945. Bibcode: 2008Sci...322.1865S.
- "Memory and the hippocampus: a synthesis from findings with rats, monkeys, and humans". Psychological Review 99 (2): 195–231. Apr 1992. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.99.2.195. PMID 1594723.
- The Neuropsychology of Memory. Guilford Press. 2002.
- "The legacy of patient H.M. for neuroscience". Neuron 61 (1): 6–9. Jan 2009. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2008.12.023. PMID 19146808.
- "Memory trace reactivation in hippocampal and neocortical neuronal ensembles". Current Opinion in Neurobiology 10 (2): 180–186. Apr 2000. doi:10.1016/S0959-4388(00)00079-9. PMID 10753801.
- "Spatial mapping: definitive disruption by hippocampal or medial frontal cortical damage in the rat". Neuroscience Letters 31 (3): 271–276. Aug 1982. doi:10.1016/0304-3940(82)90032-5. PMID 7133562.
- "Retrograde amnesia after hippocampal damage: recent vs. remote memories in two tasks". Hippocampus 11 (1): 27–42. 2001. doi:10.1002/1098-1063(2001)11:1<27::AID-HIPO1017>3.0.CO;2-4. PMID 11261770.
- "Male-specific volume expansion of the human hippocampus during adolescence". Cerebral Cortex 15 (2): 187–193. Feb 2005. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhh121. PMID 15238436.
- "The hippocampus as an olfacto-motor mechanism: were the classical anatomists right after all?". Behavioural Brain Research 127 (1–2): 25–47. Dec 2001. doi:10.1016/S0166-4328(01)00354-0. PMID 11718883.
- "Telencephalon and geometric space in goldfish". The European Journal of Neuroscience 24 (10): 2870–2878. Nov 2006. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.05174.x. PMID 17156211.
- "Environmental novelty is associated with a selective increase in Fos expression in the output elements of the hippocampal formation and the perirhinal cortex". Learning & Memory 15 (12): 899–908. Dec 2008. doi:10.1101/lm.1196508. PMID 19050162.
- Blueprints notes & cases: Neuroscience. Oxford: Blackwell Publishing. 2004. p. 37. ISBN 978-1-4051-0349-7. https://books.google.com/books?id=k8qv-6tqZL0C&pg=PA37. Retrieved 2016-03-05.
- "Chapter 2 Stereological studies of the hippocampus: A comparison of the hippocampal subdivisions of diverse species including hedgehogs, laboratory rodents, wild mice and men". Stereological studies of the hippocampus: a comparison of the hippocampal subdivisions of diverse species including hedgehogs, laboratory rodents, wild mice and men. Progress in Brain Research. 83. 1990. pp. 13–36. doi:10.1016/S0079-6123(08)61238-8. ISBN 9780444811493.
- "Reactivation of hippocampal ensemble memories during sleep". Science 265 (5172): 676–679. Jul 1994. doi:10.1126/science.8036517. PMID 8036517. Bibcode: 1994Sci...265..676W.
- "Loss of hippocampal theta rhythm results in spatial memory deficit in the rat". Science 201 (4351): 160–163. Jul 1978. doi:10.1126/science.663646. PMID 663646. Bibcode: 1978Sci...201..160W.
Further reading
- Hippocampus (Wiley)
- "Study of pallial neurogenesis in shark embryos and the evolutionary origin of the subventricular zone". Brain Structure and Function 223 (8): 3593–3612. July 2018. doi:10.1007/s00429-018-1705-2. PMID 29980930.
- Space, Time and Memory in the Hippocampal Formation. Springer. 2014. ISBN 978-3-7091-1292-2.
External links
- Stained brain slice images which include the "hippocampus" at the BrainMaps project
- Diagram of a Hippocampal Brain Slice
- Hippocampus – Cell Centered Database
- Temporal-lobe.com An interactive diagram of the rat parahippocampal-hippocampal region
- Search Hippocampus on BrainNavigator via BrainNavigator
Original source: https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Hippocampus.
Read more |