Biology:Episodic memory

From HandWiki
Short description: Memory of autobiographical events

Episodic memory is the memory of every day events (such as times, location geography, associated emotions, and other contextual information) that can be explicitly stated or conjured. It is the collection of past personal experiences that occurred at particular times and places; for example, the party on one's 7th birthday.[1] Along with semantic memory, it comprises the category of explicit memory, one of the two major divisions of long-term memory (the other being implicit memory).[2]

The term "episodic memory" was coined by Endel Tulving in 1972, referring to the distinction between knowing and remembering: knowing is factual recollection (semantic) whereas remembering is a feeling that is located in the past (episodic).[3]

One of the main components of episodic memory is the process of recollection, which elicits the retrieval of contextual information pertaining to a specific event or experience that has occurred. Tulving seminally defined three key properties of episodic memory recollection as:

Aside from Tulving, others named additional aspects of recollection, including visual imagery, narrative structure, retrieval of semantic information and feelings of familiarity.[4]

Events that are recorded into episodic memory may trigger episodic learning, i.e. a change in behavior that occurs as a result of an event,[5][6] such as a fear of dogs after being bitten by a dog.

Nine properties

There are essentially nine properties of episodic memory that collectively distinguish it from other types of memory. Other types of memory may exhibit a few of these properties, but only episodic memory has all nine:[7]

  1. Contain summary records of sensory-perceptual-conceptual-affective processing.
  2. Retain patterns of activation/inhibition over long periods.
  3. Often represented in the form of (visual) images.
  4. They always have a perspective (field or observer).
  5. Represent short time slices of experience.
  6. They are represented on a temporal dimension roughly in order of occurrence.
  7. They are subject to rapid forgetting.
  8. They make autobiographical remembering specific.
  9. They are recollectively experienced when accessed.

Cognitive neuroscience

The formation of new episodic memories requires the medial temporal lobe, a structure that includes the hippocampus. Without the medial temporal lobe, one is able to form new procedural memories (such as playing the piano) but cannot remember the events during which they happened (See the hippocampus and memory).

The prefrontal cortex (and in particular the right hemisphere) is also involved in the formation of new episodic memories (also known as episodic encoding). Patients with damage to the prefrontal cortex can learn new information, but tend to do so in a disordered fashion. For example, they might show normal recognition of an object they had seen in the past, but fail to recollect when or where it had been viewed.[8] Some researchers believe that the prefrontal cortex helps organize information for more efficient storage, drawing upon its role in executive function. Others believe that the prefrontal cortex underlies semantic strategies which enhance encoding, such as thinking about the meaning of the study material or rehearsing it in working memory.[9]

Other work has shown that portions of the inferior parietal lobe play a role in episodic memory, potentially acting as an accumulator to support the subjective feeling that something is “old”, or perhaps supporting mental imagery which allows you a sense of the vividness of memories. Indeed, bilateral damage to the inferior parietal lobe results in episodic memory that is largely intact, however it lacks details [10] and lesion patients report low levels of confidence in their memories.[11]

Researchers do not agree about how long episodic memories are stored in the hippocampus. Some researchers believe that episodic memories always rely on the hippocampus. Others believe the hippocampus only stores episodic memories for a short time, after which the memories are consolidated to the neocortex. The latter view is strengthened by recent evidence that neurogenesis in the adult hippocampus may ease the removal of old memories and increase the efficiency of forming new memories.[12]

Relationship to semantic memory

Endel Tulving originally described episodic memory as a record of a person's experience that held temporally dated information and spatio-temporal relations.[13] A feature of episodic memory that Tulving later elaborates on is that it allows an agent to imagine traveling back in time.[14] A current situation may cue retrieval of a previous episode, so that context that colours the previous episode is experienced at the immediate moment. The agent is provided with a means of associating previous feelings with current situations. Semantic memory, on the other hand, is a structured record of facts, concepts, and skills that we have acquired. Semantic information is derived from accumulated episodic memory. Episodic memory can be thought of as a "map" that ties together items in semantic memory. For example, all encounters with how a "dog" looks and sounds will make up the semantic representation of that word. All episodic memories concerning a dog will then reference this single semantic representation of "dog" and, likewise, all new experiences with the dog will modify the single semantic representation of that dog.

Together, semantic and episodic memory make up our declarative memory.[15] They each represent different parts of context to form a complete picture. As such, something that affects episodic memory can also affect semantic memory. For example, anterograde amnesia, from damage of the medial temporal lobe, is an impairment of declarative memory that affects both episodic and semantic memory operations.[16] Originally, Tulving proposed that episodic and semantic memory were separate systems that competed with each other in retrieval. However, this theory was rejected when Howard and Kahana completed experiments on latent semantic analysis (LSA) that supported the opposite. Instead of an increase in semantic similarity when there was a decrease in the strength of temporal associations, the two worked together so semantic cues on retrieval were strongest when episodic cues were strong as well.[17]

Age differences

Episodic memory emerges at approximately 3 to 4 years of age.[18] Activation of specific brain areas (mostly the hippocampus) seems to be different between younger (aged 23–39) and older people (aged 67–80) upon episodic memory retrieval.[19] Older people tend to activate both their left and right hippocampus, while younger people activate only the left one.

Relationship to emotion

The relationship between emotion and memory is complex, but generally, emotion tends to increase the likelihood that an event will be remembered later and that it will be remembered vividly. Flashbulb memory is one example of this. An example of this would be an experience such as a close family member dying or the Christmas that you got the exact toy you wanted as a kid. The experience holds so much emotional significance that it is encoded as an extremely vivid, almost picture-perfect memory. However, whether the vividness of the flashbulb memory is due to a virtual "flash" that occurs because of the emotional experience has been hotly contested. Flashbulb memories may occur because of our propensity to rehearse and retell those highly emotional events, which strengthens the memory.[20]

Pharmacological enhancement

In healthy adults, longterm visual episodic memory can be enhanced specifically[21] through administration of the Acetylcholine esterase inhibitor Donepezil, whereas verbal episodic memory can be improved in persons with the val/val genotype of the val158met polymorphism through administration of the CNS penetrant specific catecholamine-O-methyltransferase inhibitor Tolcapone.[22] Furthermore, episodic memory is enhanced through AZD3480, a selective agonist at the neuronal alpha4beta2 nicotinic receptor, which is developed by the company Targacept.[23] Currently, there are several other products developed by several companies—including new catecholamine-O-methyltransferase inhibitors with fewer side effects—that aim for improving episodic memory. A recent placebo controlled study found that DHEA, which is a functional cortisol antagonist, improves episodic memory in healthy young men (Alhaj et al. 2006).[24]

A 2015 meta-analysis of high quality evidence found that therapeutic doses of amphetamine and methylphenidate improve performance on working memory, episodic memory, and inhibitory control tests in normal healthy adults.[25]

Damage

  • Based on a review of behavioral studies, it is suggested that there may be selective damage to the limbic-prefrontal episodic memory system in some autistic people.[26] Another study points to evidence of autistic deficits in the episodic or self-conscious memory of personally experienced events.[27]
  • The label "amnesia" is most often given to patients with deficits in episodic memory.
  • Alzheimer's disease tends to damage the hippocampus before other brain areas.
  • A rare type of shellfish poisoning called amnesic shellfish poisoning or "ASP" quite effectively and irreversibly damages the hippocampus, rendering one amnesic.
  • Korsakoff's syndrome is caused by thiamine (vitamin B1) deficiency, a form of malnutrition which can be precipitated by overconsumption of alcohol compared to foods.
  • An acute cortisol level (by injection) has been found to significantly inhibit the recall of autobiographical memories[28] which may contribute to memory deficits found in depression.
  • The use of MDMA ("Ecstasy") has been associated with persistent deficits in episodic memory.[29][30]

In animals

Main page: Biology:Episodic-like memory

Tulving (1983) proposed that to meet the criteria of episodic memory, evidence of conscious recollection must be provided. Demonstrating episodic memory in the absence of language, and thus in non-human animals, is impossible, because there are no agreed upon non-linguistic behavioral indicators of conscious experience (Griffiths et al., 1999).

This idea was first challenged by Clayton and Dickinson in their work with the western scrub jay (Aphelocoma californica). They were able to demonstrate that these birds may possess an episodic-like memory system as they found that they remember where they cached different food types, and discriminately recovered them depending on the perishability of the item and time that elapsed since caching. Thus, scrub-jays appear to remember the "what-where-and-when" of specific past caching events. The authors argued that such performance meets the behavioral criteria for episodic memory, but referred to the ability as "episodic-like" memory because the study did not address the phenomenological aspects of episodic memory.

After a study done by the University of Edinburgh (2006), hummingbirds were the first animal to demonstrate two of the aspects of episodic memory—the ability to recall where certain flowers were located and how recently they were visited. Other studies have demonstrated this type of memory in different animal species, such as dogs,[31][32] rats, honey bees, and primates.

The ability of animals to encode and retrieve past experiences relies on the circuitry of the medial temporal lobe, a structure including the hippocampus.[33] Animal lesion studies have provided significant findings related to the importance of particular brain structures in episodic-like memory. For example, hippocampal lesions have severely impacted all three components (what, where, and when) in animals, suggesting that the hippocampus is responsible for detecting novel events, stimuli, and places when forming new memories and on retrieving that information later on.

Despite similar neural areas and evidence from experiments, some scholars remain cautious about comparisons to human episodic memory.[34] Purported episodic-like memory often seems fixed to a particular domain or could be explained in terms of procedural or semantic memory. The problem may be better tractable by studying episodic memory's adaptive counterpart: the capacity to flexibly imagine future events. However, a recent experiment addressed one of Suddendorf and Busby (2003)'s specific criticisms (the Bischof-Köhler hypothesis, which states that nonhuman animals can only take actions based on immediate needs, as opposed to future needs). Correia and colleagues demonstrated [35] that western scrub-jays can selectively cache different types of foods depending on which type of food they will desire at a future time, offering strong evidence against the Bischof-Köhler hypothesis by demonstrating that scrub-jays can flexibly adjust their behavior based on past experience of desiring a particular food. Similarities and differences between humans and other animals are currently much debated.[36]

Autobiographical memory

An autobiographical memory is a personal representation of general or specific events and personal facts. Autobiographical memory also refers to memory of a person's history. An individual does not remember exactly everything that has happened in one's past. Memory is constructive, where previous experience affects how we remember events and what we end up recalling from memory. Autobiographical memory is constructive and reconstructed as an evolving process of history. A person's autobiographical memory is fairly reliable, although the reliability of autobiographical memories is questionable because of memory distortions.[37]

Autobiographical memories can differ for special periods of life. People recall few personal events from the first years of their lives. The loss of these first events is called childhood or infantile amnesia. People tend to recall many personal events from adolescence and early adulthood. This effect is called the reminiscence bump. Additionally, people recall many personal events from their previous few years. For adolescents and young adults the reminiscence bump and the recent events can coincide.[38]

It is known that autobiographical memories initially are stored as episodic memories, but it is currently unknown if autobiographical memories are the same as episodic memories or if the autobiographical memories become converted to semantic memories with time.[39]

Types

  • Specific events
    • When you first set foot in the ocean.
  • General events
    • What it feels like stepping into the ocean in general. This is a memory of what a personal event is generally like. It might be based on the memories of having stepped in the ocean, many times during the years.
  • Flashbulb memories

Neural network models

Episodic memories can be stored in autoassociative neural networks (e.g., a Hopfield network) if the stored representation includes information on the spatiotemporal context in which an item was studied.[40]

Neural networks help us understand how the brain sends and receives different messages to the body, and how they are connected. These networks are a group of neurons or structures that are connected together. These structures work harmoniously to produce different cognitions within the brain. One of the largest proposals for this ideology is that of Diffusion Tensor Imaging.[41] This technique traces the differing pathways of nerve fibres that further create communication throughout differing structures. These networks can be thought of as neural maps that can expand or contract according to the information being processed at that time.[42]

References

  1. Schacter, Daniel L.; Gilbert, Daniel T.; Wegner, Daniel M. (2009). "Semantic and episodic memory". Psychology. pp. 185–6. ISBN 9780716752158. https://books.google.com/books?id=-9x8dngFRe0C&pg=PA185. 
  2. Tulving, Endel (2010). "Précis of Elements of episodic memory". Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7 (2): 223–238. doi:10.1017/S0140525X0004440X. 
  3. "Episodic memory". Current Biology 17 (6): R189–91. March 2007. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2007.01.011. PMID 17371752. 
  4. Assabis D; Aguire EA (July 2007). "Deconstructing episodic memory with construction". Trends in Cognitive Sciences 11 (7): 299–306. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2007.05.001. PMID 17548229. 
  5. Terry WS (2006). Learning and Memory: Basic principles, processes, and procedures. Boston: Pearson Education, Inc. [page needed]
  6. Cognition, Brain, and Consciousness: Introduction to cognitive neuroscience. London: Elsevier Ltd.. 2007. [page needed]
  7. "Episodic memories". Neuropsychologia 47 (11): 2305–13. September 2009. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.02.003. PMID 19524094. 
  8. "Source memory impairment in patients with frontal lobe lesions". Neuropsychologia 27 (8): 1043–56. 1989. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(89)90184-X. PMID 2797412. 
  9. "The role of left prefrontal cortex in language and memory". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 95 (3): 906–13. February 1998. doi:10.1073/pnas.95.3.906. PMID 9448258. Bibcode1998PNAS...95..906G. 
  10. Berryhill, M.E.; Picasso, L.; Phuong, L.; Cabeza, R.; Olson, I.R. (2007). "Parietal lobe and episodic memory: bilateral damage causes impaired free recall of autobiographical memory". Journal of Neuroscience 27 (52): 14415–23. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4163-07.2007. PMID 18160649. 
  11. Hower, K., Wixted, J.; Berryhill, M.E., Olson, I.R. (2014). "Impaired perception of mnemonic oldness, but not mnemonic newness, after parietal lobe damage". Neuropsychologia 56: 409–17. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.02.014. PMID 24565734. 
  12. "Excitation-neurogenesis coupling in adult neural stem/progenitor cells". Neuron 42 (4): 535–52. May 2004. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(04)00266-1. PMID 15157417. 
  13. Tulving, Endel (1983). Elements of Episodic Memory. New York: Oxford University Press. [page needed]
  14. "Episodic memory: from mind to brain". Annual Review of Psychology 53: 1–25. 2002. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135114. PMID 11752477. 
  15. "Priming and human memory systems". Science 247 (4940): 301–6. January 1990. doi:10.1126/science.2296719. PMID 2296719. Bibcode1990Sci...247..301T. 
  16. "Episodic and declarative memory: role of the hippocampus". Hippocampus 8 (3): 198–204. 1998. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(1998)8:3<198::AID-HIPO2>3.0.CO;2-G. PMID 9662134. 
  17. "When Does Semantic Similarity Help Episodic Retrieval?". Journal of Memory and Language 46: 85–98. 2002. doi:10.1006/jmla.2001.2798. 
  18. "To have and to hold: episodic memory in 3- and 4-year-old children". Developmental Psychobiology 55 (2): 125–32. March 2013. doi:10.1002/dev.21004. PMID 22213009. 
  19. "Aging affects the engagement of the hippocampus during autobiographical memory retrieval". Brain 126 (Pt 7): 1511–23. July 2003. doi:10.1093/brain/awg157. PMID 12805116. 
  20. McCloskey, Michael; Wible, Cynthia G.; Cohen, Neal J. (1988). "Is there a special flashbulb-memory mechanism?". Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 117 (2): 171–181. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.117.2.171. 
  21. "Cholinergic enhancement of episodic memory in healthy young adults". Psychopharmacology 182 (1): 170–9. October 2005. doi:10.1007/s00213-005-0043-2. PMID 16021483. 
  22. "Tolcapone improves cognition and cortical information processing in normal human subjects". Neuropsychopharmacology 32 (5): 1011–20. May 2007. doi:10.1038/sj.npp.1301227. PMID 17063156. 
  23. "Effects of TC-1734 (AZD3480), a selective neuronal nicotinic receptor agonist, on cognitive performance and the EEG of young healthy male volunteers". Psychopharmacology 191 (4): 919–29. May 2007. doi:10.1007/s00213-006-0675-x. PMID 17225162. 
  24. "Effects of DHEA administration on episodic memory, cortisol and mood in healthy young men: a double-blind, placebo-controlled study". Psychopharmacology 188 (4): 541–51. November 2006. doi:10.1007/s00213-005-0136-y. PMID 16231168. 
  25. "Prescription Stimulants' Effects on Healthy Inhibitory Control, Working Memory, and Episodic Memory: A Meta-analysis". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 27 (6): 1069–89. June 2015. doi:10.1162/jocn_a_00776. PMID 25591060. https://repository.upenn.edu/neuroethics_pubs/130. 
  26. "Memory in autism: review and synthesis". Cortex; A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior 39 (4–5): 1129–38. 2003. doi:10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70881-5. PMID 14584570. 
  27. "Self-ordered pointing in children with autism: failure to use verbal mediation in the service of working memory?". Neuropsychologia 43 (10): 1400–11. 2005. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.01.010. PMID 15989932. 
  28. "Autobiographic memory impairment following acute cortisol administration". Psychoneuroendocrinology 29 (8): 1093–6. September 2004. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2003.09.006. PMID 15219661. 
  29. "Cognitive performance in recreational users of MDMA of 'ecstasy': evidence for memory deficits". Journal of Psychopharmacology 12 (1): 79–83. 2016. doi:10.1177/026988119801200110. PMID 9584971. 
  30. "Memory deficits associated with recreational use of "ecstasy" (MDMA)". Psychopharmacology 141 (1): 30–6. January 1999. doi:10.1007/s002130050803. PMID 9952062. 
  31. Panko, Ben. "Dogs May Possess a Type of Memory Once Considered 'Uniquely Human'". http://www.smithsonianmag.com/science-nature/dogs-remember-more-than-we-think-180961219/. 
  32. "Recall of Others' Actions after Incidental Encoding Reveals Episodic-like Memory in Dogs". Current Biology 26 (23): 3209–3213. December 2016. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2016.09.057. PMID 27889264. 
  33. Clayton, N. S.; Griffiths, D. P.; Emery, N. J.; Dickinson, A. (2001). "Elements of Episodic-like Memory in Animals". Philosophical Transactions: Biological Sciences 356 (1413): 1483–1491. doi:10.1098/rstb.2001.0947. ISSN 0962-8436. PMID 11571038. 
  34. "Mental time travel in animals?". Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7 (9): 391–396. September 2003. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00187-6. PMID 12963469. 
  35. "Western scrub-jays anticipate future needs independently of their current motivational state". Current Biology 17 (10): 856–61. May 2007. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2007.03.063. PMID 17462894. 
  36. "A spoon full of studies helps the comparison go down: a comparative analysis of Tulving's spoon test". Frontiers in Psychology 5: 893. 2014. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00893. PMID 25161644. 
  37. Brewer, William F. (1986), "What is autobiographical memory?", Autobiographical Memory, Cambridge University Press, pp. 25–49, doi:10.1017/cbo9780511558313.006, ISBN 978-0-511-55831-3 
  38. Jansari, Ashok; Parkin, Alan J. (1996). "Things that go bump in your life: Explaining the reminiscence bump in autobiographical memory.". Psychology and Aging 11 (1): 85–91. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.11.1.85. ISSN 0882-7974. PMID 8726374. 
  39. Piolino, Pascale; Desgranges, Béatrice; Benali, Karim; Eustache, Francis (July 2002). "Episodic and semantic remote autobiographical memory in ageing". Memory 10 (4): 239–257. doi:10.1080/09658210143000353. ISSN 0965-8211. PMID 12097209. 
  40. Khalil, Radwa; Moftah, Marie Z.; Moustafa, Ahmed A. (2017-10-19). "The effects of dynamical synapses on firing rate activity: a spiking neural network model". European Journal of Neuroscience 46 (9): 2445–2470. doi:10.1111/ejn.13712. ISSN 0953-816X. PMID 28921686. 
  41. Henderson, Jaimie M. (2012). ""Connectomic surgery": diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) tractography as a targeting modality for surgical modulation of neural networks". Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience 6: 15. doi:10.3389/fnint.2012.00015. ISSN 1662-5145. PMID 22536176. 
  42. Nestor, Paul G.; Kubicki, Marek; Gurrera, Ronald J.; Niznikiewicz, Margaret; Frumin, Melissa; McCarley, Robert W.; Shenton, Martha E. (October 2004). "Neuropsychological Correlates of Diffusion Tensor Imaging in Schizophrenia.". Neuropsychology 18 (4): 629–637. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.18.4.629. ISSN 1931-1559. PMID 15506830. 

Further reading

External links

Note: This topic belongs to "Psychology" portal