Social:Trait theory

From HandWiki
Short description: Approach to the study of human personality

In psychology, trait theory (also called dispositional theory) is an approach to the study of human personality. Trait theorists are primarily interested in the measurement of traits, which can be defined as habitual patterns of behavior, thought, and emotion.[1] According to this perspective, traits are aspects of personality that are relatively stable over time, differ across individuals (e.g. some people are outgoing whereas others are not), are relatively consistent over situations, and influence behaviour. Traits are in contrast to states, which are more transitory dispositions.

In some theories and systems, traits are something a person either has or does not have, but in many others traits are dimensions such as extraversion vs. introversion, with each person rating somewhere along this spectrum.

Trait theory suggests that some natural behaviours may give someone an advantage in a position of leadership.[2]

There are two approaches to define traits: as internal causal properties or as purely descriptive summaries. The internal causal definition states that traits influence our behaviours, leading us to do things in line with that trait. On the other hand, traits as descriptive summaries are descriptions of our actions that do not try to infer causality.[3]

History

Gordon Allport was an early pioneer in the study of traits. This early work was viewed as the beginning of the modern psychological study of personality.[4] He also referred to traits within his work as dispositions. In his approach, "cardinal" traits are those that dominate and shape a person's behavior; their ruling passions/obsessions, such as a need for money, fame etc. By contrast, "central" traits such as honesty are characteristics found in some degree in every person – and finally "secondary" traits are those seen only in certain circumstances (such as particular likes or dislikes that a very close friend may know), which are included to provide a complete picture of human complexity.[5]

A wide variety of alternative theories and scales were later developed, including:

Currently, two general approaches are the most popular:[citation needed]

  • Eysenck Personality Questionnaire, (EPQ) ("the three-factor model"). Using factor analysis Hans Eysenck suggested that personality is reducible to three major traits: neuroticism, extraversion, and psychoticism.[6]
  • Big Five personality traits, ("the five-factor model"). Many psychologists currently believe that five factors are sufficient: neuroticism, extraversion, openness to experience, agreeableness, and conscientiousness.[7][8]

Trait theory in cross-cultural use

Cultures are widely known and accepted as being different in varying degrees. This can make the study of personality difficult as meaning and the expression of traits may be different within cultural groups. Trait theory uses a hierarchy of traits in order to separate culture from the traits; it can be said the culture is ignored in order to focus on the individual traits and how they are connected to the individual.[9] Gordon Allport's trait theory not only served as a foundational approach within personality psychology, but also is continued to be viewed and discussed by other disciplines such as anthropology because of how he approached culture within trait theory.[9]

Trait theory tends to focus on the individual over the situation in which they are in.[10] This focus has relaxed within modern studies allowing for a consideration of the external factors outside of the self. As the focus becomes more relaxed (but still prominent as it is a main part of the theory) research expands.

Comparing EPQ and Big Five

Testing methodology, and factors

Both the EPQ and Big Five approaches extensively use self-report questionnaires. The factors are intended to be orthogonal (uncorrelated),[11] though there are often small positive correlations between factors. The five factor model in particular has been criticized for losing the orthogonal structure between factors.[12] Hans Eysenck has argued that fewer factors are superior to a larger number of partly related ones.[13] Although these two approaches are comparable because of the use of factor analysis to construct hierarchical taxonomies, they differ in the organization and number of factors.

Whatever the causes, psychoticism marks the two approaches apart, as the five factor model contains no such trait. Moreover, psychoticism, unlike any of the other factors in either approach, does not fit a normal distribution curve. Indeed, scores are rarely high, thus skewing a normal distribution.[14] However, when they are high, there is considerable overlap with psychiatric conditions such as antisocial and schizoid personality disorders. Similarly, high scorers on neuroticism are more susceptible to sleep and psychosomatic disorders.[15] Five factor approaches can also predict future mental disorders.[16]

Lower-order factors

Similarities between lower-order factors for psychoticism and the facets of openness, agreeableness, and conscientiousness (from Matthews, Deary & Whiteman, 2003)

There are two higher-order factors that both taxonomies clearly share: extraversion and neuroticism. Both approaches broadly accept that extraversion is associated with sociability and positive affect, whereas neuroticism is associated with emotional instability and negative affect.[14]

Many lower-order factors, or facets, are similar between the two taxonomies. For instance, both approaches contain factors for sociability/gregariousness, for activity levels, and for assertiveness within the higher order factor extraversion. However, there are differences too. First, the three-factor approach contains nine lower-order factors and the five-factor approach has six.[14]

Eysenck's psychoticism factor incorporates some of the polar opposites of the lower order factors of openness, agreeableness and conscientiousness. A high scorer on tough-mindedness in psychoticism would score low on tender-mindedness in agreeableness. Most of the differences between the taxonomies stem from the three factor model's emphasis on fewer high-order factors.

Causality

Although both major trait models are descriptive, only the three-factor model offers a detailed causal explanation. Eysenck suggests that different personality traits are caused by the properties of the brain, which themselves are the result of genetic factors.[17] In particular, the three-factor model identifies the reticular system and the limbic system in the brain as key components that mediate cortical arousal and emotional responses respectively. Eysenck advocates that extraverts have low levels of cortical arousal and introverts have high levels, leading extraverts to seek out more stimulation from socializing and being venturesome.[18] Moreover, Eysenck surmised that there would be an optimal level of arousal, after which inhibition would occur and that this would be different for each person.[19]

In a similar vein, the three-factor approach theorizes that neuroticism is mediated by levels of arousal in the limbic system and that individual differences arise because of variable activation thresholds between people. Therefore, highly neurotic people when presented with minor stressors, will exceed this threshold, whereas people low in neuroticism will not exceed normal activation levels, even when presented with large stressors. By contrast, proponents of the five-factor approach assume a role of genetics[8] and environment[20] but offer no explicit causal explanation.

Given this emphasis on biology in the three-factor approach, it would be expected that the third trait, psychoticism, would have a similar explanation. However, the causal properties of this state are not well defined. Eysenck has suggested that psychoticism is related to testosterone levels and is an inverse function of the serotonergic system,[21] but he later revised this, linking it instead to the dopaminergic system.[22]

List of personality traits

Personality traits
Openness to experience Composed of two related but separable traits, Openness to Experience and Intellect. Behavioral aspects include having wide interests, and being imaginative and insightful, correlated with activity in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Considered primarily a cognitive trait.[23]
Conscientiousness Scrupulous, meticulous, principled behavior guided or conforming to one's own conscience. Associated with the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex.[24]
Extraversion Gregarious, outgoing, sociable, projecting one's personality outward. The opposite of extraversion is introversion. Extraversion has shown to share certain genetic markers with substance abuse. Extraversion is associated with various regions of the prefrontal cortex and the amygdala.[25]
Agreeableness Refers to a compliant, trusting, empathic, sympathetic, friendly and cooperative nature.[26]
Neuroticism Identifies people who are prone to psychological distress. Individuals who are high in neuroticism tend to be anxious, depressed, self-conscious, impulsive, vulnerable and display angry hostility. "Neuroticism is the major factor of personality pathology."[27] Neuroticism has been linked to serotonin transporter (5-HTT) binding sites in the thalamus: as well as activity in the insular cortex.[28] Neuroticism also predicts the occurrence of more negative life experiences.[20]
Honesty-humility Tendency towards sincerity, modesty, fairness, and greed avoidance. Those who score high on this trait feel little desire to manipulate others or to break the rules for personal gain.
Self-esteem (low) A "favorable or unfavorable attitude toward oneself".[29] An individual's sense of his or her value or worth, or the extent to which a person values, approves of, appreciates, prizes, or likes him or herself".[30]
Harm avoidance A tendency towards shyness, being fearful and uncertain, tendency to worry. Neonatal complications such as preterm birth have been shown to affect harm avoidance. People affected by eating disorders exhibit high levels of harm avoidance.[31] The volume of the left amygdala in girls was correlated to levels of HA, in separate studies HA was correlated with reduced grey matter volume in the orbitofrontal, occipital and parietal regions.[32]
Novelty seeking Impulsive, exploratory, fickle, excitable, quick-tempered, and extravagant. Associated with addictive behavior.
Sensory processing sensitivity (SPS) The defining trait of highly sensitive persons, characterized by the increased depth of processing of sensory input that underlies HSPs' greater proclivity to overstimulation, emotional reactivity and empathy, and sensitivity to stimuli.[33]
Perfectionism "I don't think needing to be perfect is in any way adaptive." (Paul Hewitt, PhD)[34]

Socially prescribed perfectionism – "believing that others will value you only if you are perfect."

Self-oriented perfectionism – "an internally motivated desire to be perfect."

Perfectionism is one of the traits associated with obsessional behavior and like obsessionality is also believed to be regulated by the basal ganglia.[35]

Alexithymia The inability to express emotions. "To have no words for one's inner experience".[36] In studies done with stroke patients, alexithymia was found to be more prevalent in those who developed lesions in the right hemisphere following a cerebral infarction. There is a positive association with post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), childhood abuse and neglect and alexithymia. Utilizing psychometric testing and fMRI, studies showed positive response in the insula, posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), and thalamus.[37]
Rigidity Inflexibility, difficulty making transitions, adherence to set patterns. Mental rigidity arises out of a deficit of the executive functions. Originally termed frontal lobe syndrome it is also referred to as dysexecutive syndrome and usually occurs as a result of damage to the frontal lobe. This may be due to physical damage, disease (such as Huntington's disease) or a hypoxic or anoxic insult.[38]
1=Bechara, A.; Damasio, A. R.; Damasio, H.; Anderson, S. W. (Apr–Jun 1994). "Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex". Cognition 50 (1–3): 7–15. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(94)90018-3. PMID 8039375.  2=Eysenck, S. B.; Eysenck, H. J. (Feb 1977). "The place of impulsiveness in a dimensional system of personality description". British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology 16 (1): 57–68. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8260.1977.tb01003.x. PMID 843784.  3=Welch, S. L.; Fairburn, C. G. (Oct 1996). "Impulsivity or comorbidity in bulimia nervosa. A controlled study of deliberate self-harm and alcohol and drug misuse in a community sample". British Journal of Psychiatry 169 (4): 451–8. doi:10.1192/bjp.169.4.451. PMID 8894196.  4=Corstorphine, E.; Waller, G.; Lawson, R.; Ganis, C. (Jan 2007). "Trauma and multi-impulsivity in the eating disorders". Eating Behaviors 8 (1): 23–30. doi:10.1016/j.eatbeh.2004.08.009. PMID 17174848.  5=Patton, J. H.; Stanford, M. S.; Barratt, E. S. (November 1995). "Factor structure of the Barratt impulsiveness scale". Journal of Clinical Psychology 51 (6): 768–74. doi:10.1002/1097-4679(199511)51:6<768::aid-jclp2270510607>3.0.co;2-1. PMID 8778124.  6=Chamberlain, S. R.; Sahakian, B. J. (May 2007). "The neuropsychiatry of impulsivity". Current Opinion in Psychiatry 20 (3): 255–61. doi:10.1097/YCO.0b013e3280ba4989. PMID 17415079. 

}}</ref>

Disinhibition Behavioral disinhibition is an inability or unwillingness to constrain impulses, it is a key component of executive functioning. Researchers have emphasized poor behavioral inhibition as the central impairment of ADHD. It may be symptomatic of orbitofrontal lobe syndrome, a subtype of frontal lobe syndrome which may be an acquired disorder as a result of traumatic brain injury, hypoxic ischemic encephalopathy (HIE), anoxic encephalopathy, degenerative diseases such as Parkinson's, bacterial or viral infections such as Lyme disease and neurosyphilis. Disinhibition has been consistently associated with substance abuse disorders, obesity, higher BMI, excessive eating, an increased rate of eating, and perceived hunger.[39]
Psychoticism Psychoticism is a personality pattern typified by aggressiveness and interpersonal hostility, one of four traits in Hans Eysenck's model of personality. High levels of this trait were believed by Eysenck to be linked to increased vulnerability to psychosis such as schizophrenia. He also believed that blood relatives of psychotics would show high levels of this trait, suggesting a genetic basis to the trait.[40]
Obsessionality Persistent, often unwelcome, and frequently disturbing ideas, thoughts, images or emotions, rumination, often inducing an anxious state. Obsessionality may result as a dysfunction of the basal ganglia.[41]

See also

References

  1. Kassin, Saul (2003). Psychology. U.S.: Prentice-Hall, Inc.. 
  2. Northouse, Peter Guy (2010). Leadership: theory and practice /. Sage Publications. ISBN 978-1-4129-7488-2. https://cmc.marmot.org/Record/.b31214915. 
  3. Abel, Steve. "What Is a Trait Two Basic Formulations". https://www.doctorabel.us/personality-psychology/what-is-a-trait-two-basic-formulations.html. 
  4. Long, Lewis M. K. (1952). "Alfred Adler and Gordon W. Allport: A Comparison on Certain Topics in Personality Theory" (in en). American Journal of Individual Psychology (Chicago, Ill.) 10 (1). https://www.proquest.com/openview/8bf99aa9b92cd48af4a112ceb7a01078/1?pq-origsite=gscholar&cbl=1816607. Retrieved 2018-03-23. 
  5. Allport, Gordon W. (1961). Pattern and growth in personality (14 print. ed.). New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. ISBN 978-0030108105. https://archive.org/details/patterngrowthinp00allprich. 
  6. McCrae, R. R.; Costa, P. C. Jr. (1987). "Validation of the five-factor model across instruments and observers". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 52 (1): 81–90. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.52.1.81. PMID 3820081. 
  7. 8.0 8.1 Costa, P. T.; McCrae, R. R. (1992). "Four ways five factors are basic". Personality and Individual Differences 13 (6): 653–665. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(92)90236-i. 
  8. 9.0 9.1 Marsella, Anthony J.; Dubanoski, Joan; Hamada, Winter C.; Morse, Heather (2000). "The measurement of personality across cultures: Historical conceptual, and methodological issues and considerations". American Behavioral Scientist 44 (1): 41–62. doi:10.1177/00027640021956080. 
  9. Mischel, Walter; Shoda, Yuichi (1998-02-01). "Reconciling processing dynamics and personality dispositions". Annual Review of Psychology 49 (1): 229–258. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.49.1.229. ISSN 0066-4308. PMID 9496625. 
  10. Eysenck, Hans (1990). "Biological dimensions of personality". in Pervin, L. A.. Handbook of personality: Theory and research. New York: Guilford. pp. 244–76. 
    • Block, J. (1995). "A contrarian view of the five-factor approach to personality description". Psychological Bulletin 117 (2): 187–215. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.117.2.187. PMID 7724687. 
    • Draycott, S. G.; Kline, P. (1995). "The Big Three or the Big Five—the EPQ-R vs the NEO-PI: a research note, replication and elaboration". Personality and Individual Differences 18 (6): 801–804. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(95)00010-4. 
  11. Eysenck, H. J. (1992). "A reply to Costa and McCrae. P or A and C: The role of theory". Personality and Individual Differences 13 (8): 867–868. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(92)90003-8. 
  12. 14.0 14.1 14.2 Matthews, G.; Deary, I. J.; Whiteman, M. C. (2003). Personality traits (2nd ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 
  13. Eysenck, Hans; Eysenck, S. B. G. (1991). The Eysenck Personality Questionnaire-Revised. Sevenoaks: Hodder & Stoughton. 
    • Costa, P. T.; McCrae, R. R. (1990). "Personality disorders and the five factor model of personality". Journal of Personality Disorders 4 (4): 362–371. doi:10.1521/pedi.1990.4.4.362. 
    • Lynam, D. R.; Caspi, A.; Moffitt, T. E.; Raine, A.; Loeber, R.; Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (2005). "Adolescent psychopathy and the Big Five: Results from two samples". Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 33 (4): 431–443. doi:10.1007/s10648-005-5724-0. PMID 16118990. 
  14. Eysenck, H. J.; Eysenck, M. W. (1985). Personality and individual differences: A natural science approach. New York: Plenum. 
  15. Eysenck, H. J. (1967). The biological basis of personality. Springfield, Ill.: Thomas. 
  16. Eysenck, H. J. (1994). "Creativity and personality: Word association, origence, and Psychoticism". Creativity Research Journal 7 (2): 209–216. doi:10.1080/10400419409534525. 
  17. 20.0 20.1 Jeronimus, B. F.; Riese, H.; Sanderman, R.; Ormel, J. (2014). "Mutual Reinforcement Between Neuroticism and Life Experiences: A Five-Wave, 16-Year Study to Test Reciprocal Causation". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 107 (4): 751–64. doi:10.1037/a0037009. PMID 25111305. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/264627671. 
  18. Eysenck, H. J. (1992). "The definition and measurement of psychoticism". Personality and Individual Differences 13 (7): 757–785. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(92)90050-y. 
  19. Eysenck, H. J. (1997). "Personality and experimental psychology: The unification of psychology and the possibility of a paradigm". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 73 (6): 1224–1237. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.73.6.1224. 
  20. DeYoung, C. G.; Peterson, J. B.; Higgins, D. M. (Aug 2005). "Sources of openness/intellect: cognitive and neuropsychological correlates of the fifth factor of personality". Journal of Personality 73 (4): 825–58. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2005.00330.x. PMID 15958136. 
    • MacLaren, V. V.; Best, L. A. (Aug 2009). "Female students' disordered eating and the big five personality facets". Eating Behaviors 10 (3): 192–5. doi:10.1016/j.eatbeh.2009.04.001. PMID 19665103. 
    • Heaven, P. C.; Mulligan, K.; Merrilees, R.; Woods, T.; Fairooz, Y. (Sep 2001). "Neuroticism and conscientiousness as predictors of emotional, external, and restrained eating behaviors". International Journal of Eating Disorders 30 (2): 161–6. doi:10.1002/eat.1068. PMID 11449449. 
    • Casper, R. C.; Hedeker, D.; McClough, J. F. (Sep 1992). "Personality dimensions in eating disorders and their relevance for subtyping". Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 31 (5): 830–40. doi:10.1097/00004583-199209000-00008. PMID 1400113. 
  21. Eysenck, Sybil B. G.; Eysenck, H. J. (1969). "Scores on Three Personality Variables as a Function of Age, Sex and Social Class". British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology (Wiley) 8 (1): 69–76. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8260.1969.tb00588.x. ISSN 0007-1293. 
    • Miller, J. L. (Jan 2006). "Neuroticism and introversion: a risky combination for disordered eating among a non-clinical sample of undergraduate women". Eating Behaviors 7 (1): 69–78. doi:10.1016/j.eatbeh.2005.07.003. PMID 16360625. 
    • Takano, A. (Sep 2007). "Relationship between neuroticism personality trait and serotonin transporter binding". Biological Psychiatry 62 (6): 588–92. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.11.007. PMID 17336939. 
    • Deckersbach, T. (2006). "Regional cerebral brain metabolism correlates of neuroticism and extraversion". Depression and Anxiety 23 (3): 133–8. doi:10.1002/da.20152. PMID 16470804. 
  22. Rosenberg, Morris (1968). Society and the Adolescent Self-Image. Princeton, N.J.: Princeton University Press. p. 18. ISBN 0691028052. https://archive.org/details/societyadolescen0000rose_m3e5. 
    • Blascovich, J.; Tomaka, J. (1991). "Measures of self-esteem". Measures of personality and social psychological attitudes. I. San Diego, Calif.: Academic Press. 
    • Button, E. J. (Jan 1997). "Self-esteem, eating problems and psychological wellbeing in a cohort of school age 15–16: question and interview". Int J Eat Disord 21 (1): 39–41. doi:10.1002/(sici)1098-108x(199701)21:1<39::aid-eat5>3.0.co;2-4. PMID 8986516. 
    • Strober, M. (1983). "Personality factors in anorexia nervosa". Pediatrician 12 (2–3): 134–8. PMID 6400211. 
    • Eiber, R. (2003). "Self-esteem: a comparison study between eating disorders and social phobia". Encephale 29 (1): 35–41. PMID 12640325. 
  23. Bulik, C. M. (1997). "Eating disorders and antecedent anxiety disorders: a controlled study". Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 96 (2): 101–107. doi:10.1111/j.1600-0447.1997.tb09913.x. PMID 9272193. 
  24. Benson, Etienne (November 2003). "The many faces of perfectionism". Monitor on Psychology 34 (10). 
    • Halmi, K. A. (Nov 2000). "Perfectionism in anorexia nervosa: variation by clinical subtype, obsessionality, and pathological eating behavior". American Journal of Psychiatry 157 (11): 1799–805. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.157.11.1799. PMID 11058477. 
    • Ruggiero, G. M. (Sep 2003). "Stress situation reveals an association between perfectionism and drive for thinness". International Journal of Eating Disorders 34 (2): 220–6. doi:10.1002/eat.10191. PMID 12898558. 
    • Hewitt, P. L. (2008). "The impact of perfectionistic self-presentation on the cognitive, affective, and physiological experience of a clinical interview". Psychiatry 71 (2): 93–122. doi:10.1521/psyc.2008.71.2.93. PMID 18573033. 
  25. Muller, Rene J. (2000). "When a patient has no story to tell: Alexithymia". Psychiatric Times 17 (7): 1–6. 
    • Frewen, P. A.; Pain, C.; Dozois, D. J.; Lanius, R. A. (Jul 2006). "Alexithymia in PTSD: psychometric and FMRI studies". Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1071 (1): 397–400. doi:10.1196/annals.1364.029. PMID 16891585. Bibcode2006NYASA1071..397F. 
    • Guilbaud, O.; Corcos, M.; Chambry, J.; Paterniti, S.; Loas, G.; Jeammet, P. (2000). "[Alexithymia and depression in eating disorders]". Encephale 26 (5): 1–6. PMID 11192799. 
    • Smith, G. J. (Aug 1997). "Alexithymia in patients with eating disorders: an investigation using a new projective technique". Perceptual and Motor Skills 85 (1): 247–56. doi:10.2466/pms.1997.85.1.247. PMID 9293583. 
    • Peskine, A.; Picq, C.; Pradat-Diehl, P. (Dec 2004). "Cerebral anoxia and disability". Brain Injury 18 (12): 1243–54. doi:10.1080/02699050410001719899. PMID 15666568. <
    • Ho, Aileen K.; Robbins, Anna O. G.; Barker, Roger A. (Mar 2006). "Huntington's disease patients have selective problems with insight". Movement Disorders 21 (3): 385–9. doi:10.1002/mds.20739. PMID 16211608. 
    • Tchanturia, K. (Sep 2001). "Perceptual illusions in eating disorders: rigid and fluctuating styles". Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry 32 (3): 107–15. doi:10.1016/s0005-7916(01)00025-8. PMID 11934124. 
    • Cserjési, R. (Jun 2009). "Affect, cognition, awareness and behavior in eating disorders. Comparison between obesity and anorexia nervosa". Orvosi Hetilap 150 (24): 1135–43. doi:10.1556/OH.2009.28590. PMID 19482720. 
    • Zubieta, J. K. (1995). "Obsessionality in eating-disorder patients: relationship to clinical presentation and two-year outcome". Journal of Psychiatric Research 29 (4): 333–42. doi:10.1016/0022-3956(95)00020-6. PMID 8847659. 
    • Salkovskis, P. M.; Forrester, E.; Richards, C. (1998). "Cognitive-behavioural approach to understanding obsessional thinking". British Journal of Psychiatry Supplement 173 (35): 53–63. doi:10.1192/S0007125000297900. PMID 9829027. 
    • Corcoran, K. M.; Woody, S. R. (Jan 2008). "Appraisals of obsessional thoughts in normal samples". Behaviour Research and Therapy 46 (1): 71–83. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2007.10.007. PMID 18093572.