Social:Music psychology

From HandWiki

Music psychology, or the psychology of music, may be regarded as a branch of both psychology and musicology. It aims to explain and understand musical behaviour and experience, including the processes through which music is perceived, created, responded to, and incorporated into everyday life.[1][2] Modern music psychology is primarily empirical; its knowledge tends to advance on the basis of interpretations of data collected by systematic observation of and interaction with human participants. Music psychology is a field of research with practical relevance for many areas, including music performance, composition, education, criticism, and therapy, as well as investigations of human attitude, skill, performance, intelligence, creativity, and social behavior.

Music psychology can shed light on non-psychological aspects of musicology and musical practice. For example, it contributes to music theory through investigations of the perception and computational modelling of musical structures such as melody, harmony, tonality, rhythm, meter, and form. Research in music history can benefit from systematic study of the history of musical syntax, or from psychological analyses of composers and compositions in relation to perceptual, affective, and social responses to their music.

History

Early history (pre-1850)

The study of sound and musical phenomena prior to the 19th century was focused primarily on the mathematical modelling of pitch and tone.[3] The earliest recorded experiments date from the 6th century BCE, most notably in the work of Pythagoras and his establishment of the simple string length ratios that formed the consonances of the octave. This view that sound and music could be understood from a purely physical standpoint was echoed by such theorists as Anaxagoras and Boethius. An important early dissenter was Aristoxenus, who foreshadowed modern music psychology in his view that music could only be understood through human perception and its relation to human memory. Despite his views, the majority of musical education through the Middle Ages and Renaissance remained rooted in the Pythagorean tradition, particularly through the quadrivium of astronomy, geometry, arithmetic, and music.[3]

Research by Vincenzo Galilei (father of Galileo) demonstrated that, when string length was held constant, varying its tension, thickness, or composition could alter perceived pitch. From this, he argued that simple ratios were not enough to account for musical phenomenon and that a perceptual approach was necessary. He also claimed that the differences between various tuning systems were not perceivable, thus the disputes were unnecessary. Study of topics including vibration, consonance, the harmonic series, and resonance were furthered through the scientific revolution, including work by Galileo, Kepler, Mersenne, and Descartes. This included further speculation concerning the nature of the sense organs and higher-order processes, particularly by Savart, Helmholtz, and Koenig.[3]

Rise of empirical study (1860–1960)

A brass, spherical Helmholtz resonator based on his original design, circa 1890–1900

The latter 19th century saw the development of modern music psychology alongside the emergence of a general empirical psychology, one which passed through similar stages of development. The first was structuralist psychology, led by Wilhelm Wundt, which sought to break down experience into its smallest definable parts. This expanded upon previous centuries of acoustic study, and included Helmholtz developing the resonator to isolate and understand pure and complex tones and their perception, the philosopher Carl Stumpf using church organs and his own musical experience to explore timbre and absolute pitch, and Wundt himself associating the experience of rhythm with kinesthetic tension and relaxation.[3]

As structuralism gave way to Gestalt psychology and behaviorism at the turn of the century, music psychology moved beyond the study of isolated tones and elements to the perception of their inter-relationships and human reactions to them, though work languished behind that of visual perception.[3] In Europe Géza Révész and Albert Wellek developed a more complex understanding of musical pitch, and in the US the focus shifted to that of music education and the training and development of musical skill. Carl Seashore led this work, producing his The Measurement of Musical Talents and The Psychology of Musical Talent. Seashore used bespoke equipment and standardized tests to measure how performance deviated from indicated markings and how musical aptitude differed between students.

In 1963 F. Chrysler was the first one to use the term "science of music" when he was working on his "year book for musical" knowledge. European musicology was found in Greek. They were focused on the philosophy, and the concepts of any relations with music. Greek's several theories rose later to Arab and the Christians theories. Although their theories survived, they were also corrupted along the way, in the Middle Ages of Europe.[4]

Modern (1960–present)

Music psychology in the second half of the 20th century has expanded to cover a wide array of theoretical and applied areas. From the 1960s the field grew along with cognitive science, including such research areas as music perception (particularly of pitch, rhythm, harmony, and melody), musical development and aptitude, music performance, and affective responses to music.[3]

This period has also seen the founding of music psychology-specific journals, societies, conferences, research groups, centers, and degrees, a trend that has brought research toward specific applications for music education, performance, and therapy.[5] While the techniques of cognitive psychology allowed for more objective examinations of musical behavior and experience, the theoretical and technological advancements of neuroscience have greatly shaped the direction of music psychology into the 21st century.[6]

While the majority of music psychology research has focused on music in a Western context, the field has expanded along with ethnomusicology to examine how the perception and practice of music differs between cultures.[7][8] It has also emerged into the public sphere. In recent years several bestselling popular science books have helped bring the field into public discussion, notably Daniel Levitin's This Is Your Brain On Music (2006) and The World in Six Songs (2008), Oliver Sacks' Musicophilia (2007), and Gary Marcus' Guitar Zero (2012). In addition, the controversial "Mozart effect" sparked lengthy debate among researchers, educators, politicians, and the public regarding the relationship between classical music listening, education, and intelligence.[9]

Research areas

Perception and cognition

Much work within music psychology seeks to understand the cognitive processes that support musical behaviors, including perception, comprehension, memory, attention, and performance. Originally arising in fields of psychoacoustics and sensation, cognitive theories of how people understand music more recently encompass neuroscience, cognitive science, music theory, music therapy, computer science, psychology, philosophy, and linguistics.[10][11]

Affective response

Music has been shown to consistently elicit emotional responses in its listeners, and this relationship between human affect and music has been studied in depth.[3] This includes isolating which specific features of a musical work or performance convey or elicit certain reactions, the nature of the reactions themselves, and how characteristics of the listener may determine which emotions are felt. The field draws upon and has significant implications for such areas as philosophy, musicology, and aesthetics, as well the acts of musical composition and performance. The implications for casual listeners are also great; research has shown that the pleasurable feelings associated with emotional music are the result of dopamine release in the striatum—the same anatomical areas that underpin the anticipatory and rewarding aspects of drug addiction.[12] According to research, listening to music has been found to affect the mood of an individual. The main factors in whether it will affect that individual positively or negatively are based on the musics tempo and style. In addition, listening to music also increases cognitive functions, creativity, and decreases feelings of fatigue. All of these factors lead to better workflow and a more optimal result in the activity done while listening to music. This leads to the conclusion that listening to music while performing an activity is an excellent way of increasing productivity and the overall experience.[13] It has been proposed that the ability to understand the emotional meaning of music might rely on the existence of a common neural system for processing the affective meaning of voices/vocalizations and musical sounds. [14][15]

Neuropsychology

A significant amount of research concerns brain-based mechanisms involved in the cognitive processes underlying music perception and performance. These behaviours include music listening, performing, composing, reading, writing, and ancillary activities. It also is increasingly concerned with the brain basis for musical aesthetics and musical emotion. Scientists working in this field may have training in cognitive neuroscience, neurology, neuroanatomy, psychology, music theory, computer science, and other allied fields, and use such techniques as functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS), magnetoencephalography (MEG), electroencephalography (EEG), and positron emission tomography (PET).

The cognitive process of performing music requires the interaction of neural mechanisms in both motor and auditory systems. Since every action expressed in a performance produces a sound that influences subsequent expression, this leads to impressive sensorimotor interplay.[16]

Processing pitch

The primary auditory cortex is one of the main areas associated with superior pitch resolution.

Perceived pitch typically depends on the fundamental frequency, though the dependence could be mediated solely by the presence of harmonics corresponding to that fundamental frequency. The perception of a pitch without the corresponding fundamental frequency in the physical stimulus is called the pitch of the missing fundamental.[17] Neurons lateral to A1 in marmoset monkeys were found to be sensitive specifically to the fundamental frequency of a complex tone,[18] suggesting that pitch constancy may be enabled by such a neural mechanism.

Pitch constancy refers to the ability to perceive pitch identity across changes in acoustical properties, such as loudness, temporal envelope, or timbre.[17] The importance of cortical regions lateral to A1 for pitch coding is also supported by studies of human cortical lesions and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) of the brain.[19][20][21] These data suggest a hierarchical system for pitch processing, with more abstract properties of sound stimulus processed further along the processing pathways.

Absolute pitch

Absolute pitch (AP) is defined as the ability to identify the pitch of a musical tone or to produce a musical tone at a given pitch without the use of an external reference pitch.[22] Researchers estimate the occurrence of AP to be 1 in 10,000 people.[23] The extent to which this ability is innate or learned is debated, with evidence for both a genetic basis and for a "critical period" in which the ability can be learned, especially in conjunction with early musical training.[24][25]

Processing rhythm

Behavioural studies demonstrate that rhythm and pitch can be perceived separately,[26] but that they also interact[27][28][29] in creating a musical perception. Studies of auditory rhythm discrimination and reproduction in patients with brain injury have linked these functions to the auditory regions of the temporal lobe, but have shown no consistent localization or lateralization.[30][31][32] Neuropsychological and neuroimaging studies have shown that the motor regions of the brain contribute to both perception and production of rhythms.[33]

Even in studies where subjects only listen to rhythms, the basal ganglia, cerebellum, dorsal premotor cortex (dPMC) and supplementary motor area (SMA) are often implicated.[34][35][16] The analysis of rhythm may depend on interactions between the auditory and motor systems.

Neural correlates of musical training

Although auditory–motor interactions can be observed in people without formal musical training, musicians are an excellent population to study because of their long-established and rich associations between auditory and motor systems. Musicians have been shown to have anatomical adaptations that correlate with their training.[17] Some neuroimaging studies have observed that musicians show lower levels of activity in motor regions than non-musicians during the performance of simple motor tasks, which may suggest a more efficient pattern of neural recruitment.[36][37][38][39] Other studies have shown that early musical training may positively affect word reading, by promoting the specialization of an extra right-sided "note visual area" to process spatially relevant visual information (i.e., pentagram, bars, etc.)[40] This neuroplastic effect might help prevent surface dyslexia. Music learning also involves the formation of novel audio visuomotor associations, which results in the ability to detect an incorrect association between sounds and the corresponding musical gestures[41][42], also allowing to learn how to play a musical instrument. [43]

Motor imagery

Previous neuroimaging studies have consistently reported activity in the SMA and premotor areas, as well as in auditory cortices, when non-musicians imagine hearing musical excerpts.[17] Recruitment of the SMA and premotor areas is also reported when musicians are asked to imagine performing.[39][44]

Psychoacoustics

Main page: Physics:Psychoacoustics

Psychoacoustics is the scientific study of sound perception. More specifically, it is the branch of science studying the psychological and physiological responses associated with sound (including speech and music). Topics of study include perception of the pitch, timbre, loudness and duration of musical sounds and the relevance of such studies for music cognition or the perceived structure of music; and auditory illusions and how humans localize sound, which can have relevance for musical composition and the design of venues for music performance. Psychoacoustics is a branch of psychophysics.

Cognitive musicology

Cognitive musicology is a branch of cognitive science concerned with computationally modeling musical knowledge with the goal of understanding both music and cognition.[45]

Cognitive musicology can be differentiated from the fields of music cognition and cognitive neuroscience of music by a difference in methodological emphasis. Cognitive musicology uses computer modeling to study music-related knowledge representation and has roots in artificial intelligence and cognitive science. The use of computer models provides an exacting, interactive medium in which to formulate and test theories.[27][28][46][47]

This interdisciplinary field investigates topics such as the parallels between language and music in the brain. Biologically inspired models of computation are often included in research, such as neural networks and evolutionary programs.[48] This field seeks to model how musical knowledge is represented, stored, perceived, performed, and generated. By using a well-structured computer environment, the systematic structures of these cognitive phenomena can be investigated.[49]

Evolutionary musicology

Main page: Philosophy:Evolutionary musicology

Evolutionary musicology concerns the "origin of music, the question of animal song, selection pressures underlying music evolution", and "music evolution and human evolution".[50] It seeks to understand music perception and activity in the context of evolutionary theory. Charles Darwin speculated that music may have held an adaptive advantage and functioned as a protolanguage,[51] a view which has spawned several competing theories of music evolution.[52][53][54] An alternate view sees music as a by-product of linguistic evolution; a type of "auditory cheesecake" that pleases the senses without providing any adaptive function.[55] This view has been directly countered by numerous music researchers.[56][57][58]

Cultural differences

An individual's culture or ethnicity plays a role in their music cognition, including their preferences, emotional reaction, and musical memory. Musical preferences are biased toward culturally familiar musical traditions beginning in infancy, and adults' classification of the emotion of a musical piece depends on both culturally specific and universal structural features.[59][60] Additionally, individuals' musical memory abilities are greater for culturally familiar music than for culturally unfamiliar music.[61][62]

Applied research areas

Many areas of music psychology research focus on the application of music in everyday life as well as the practices and experiences of the amateur and professional musician. Each topic may utilize knowledge and techniques derived from one or more of the areas described above. Such areas include:

Music in society

Including:

  • everyday music listening
  • musical rituals and gatherings (e.g. religious, festive, sporting, political, etc.)
  • the role of music in forming personal and group identities
  • the relation between music and dancing
  • social influences on musical preference (peers, family, experts, social background, etc.)

Musical preference

Consumers' choices in music have been studied as they relate to the Big Five personality traits: openness to experience, agreeableness, extraversion, neuroticism, and conscientiousness. In general, the plasticity traits (openness to experience and extraversion) affect music preference more than the stability traits (agreeableness, neuroticism, and conscientiousness).[63] Gender has been shown to influence preference, with men choosing music for primarily cognitive reasons and women for emotional reasons.[64] Relationships with music preference have also been found with mood[65] and nostalgic association.[66]

Background music

The study of background music focuses on the impact of music with non-musical tasks, including changes in behavior in the presence of different types, settings, or styles of music.[67] In laboratory settings, music can affect performance on cognitive tasks (memory, attention, and comprehension), both positively and negatively. Used extensively as an advertising aid, music may also affect marketing strategies, ad comprehension, and consumer choices. Background music can influence learning,[68][69] working memory and recall,[70][71] performance while working on tests,[72][73] and attention in cognitive monitoring tasks.[74][75] Background music can also be used as a way to relieve boredom, create positive moods, and maintain a private space.[76] Background music has been shown to put a restless mind at ease by presenting the listener with various melodies and tones.[76] It has been shown that listening to different types of music may modulate differently psychological mood and physiological responses associated with the induced emotions[77]. For example, listening to atonal music might result in reduced heart rate (fear bradycardia) and increased blood pressure (both diastolic and systolic), possibly reflecting an increase in alertness and attention, psychological tension, and anxiety[78].

Music in marketing

In both radio and television advertisements, music plays an integral role in content recall,[79][80][81] intentions to buy the product, and attitudes toward the advertisement and brand itself.[82][83][84] Music's effect on marketing has been studied in radio ads,[81][83][84] TV ads,[79][80][82] and physical retail settings.[85][86]

One of the most important aspects of an advertisement's music is the "musical fit", or the degree of congruity between cues in the ad and song content.[87] Advertisements and music can be congruous or incongruous for both lyrical and instrumental music. The timbre, tempo, lyrics, genre, mood, as well as any positive or negative associations elicited by certain music should fit the nature of the advertisement and product.[87]

Music and productivity

Several studies have recognized that listening to music while working affects the productivity of people performing complex cognitive tasks.[88] One study suggested that listening to one's preferred genre of music can enhance productivity in the workplace,[89] though other research has found that listening to music while working can be a source of distraction, with loudness and lyrical content possibly playing a role.[90] Other factors proposed to affect the relationship between music listening and productivity include musical structure, task complexity, and degree of control over the choice and use of music.[91]

Music education

A primary focus of music psychology research concerns how best to teach music and the effects this has on childhood development.

Including:

  • optimizing music education
  • development of musical behaviors and abilities throughout the lifespan
  • the specific skills and processes involved in learning a musical instrument or singing
  • activities and practices within a music school
  • individual versus group learning of a musical instrument
  • the effects of musical education on intelligence
  • optimizing practice

Musical aptitude

Musical aptitude refers to a person's innate ability to acquire skills and knowledge required for musical activity, and may influence the speed at which learning can take place and the level that may be achieved. Study in this area focuses on whether aptitude can be broken into subsets or represented as a single construct, whether aptitude can be measured prior to significant achievement, whether high aptitude can predict achievement, to what extent aptitude is inherited, and what implications questions of aptitude have on educational principles.[3]

It is an issue closely related to that of intelligence and IQ, and was pioneered by the work of Carl Seashore. While early tests of aptitude, such as Seashore's The Measurement of Musical Talent, sought to measure innate musical talent through discrimination tests of pitch, interval, rhythm, consonance, memory, etc., later research found these approaches to have little predictive power and to be influenced greatly by the test-taker's mood, motivation, confidence, fatigue, and boredom when taking the test.[3]

Music performance

Including:

  • the physiology of performance
  • music reading and sight-reading, including eye movement
  • performing from memory and music-related memory
  • acts of improvisation and composition
  • flow experiences
  • the interpersonal/social aspects of group performance
  • music performance quality evaluation by an audience or evaluator(s) (e.g. audition or competition), including the influence of musical and non-musical factors
  • audio engineering[92]

Music and health

Including:

Journals

Music psychology journals include:

  • Music Perception
  • Musicae Scientiae
  • Psychology of Music
  • Music and Science
  • Jahrbuch Musikpsychologie[94]

Music psychologists also publish in a wide range of mainstream musicology, computational musicology, music theory/analysis, psychology, music education, music therapy, music medicine, and systematic musicology journals. The latter include for example:

Societies

Centers of research and teaching

Australia :

  • Music, Sound and Performance Lab, Macquarie University[96]
  • Music, Mind and Wellbeing Initiative, Melbourne University[97]
  • Empirical Musicology Group, University of New South Wales[98]
  • ARC Centre of Excellence for the History of Emotion, University of Western Australia[99]
  • The MARCS Institute, University of Western Sydney[100]

Austria:

  • Centre for Systematic Musicology, University of Graz[101]
  • Cognitive Psychology Unit, University of Klagenfurt[102]

Belgium:

Canada :

  • Centre for Interdisciplinary Research in Music and Media and Technology, McGill University[104]
  • Music and Health Research Collaboratory, University of Toronto[105]
  • Music Cognition Lab, Queen's University[106]
  • Auditory Perception and Music Cognition Research and Training Laboratory, University of Prince Edward Island[107]
  • SMART Lab, Ryerson University[108]
  • The Music, Acoustics, Perception, and LEarning (MAPLE) Lab, McMaster University[109]
  • The Digital Music Lab (DML), McMaster University[110]
  • McMaster Institute for Music and the Mind, McMaster University[111]
  • BRAMS - International Laboratory for Brain, Music, and Sound Research, University of Montreal and McGill University[112]
  • Centre for Research on Brain, Language and Music, University of Montreal[113]
  • Music and Neuroscience Lab, University of Western Ontario[114]

Denmark :

Finland :

  • Finnish Centre of Excellence in Interdisciplinary Music Research, University of Jyväskylä[116]

France :

  • Auditory Cognition and Psychoacoustics team, Claude Bernard University Lyon 1[117]
  • University of Burgundy
  • IRCAM, Centre Pompidou[118]

Germany :

  • University of Halle-Wittenberg
  • Institute for Systematic Musicology, Universität Hamburg[119]
  • Institute of Music Physiology and Musicians' Medicine, Hochschule für Musik, Theater und Medien Hannover[120]
  • Hanover Music Lab, Hochschule für Musik, Theater und Medien Hannover[121]
  • University of Cologne
  • University of Oldenburg
  • Hochschule für Musik Würzburg
  • Technische Universität Chemnitz

Iceland:

Ireland:

  • University of Limerick

Italy:

Japan :

  • Kyushu University

Korea:

  • Seoul National University

Netherlands:

Norway :

  • Centre for Music and Health, Norwegian Academy of Music[125]

Poland :

  • Unit of Psychology of Music, Fryderyk Chopin University of Music[126]
  • Music Performance and Brain Lab, University of Finance and Management in Warsaw[127]

Singapore:

  • Music Cognition Group, Social and Cognitive Computing Department, Institute of High Performance Computing, A*STAR[128]

Spain :

  • Music Technology Group, Pompeu Fabra University[129]

Sweden:

United Kingdom :

United States :



See also

References

  1. Tan, Siu-Lan; Pfordresher, Peter; Harré, Rom (2010). Psychology of Music: From Sound to Significance. New York: Psychology Press. pp. 2. ISBN 978-1-84169-868-7. 
  2. Thompson, William Forde (2009). Music, Thought, and Feeling: Understanding the Psychology of Music, 2nd Edition. New York: Oxford University Press. pp. 320. ISBN 978-0195377071. 
  3. 3.0 3.1 3.2 3.3 3.4 3.5 3.6 3.7 3.8 Deutsch, Diana; Gabrielsson, Alf; Sloboda, John; Cross, Ian; Drake, Carolyn; Parncutt, Richard; McAdams, Stephen; Clarke, Eric F. et al. (2001). "Psychology of music". Grove Music Online. doi:10.1093/gmo/9781561592630.article.42574. ISBN 978-1-56159-263-0. 
  4. "Musicology" (in en). https://www.britannica.com/art/musicology. 
  5. Ockelford, Adam (2009). "Beyond music psychology". in Hallam, Susan; Cross, Ian; Thaut, Michael. The Oxford Handbook of Music Psychology. Oxford: Oxford University Press. p. 539. ISBN 978-0-19-929845-7. 
  6. Thaut, Micahel (2009). "History and research". in Hallam, Susan; Cross, Ian; Thaut, Michael. The Oxford Handbook of Music Psychology. Oxford: Oxford University Press. p. 556. ISBN 978-0-19-929845-7. 
  7. Thaut, Micahel (2009). "History and research". in Hallam, Susan; Cross, Ian; Thaut, Michael. The Oxford Handbook of Music Psychology. Oxford: Oxford University Press. p. 559. ISBN 978-0-19-929845-7. 
  8. Thompson, William Forde; Balkwill, Laura-Lee (2010). "Cross-cultural similarities and differences". in Juslin, Patrik; Sloboda, John. Handbook of Music and Emotion: Theory, Research, Applications (ch. 27). Oxford: Oxford University Press. pp. 755–788. ISBN 9780199604968. https://archive.org/details/handbookofmusice0000unse/page/755. 
  9. Abbott, Alison. "Mozart doesn't make you clever". Nature.com. http://www.nature.com/news/2007/070413/full/news070409-13.html. 
  10. Deutsch, Diana (Editor) (2013). The Psychology of Music, 3rd Edition. San Diego, California: Academic Press. ISBN 978-0123814609. 
  11. Thompson, William Forde (Editor) (2014). Encyclopedia of Music in the Social and Behavioral Sciences. New York, New York: Sage Press. ISBN 9781452283036. 
  12. Salimpoor, Valorie N; Benovoy, Mitchel; Larcher, Kevin; Dagher, Alain; Zatorre, Robert J (February 2011). "Anatomically distinct dopamine release during anticipation and experience of peak emotion to music". Nature Neuroscience 14 (2): 257–262. doi:10.1038/nn.2726. PMID 21217764. 
  13. Campion, Maxine; Levita, Liat (4 March 2014). "Enhancing positive affect and divergent thinking abilities: Play some music and dance". The Journal of Positive Psychology 9 (2): 137–145. doi:10.1080/17439760.2013.848376. 
  14. Proverbio, Alice Mado; Benedetto, Francesco De; Guazzone, Martina (2020). "Shared neural mechanisms for processing emotions in music and vocalizations" (in en). European Journal of Neuroscience 51 (9): 1987–2007. doi:10.1111/ejn.14650. ISSN 1460-9568. https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.1111/ejn.14650. 
  15. Proverbio, Alice Mado; Santoni, Sacha; Adorni, Roberta (2020-03-27). "ERP Markers of Valence Coding in Emotional Speech Processing" (in English). iScience 23 (3). doi:10.1016/j.isci.2020.100933. ISSN 2589-0042. PMID 32151976. https://www.cell.com/iscience/abstract/S2589-0042(20)30117-6. 
  16. 16.0 16.1 Zatorre, Robert J.; Chen, Joyce L.; Penhune, Virginia B. (2007). "When the Brain Plays Music: Auditory–motor Interactions in Music Perception and Production". Nature Reviews Neuroscience 8 (7): 547–58. doi:10.1038/nrn2152. PMID 17585307. 
  17. 17.0 17.1 17.2 17.3 Zatorre, R. J.; Halpern, A. R. (2005). "Mental concerts: musical imagery and auditory cortex". Neuron 47 (1): 9–12. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2005.06.013. PMID 15996544. https://digitalcommons.bucknell.edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=2547&context=fac_journ. 
  18. Bendor, D.; Wang, X. (2005). "The neuronal representation of pitch in primate auditory cortex". Nature 436 (7054): 1161–1165. doi:10.1038/nature03867. PMID 16121182. Bibcode2005Natur.436.1161B. 
  19. Zatorre1, R. J. (1988). "Pitch perception of complex tones and human temporal-lobe function". J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 84 (2): 566–572. doi:10.1121/1.396834. PMID 3170948. Bibcode1988ASAJ...84..566Z. 
  20. Johnsrude, I. S.; Penhune, V. B.; Zatorre, R. J. (2000). "Functional specificity in the right human auditory cortex for perceiving pitch direction". Brain 123: 155–163. doi:10.1093/brain/123.1.155. PMID 10611129. 
  21. Penagos, H.; Melcher, J. R.; Oxenham, A. J. (2004). "A neural representation of pitch salience in nonprimary human auditory cortex revealed with functional magnetic resonance imaging". J. Neurosci. 24 (30): 6810–6815. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.0383-04.2004. PMID 15282286. 
  22. Takeuchi, Annie H.; Hulse, Stewart H. (1993). "Absolute pitch". Psychological Bulletin 113 (2): 345–61. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.113.2.345. PMID 8451339. 
  23. Sacks, O. (5 May 1995). "Musical ability". Science 268 (5211): 621–622. doi:10.1126/science.7732360. PMID 7732360. 
  24. Theusch, Elizabeth; Basu, Analabha; Gitschier, Jane (July 2009). "Genome-wide Study of Families with Absolute Pitch Reveals Linkage to 8q24.21 and Locus Heterogeneity". The American Journal of Human Genetics 85 (1): 112–119. doi:10.1016/j.ajhg.2009.06.010. PMID 19576568. 
  25. Snyder, Bob (2009). "Memory for music". in Hallam, Susan; Cross, Ian; Thaut, Michael. The Oxford Handbook of Music Psychology. Oxford: Oxford University Press. p. 111. ISBN 978-0-19-929845-7. 
  26. Krumhansl, C. L. (2000). "Rhythm and pitch in music cognition". Psychol. Bull. 126 (1): 159–179. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.126.1.159. PMID 10668354. 
  27. 27.0 27.1 Tanguiane (Tangian), Andranick (1993). Artificial Perception and Music Recognition. Lecture Notes in Artificial Intelligence. 746. Berlin-Heidelberg: Springer. ISBN 978-3-540-57394-4. [page needed]
  28. 28.0 28.1 Tanguiane (Tangian), Andranick (1994). "Principle of correlativity of perception and its applications to music recognition". Music Perception 11 (4): 465–502. doi:10.2307/40285634. 
  29. Jones, Mari Riess; Moynihan, Heather; MacKenzie, Noah; Puente, Jennifer (July 2002). "Temporal Aspects of Stimulus-Driven Attending in Dynamic Arrays". Psychological Science 13 (4): 313–319. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00458. PMID 12137133. 
  30. Penhune, V. B.; Zatorre, R. J.; Feindel, W. H. (1999). "The role of auditory cortex in retention of rhythmic patterns in patients with temporal-lobe removals including Heschl's gyrus". Neuropsychologia 37 (3): 315–331. doi:10.1016/s0028-3932(98)00075-x. PMID 10199645. 
  31. Peretz, I. (1990). "Processing of local & global musical information by unilateral brain-damaged patients". Brain 113 (4): 1185–1205. doi:10.1093/brain/113.4.1185. PMID 2397389. 
  32. Kester, D.Brian; Saykin, Andrew J.; Sperling, Michael R.; O'Connor, Michael J.; Robinson, Lindsey J.; Gur, Ruben C. (January 1991). "Acute effect of anterior temporal lobectomy on musical processing". Neuropsychologia 29 (7): 703–708. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(91)90104-g. PMID 1944872. 
  33. Janata, P.; Grafton, S. T. (2003). "Swinging in the brain: shared neural substrates for behaviors related to sequencing and music". Nature Neuroscience 6 (7): 682–687. doi:10.1038/nn1081. PMID 12830159. 
  34. Sakai, Katsuyuki; Hikosaka, Okihide; Miyauchi, Satoru; Takino, Ryousuke; Tamada, Tomoe; Iwata, Nobue Kobayashi; Nielsen, Mathew (15 November 1999). "Neural Representation of a Rhythm Depends on Its Interval Ratio". The Journal of Neuroscience 19 (22): 10074–10081. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-22-10074.1999. PMID 10559415. 
  35. Grahn, J. A.; Brett, M. (2007). "Rhythm and beat perception in motor areas of the brain". J. Cogn. Neurosci. 19 (5): 893–906. doi:10.1162/jocn.2007.19.5.893. PMID 17488212. 
  36. Hund-Georgiadis, M.; von Cramon, D. Y. (1999). "Motorlearning-related changes in piano players and nonmusicians revealed by functional magnetic-resonance signals". Exp Brain Res 125 (4): 417–425. doi:10.1007/s002210050698. PMID 10323287. 
  37. Jancke, L.; Shah, N. J.; Peters, M. (2000). "Cortical activations in primary and secondary motor areas for complex bimanual movements in professional pianists". Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 10 (1–2): 177–183. doi:10.1016/s0926-6410(00)00028-8. PMID 10978706. 
  38. Koeneke, Susan; Lutz, Kai; Wüstenberg, Torsten; Jäncke, Lutz (June 2004). "Long-term training affects cerebellar processing in skilled keyboard players". NeuroReport 15 (8): 1279–1282. doi:10.1097/01.wnr.0000127463.10147.e7. PMID 15167549. 
  39. 39.0 39.1 Meister, I.G; Krings, T; Foltys, H; Boroojerdi, B; Müller, M; Töpper, R; Thron, A (May 2004). "Playing piano in the mind—an fMRI study on music imagery and performance in pianists". Cognitive Brain Research 19 (3): 219–228. doi:10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2003.12.005. PMID 15062860. 
  40. Proverbio, Alice Mado; Manfredi, Mirella; Zani, Alberto; Adorni, Roberta (2013-02-01). "Musical expertise affects neural bases of letter recognition" (in en). Neuropsychologia 51 (3): 538–549. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.12.001. ISSN 0028-3932. https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S002839321200509X. 
  41. Proverbio, Alice M.; Attardo, Lapo; Cozzi, Matteo; Zani, Alberto (2015). "The effect of musical practice on gesture/sound pairing". Frontiers in Psychology 6: 376. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00376. ISSN 1664-1078. https://www.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00376. 
  42. Proverbio, Alice Mado; Cozzi, Matteo; Orlandi, Andrea; Carminati, Manuel (2017-03-27). "Error-related negativity in the skilled brain of pianists reveals motor simulation" (in en). Neuroscience 346: 309–319. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2017.01.030. ISSN 0306-4522. https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S030645221730043X. 
  43. Mado Proverbio, Alice; Calbi, Marta; Manfredi, Mirella; Zani, Alberto (2014-07-29). "Audio-visuomotor processing in the Musician's brain: an ERP study on professional violinists and clarinetists". Scientific Reports 4: 5866. doi:10.1038/srep05866. ISSN 2045-2322. PMID 25070060. PMC 5376193. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5376193/. 
  44. Langheim, F; Callicott, JH; Mattay, VS; Duyn, JH; Weinberger, DR (August 2002). "Cortical Systems Associated with Covert Music Rehearsal". NeuroImage 16 (4): 901–908. doi:10.1006/nimg.2002.1144. PMID 12202078. 
  45. Laske, Otto (1999). Navigating New Musical Horizons (Contributions to the Study of Music and Dance). Westport: Greenwood Press. ISBN 978-0-313-30632-7. https://archive.org/details/ottolaskenavigat0000unse. 
  46. Tanguiane, Andranik (September 1995). "Towards axiomatization of music perception 1 ast;". Journal of New Music Research 24 (3): 247–281. doi:10.1080/09298219508570685. 
  47. Laske, O. (1999). AI and music: A cornerstone of cognitive musicology. In M. Balaban, K. Ebcioglu, & O. Laske (Eds.), Understanding music with ai: Perspectives on music cognition. Cambridge: The MIT Press.[page needed]
  48. Graci, Craig (December 2009). "A Brief Tour of the Learning Sciences via a Cognitive Tool for Investigating Melodic Phenomena". Journal of Educational Technology Systems 38 (2): 181–211. doi:10.2190/ET.38.2.i. 
  49. Hamman, M., 1999. "Structure as Performance: Cognitive Musicology and the Objectification of Procedure," in Otto Laske: Navigating New Musical Horizons, ed. J. Tabor. New York: Greenwood Press.[page needed]
  50. Wallin, Nils L./Björn Merker/Steven Brown (1999): "An Introduction to Evolutionary Musicology." In: Wallin, Nils L./Björn Merker/Steven Brown (Eds., 1999): The Origins of Music, pp. 5–6. ISBN:0-262-23206-5.
  51. "The Descent of Man, and Selection in Relation to Sex". 1871. http://www.gutenberg.org/dirs/etext00/dscmn10.txt. [|permanent dead link|dead link}}] Chapter III; Language
  52. Nils L. Wallin, Björn Merker, and Steven Brown (Editors) (2000). The Origins of Music. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. ISBN 0-262-23206-5. 
  53. Steven Mithen, The Singing Neanderthals: the Origins of Music, Language, Mind and Body, Harvard University Press, 2006.[page needed]
  54. Hagen, Edward H.; Hammerstein, Peter (September 2009). "Did Neanderthals and other early humans sing? Seeking the biological roots of music in the territorial advertisements of primates, lions, hyenas, and wolves". Musicae Scientiae 13 (2_suppl): 291–320. doi:10.1177/1029864909013002131. 
  55. Pinker, Steven (1997). How the Mind Works. New York: W. W. Norton. p. 534. ISBN 978-0-393-04535-2. 
  56. Perlovsky, Leonid (July 2012). "Cognitive function, origin, and evolution of musical emotions". Musicae Scientiae 16 (2): 185–199. doi:10.1177/1029864912448327. 
  57. Abbott, Alison (2002). "Neurobiology: Music, maestro, please!". Nature 416 (6876): 12–14. doi:10.1038/416012a. PMID 11882864. Bibcode2002Natur.416...12A. 
  58. Honing, Henkjan; Ploeger, Annemie (October 2012). "Cognition and the Evolution of Music: Pitfalls and Prospects". Topics in Cognitive Science 4 (4): 513–524. doi:10.1111/j.1756-8765.2012.01210.x. PMID 22760967. 
  59. Soley, Gaye; Hannon, Erin E. (2010). "Infants prefer the musical meter of their own culture: A cross-cultural comparison". Developmental Psychology 46 (1): 286–292. doi:10.1037/a0017555. PMID 20053025. 
  60. Balkwill, L.; Thompson, W. F.; Matsunaga, R. (2004). "Recognition of emotion in Japanese, Western, and Hindustani music by Japanese listeners". Japanese Psychological Research 46 (4): 337–349. doi:10.1111/j.1468-5584.2004.00265.x. 
  61. Demorest, S. M.; Morrison, S. J.; Beken, M. N.; Jungbluth, D. (2008). "Lost in translation: An enculturation effect in music memory performance". Music Perception 25 (3): 213–223. doi:10.1525/mp.2008.25.3.213. 
  62. Groussard, M.; Rauchs, G.; Landeau, B.; Viader, F.; Desgranges, B.; Eustache, F.; Platel, H. (December 2010). "The neural substrates of musical memory revealed by fMRI and two semantic tasks". NeuroImage 53 (4): 1301–1309. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.07.013. PMID 20627131. https://www.hal.inserm.fr/inserm-00538633/file/Manuscript_Groussard_Neuroimage2010.pdf. 
  63. Miranda, Dave; Morizot, Julien; Gaudreau, Patrick (January 2010). "Personality Metatraits and Music Preferences in Adolescence: A Pilot Study". International Journal of Adolescence and Youth 15 (4): 289–301. doi:10.1080/02673843.2010.9748036. 
  64. Chamorro-Premuzic, Tomas; Swami, Viren; Cermakova, Blanka (May 2012). "Individual differences in music consumption are predicted by uses of music and age rather than emotional intelligence, neuroticism, extraversion or openness". Psychology of Music 40 (3): 285–300. doi:10.1177/0305735610381591. 
  65. Vuoskoski, Jonna K.; Eerola, Tuomas (July 2011). "Measuring music-induced emotion: A comparison of emotion models, personality biases, and intensity of experiences". Musicae Scientiae 15 (2): 159–173. doi:10.1177/1029864911403367. 
  66. Barrett, Frederick S.; Grimm, Kevin J.; Robins, Richard W.; Wildschut, Tim; Sedikides, Constantine; Janata, Petr (2010). "Music-evoked nostalgia: Affect, memory, and personality". Emotion 10 (3): 390–403. doi:10.1037/a0019006. PMID 20515227. 
  67. Kämpfe, Juliane; Sedlmeier, Peter; Renkewitz, Frank (October 2011). "The impact of background music on adult listeners: A meta-analysis". Psychology of Music 39 (4): 424–448. doi:10.1177/0305735610376261. 
  68. de Groot, Annette M. B. (September 2006). "Effects of Stimulus Characteristics and Background Music on Foreign Language Vocabulary Learning and Forgetting: Language Learning". Language Learning 56 (3): 463–506. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9922.2006.00374.x. 
  69. Aheadi, Afshin; Dixon, Peter; Glover, Scott (January 2010). "A limiting feature of the Mozart effect: listening enhances mental rotation abilities in non-musicians but not musicians". Psychology of Music 38 (1): 107–117. doi:10.1177/0305735609336057. 
  70. Alley, Thomas R.; Greene, Marcie E. (December 2008). "The Relative and Perceived Impact of Irrelevant Speech, Vocal Music and Non-vocal Music on Working Memory". Current Psychology 27 (4): 277–289. doi:10.1007/s12144-008-9040-z. 
  71. Cassidy, Gianna; MacDonald, Raymond A.R. (July 2007). "The effect of background music and background noise on the task performance of introverts and extraverts". Psychology of Music 35 (3): 517–537. doi:10.1177/0305735607076444. 
  72. Patston, Lucy L. M.; Tippett, Lynette J. (1 December 2011). "The Effect of Background Music on Cognitive Performance in Musicians and Nonmusicians". Music Perception 29 (2): 173–183. doi:10.1525/mp.2011.29.2.173. 
  73. Avila, Christina; Furnham, Adrian; McClelland, Alastair (January 2012). "The influence of distracting familiar vocal music on cognitive performance of introverts and extraverts". Psychology of Music 40 (1): 84–93. doi:10.1177/0305735611422672. 
  74. Olivers, Christian N.L.; Nieuwenhuis, Sander (April 2005). "The Beneficial Effect of Concurrent Task-Irrelevant Mental Activity on Temporal Attention". Psychological Science 16 (4): 265–269. doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01526.x. PMID 15828972. https://research.vu.nl/ws/files/2043913/178424.pdf. 
  75. Beanland, Vanessa; Allen, Rosemary A.; Pammer, Kristen (December 2011). "Attending to music decreases inattentional blindness". Consciousness and Cognition 20 (4): 1282–1292. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2011.04.009. PMID 21555226. 
  76. 76.0 76.1 Schäfer, Thomas; Sedlmeier, Peter; Städtler, Christine; Huron, David (2013). "The psychological functions of music listening". Frontiers in Psychology 4: 511. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00511. PMID 23964257. 
  77. Proverbio, A. M.; De Benedetto, F. (2018-02-01). "Auditory enhancement of visual memory encoding is driven by emotional content of the auditory material and mediated by superior frontal cortex" (in en). Biological Psychology 132: 164–175. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2017.12.003. ISSN 0301-0511. https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0301051117303447. 
  78. Proverbio, Alice M.; Manfrin, Luigi; Arcari, Laura A.; De Benedetto, Francesco; Gazzola, Martina; Guardamagna, Matteo; Lozano Nasi, Valentina; Zani, Alberto (2015). "Non-expert listeners show decreased heart rate and increased blood pressure (fear bradycardia) in response to atonal music". Frontiers in Psychology 6: 1646. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01646. ISSN 1664-1078. https://www.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01646. 
  79. 79.0 79.1 Hahn, Minhi; Hwang, Insuk (1999). "Effects of tempo and familiarity of background music on message processing in TV advertising: A resource-matching perspective". Psychology & Marketing 16 (8): 659–675. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1520-6793(199912)16:8<659::AID-MAR3>3.0.CO;2-S. 
  80. 80.0 80.1 Park, C. Whan; Young, S. Mark (February 1986). "Consumer Response to Television Commercials: The Impact of Involvement and Background Music on Brand Attitude Formation". Journal of Marketing Research 23 (1): 11. doi:10.2307/3151772. 
  81. 81.0 81.1 Oakes, Steve; North, Adrian C. (May 2006). "The impact of background musical tempo and timbre congruity upon ad content recall and affective response". Applied Cognitive Psychology 20 (4): 505–520. doi:10.1002/acp.1199. 
  82. 82.0 82.1 Lalwani, Ashok K.; Lwin, May O.; Ling, Pee Beng (14 April 2009). "Does Audiovisual Congruency in Advertisements Increase Persuasion? The Role of Cultural Music and Products". Journal of Global Marketing 22 (2): 139–153. doi:10.1080/08911760902765973. 
  83. 83.0 83.1 Zander, Mark F. (October 2006). "Musical influences in advertising: how music modifies first impressions of product endorsers and brands". Psychology of Music 34 (4): 465–480. doi:10.1177/0305735606067158. 
  84. 84.0 84.1 Lavack, Anne M.; Thakor, Mrugank V.; Bottausci, Ingrid (January 2008). "Music-brand congruency in highand low-cognition radio advertising". International Journal of Advertising 27 (4): 549–568. doi:10.2501/S0265048708080141. 
  85. Eroglu, Sevgin A.; Machleit, Karen A.; Chebat, Jean-Charles (July 2005). "The interaction of retail density and music tempo: Effects on shopper responses". Psychology and Marketing 22 (7): 577–589. doi:10.1002/mar.20074. 
  86. Chebat, Jean-Charles; Chebat, Claire Gélinas; Vaillant, Dominique (November 2001). "Environmental background music and in-store selling". Journal of Business Research 54 (2): 115–123. doi:10.1016/S0148-2963(99)00089-2. 
  87. 87.0 87.1 OAKES, STEVE (1 January 2007). "Evaluating Empirical Research into Music in Advertising: A Congruity Perspective". Journal of Advertising Research 47 (1): 38. doi:10.2501/S0021849907070055. 
  88. "Is Background Music a Boost or a Bummer?" (in en). https://www.psychologytoday.com/us/blog/conquering-cyber-overload/201305/is-background-music-boost-or-bummer. 
  89. Lesiuk, Teresa (April 2005). "The effect of music listening on work performance". Psychology of Music 33 (2): 173–191. doi:10.1177/0305735605050650. 
  90. "Music at work: distracting or beneficial? - Anneli B. Haake PhD" (in en-US). http://musicatwork.net/music-at-work-distracting-or-beneficial/. 
  91. "Research Shows Listening to Music Increases Productivity (and Some Types of Music Are Super Effective)". Inc.com. 2017-09-19. https://www.inc.com/melissa-chu/research-shows-listening-to-music-increases-produc.html. 
  92. Shelvock, Matt (16 January 2017). "Gestalt Theory and Mixing Audio". in Hepworth-Sawyer, R.; Hodgson, J.; Toulson, R. et al.. Innovation in Music II. Lulu.com. pp. 75–87. ISBN 978-1-911108-04-7. https://books.google.com/books?id=1OLvDQAAQBAJ&pg=PA75. 
  93. Musgrave, George; Gross, Sally Anne (2020-09-29) (in en). Can Music Make You Sick?. University of Westminster Press. doi:10.16997/book43. ISBN 978-1-912656-62-2. https://uwestminsterpress.co.uk/site/books/m/10.16997/book43/. 
  94. "Dt. Gesellsch. f. Musikpsychologie" (in de). http://www.music-psychology.de/. 
  95. "Journal of Mathematics and Music". http://www.tandfonline.com/toc/tmam20/current. 
  96. "Macquarie University; Music, Sound and Performance Lab". 2002-05-22. http://www.psy.mq.edu.au/me2/. 
  97. "Melbourne University; Music, Music, Mind and Wellbeing Initiative". http://cmmw.unimelb.edu.au. 
  98. "UNSW; Empirical Musicology Group". https://sam.arts.unsw.edu.au/research/our-research/empirical-musicology/. 
  99. "University of Western Australia; ARC Centre of Excellence for the History of Emotion". http://www.historyofemotions.org.au/research/research-programs/performance.aspx. 
  100. "University of Western Sydney; The MARCS Institute". http://marcs.uws.edu.au/research. 
  101. "University of Graz; Centre for Systematic Musicology". http://systematische-musikwissenschaft.uni-graz.at/en/zentrum/. 
  102. "University of Klagenfurt; Cognitive Psychology Unit". http://cognition.aau.at/index.php?page=home&lang=english. 
  103. "Ghent University; Institute for Psychoacoustics and Electronic Music". http://www.ipem.ugent.be. 
  104. "McGill University; CIRMMT". http://www.cirmmt.org/home. 
  105. "University of Toronto; MaHRC". http://www.music.utoronto.ca/about/MaHRChome.htm. 
  106. "Queens University; Music Cognition Lab". http://www.queensu.ca/psychology/mcl.html. 
  107. "University of PEI; Auditory Perception and Music Cognition Research and Training Laboratory". http://www.upei.ca/%7Emusicog/. 
  108. "Ryerson University; SMART Lab". http://www.ryerson.ca/smart/. 
  109. "McMaster University; MAPLE Lab". http://maplelab.net. 
  110. "McMaster University; Digital Music Lab". https://digitalmusiclab.org/. 
  111. "McMaster University; MIMM". http://mimm.mcmaster.ca. 
  112. "BRAMS - International Laboratory for Brain, Music, and Sound Research". http://www.brams.org/en/. 
  113. "University of Montreal; Centre for Research on Brain, Language and Music". http://www.crblm.ca. 
  114. "University of Western Ontario; Music and Neuroscience Lab". http://www.jessicagrahn.com. 
  115. "Aarhus University; Center for Music in the brain". http://musicinthebrain.au.dk/. 
  116. "University of Jyväskylä, Finnish Centre of Excellence in Interdisciplinary Music Research". https://www.jyu.fi/hum/laitokset/musiikki/en/research/coe. 
  117. "Claude Bernard University Lyon 1; CAP". http://www-crnl.univ-lyon1.fr/spip.php?article89&lang=fr. 
  118. "Centre Pompidou; IRCAM; Research". http://www.ircam.fr/recherche.html?L=1. 
  119. "Institute for Systematic Musicology, Universität Hamburg". https://www.fbkultur.uni-hamburg.de/en/sm.html. 
  120. "HMTMH; Institute of Music Physiology and Musicians' Medicine". http://www.immm.hmtm-hannover.de/en/. 
  121. "HMTMH; Hanover Music Lab". http://www.hml.hmtm-hannover.de/en/. 
  122. "University of Iceland, Research Units". http://english.hi.is/school_of_education/research_units. 
  123. "University of Milano-Bicocca;". https://bicoccaerplab.wordpress.com/. 
  124. "University of Amsterdam; Music Cognition Group". http://www.hum.uva.nl/mmm/. 
  125. "Norwegian Academy of Music; Centre for Music and Health". http://nmh.no/en/research_and_artistic_development/Centre_for_Music_and_Health. 
  126. "FC University of Music; Unit of Psychology of Music". http://www.chopin.edu.pl/en/departments-of-the-university/unit-of-psychology-of-music/. 
  127. "University of Finance and Management in Warsaw; Music Performance and Brain Lab". http://www.mpblab.vizja.pl. 
  128. "Institute of High Performance Computing, A*STAR; Music Cognition Group". http://www.a-star.edu.sg/ihpc/Research/Social-Cognitive-Computing-SCC/Music-Cognition/. 
  129. "Pompeu Fabra University; Music Technology Group". http://mtg.upf.edu. 
  130. "Royal Institute of Technology, Speech, Music and Hearing". http://www.kth.se/en/csc/forskning/tmh. 
  131. "Uppsala University; Music Psychology Group". http://www.psyk.uu.se/research/researchgroups/musicpsychology/. 
  132. "Cambridge University; Centre for Music and Science". http://cms.mus.cam.ac.uk. 
  133. "University of Edinburgh; Music and the Human Sciences". http://www.eca.ed.ac.uk/reid-school-of-music/research/themes. 
  134. "Keele University; Centre for Psychological Research". http://www.keele.ac.uk/risocsci/researchcentres/psychologicalresearch/. 
  135. "Durham University; Music and Science Lab". https://musicscience.net. 
  136. "University of Leeds; ICSRiM". http://www.leeds.ac.uk/icsrim/. 
  137. "University of Leicester; Social and Applied Psychology Group". http://www2.le.ac.uk/Members/bc45/experimentalarea/psychnet/research-groups/social-and-applied-psychology/social-and-applied-psychology/. 
  138. "Goldsmiths; Music, Mind and Brain". http://www.gold.ac.uk/music-mind-brain/. 
  139. "UCL Institute of Education; International Music Education Research Centre". http://www.imerc.org/. 
  140. "Queen Mary University of London; Music Cognition Lab". http://webprojects.eecs.qmul.ac.uk/marcusp/lab/. 
  141. "University of Oxford; Psychology of Music". http://www.music.ox.ac.uk/research/disciplines/psychology-of-music//. 
  142. "University of Roehampton; Applied Music Research Centre". http://www.roehampton.ac.uk/Research-Centres/Applied-Music-Research-Centre/. 
  143. "Royal College of Music; Centre for Performance Science". http://www.rcm.ac.uk/cps. 
  144. "Royal Northern College of Music; Centre for Music Performance Research". http://www.rncm.ac.uk/research/performance-research/. 
  145. "Sheffield University; Department of Music, Psychology of Music Research". http://www.sheffield.ac.uk/music/research/psychology. 
  146. "Music and Neuroimaging Laboratory". http://www.musicianbrain.com/#index. 
  147. "University at Buffalo; Auditory Perception & Action Lab". https://pfordresher.wordpress.com. 
  148. "UCD; Janata Lab". http://atonal.ucdavis.edu. 
  149. "UCLA; Roger Kendall Bio". http://www.schoolofmusic.ucla.edu/roger-kendall-bio. 
  150. "UCSD; Diana Deutsch profile". http://www.psychology.ucsd.edu/people/profiles/ddeutsch.html. 
  151. "UCSB Music Cognition Lab". https://ucsbmusiccognitionlab.weebly.com/. 
  152. Foran, Sheila (2014-02-03). "Theoretical Neuroscientist Ed Large Joins UConn Faculty". UConn Today. http://today.uconn.edu/blog/2014/02/theoretical-neuroscientist-ed-large-joins-uconn-faculty/. 
  153. "Cornell University; The Music Cognition Laboratory". http://music.psych.cornell.edu/index.html. 
  154. "University of Rochester; Music Cognition at Eastman School of Music". http://theory.esm.rochester.edu/music-cognition/. 
  155. "Florida State University; Center for Music Research". http://www.music.fsu.edu/Music-Research-Centers/Center-for-Music-Research. 
  156. "Louisiana State University; Music Cognition and Computation Lab". https://musiccog.lsu.edu/. 
  157. "University of Maryland; Language and Music Cognition Lab". http://lmcl.umd.edu. 
  158. "UNLV; Auditory Cognition and Development Lab". https://faculty.unlv.edu/ehannon/Home.html. 
  159. "Northwestern University; Auditory Neuroscience Laboratory". http://www.soc.northwestern.edu/brainvolts/. 
  160. "Northwestern University; Music Theory and Cognition Program". http://www.music.northwestern.edu/academics/areas-of-study/music-theory-and-cognition/index.html. 
  161. "Princeton University; Music Cognition Lab". http://www.elizabethmargulis.com/about-music-cognition-lab. 
  162. "Ohio State University; Cognitive and Systematic Musicology Laboratory". http://musiccog.ohio-state.edu/home/index.php/Home. 
  163. "University of Oregon; Music Learning, Perception, and Cognition Focus Group". http://icds.uoregon.edu/groups/focus/music-learning-perception-and-cognition-focus-group/. 
  164. "Stanford University; Center for Computer Research in Music and Acoustics". https://ccrma.stanford.edu. 
  165. "University of Texas at Dallas; Dowling Laboratory". http://bbs.utdallas.edu/psysciphd/labs.html. 
  166. "University of Texas at San Antonio; Institute for Music Research". http://music.utsa.edu/index.php/areas/imr. 
  167. "University of Washington; MCCL". https://depts.washington.edu/mccl/Home.html. 
  168. "Wesleyan University; MIND Lab". http://mindlab.research.wesleyan.edu. 
  169. "Western Michigan University; BRAIN Lab". http://www.wmich.edu/brain/. 

Further reading

Encyclopedia entries

  • Palmer, Caroline & Melissa K. Jungers (2003): Music Cognition. In: Lynn Nadel: Encyclopedia of Cognitive Science, Vol. 3, London: Nature Publishing Group, pp. 155–158.
  • Deutsch, Diana (2013): Music. In Oxford Bibliographies in Music. Edited by Dunn, D.S. New York: Oxford University Press. 2013, Web Link
  • Thompson, William Forde (2014): "Music in the Social and Behavioral Sciences, An Encyclopedia". Sage Publications Inc., New York. ISBN:9781452283036 Web Link

Introductory reading

Advanced reading

  • Deutsch, D. (Ed.) (1982). The Psychology of Music, 1st Edition. New York: Academic Press. ISBN:0-12-213562-8.
  • Deutsch, D. (Ed.) (1999). The Psychology of Music, 2nd Edition. San Diego: Academic Press. ISBN:0-12-213565-2.
  • Deutsch, D. (Ed.) (2013). The Psychology of Music, 3rd Edition. San Diego: Academic Press. ISBN:0-12-381460-X.
  • Dowling, W. Jay and Harwood, Dane L. (1986). Music Cognition. San Diego: Academic Press. ISBN:0-12-221430-7.
  • Hallam, Cross, & Thaut, (eds.) (2008). The Oxford Handbook of Music Psychology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Krumhansl, Carol L. (2001). Cognitive Foundations of Musical Pitch. Oxford: Oxford University Press. ISBN:0-19-514836-3.
  • Patel, Anirrudh D. (2010). Music, language, and the brain. New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Parncutt, R. (1989). Harmony: A Psychoacoustical Approach. Berlin: Springer.
  • Proverbio, A.M. (2019). Neuroscienze Cognitive della Musica: Il cervello musicale tra Arte e Scienza, Zanichelli, Bologna.[1]
  • Sloboda, John A. (1985). The Musical Mind: The Cognitive Psychology of Music. Oxford: Oxford University Press. ISBN:0-19-852128-6.
  • Lerdahl, F. and Jackendoff, R. (21996) A Generative Theory of Tonal Music. The MIT Press. ISBN:978-0-262-62107-6.
  • Jackendoff, Ray (1987): Consciousness and the Computational Mind. Cambridge: MIT Press. Chapter 11: Levels of Musical Structure, section 11.1: What is Musical Cognition?
  • Temperley, D. (2004). The Cognition of Basic Musical Structures. The MIT Press. ISBN:978-0-262-70105-1.
  • Thompson, W. F. (2009). Music, Thought, and Feeling: Understanding the Psychology of Music New York: Oxford University Press. ISBN:978-0-19-537707-1.
  • Zbikowski, Lawrence M. (2004). Conceptualizing Music: Cognitive Structure, Theory, and Analysis. Oxford University Press, USA. ISBN:978-0-19-514023-1.
  • North, A.C. & Hargreaves, D.J. (2008). The Social and Applied Psychology of Music. Oxford: Oxford University Press. ISBN:978-0-19-856742-4.

External links