Biology:Brain size

From HandWiki
Revision as of 05:31, 12 February 2024 by Raymond Straus (talk | contribs) (url)
(diff) ← Older revision | Latest revision (diff) | Newer revision → (diff)
Short description: Topic of study


The size of the brain is a frequent topic of study within the fields of anatomy, biological anthropology, animal science and evolution. Measuring brain size and cranial capacity is relevant both to humans and other animals, and can be done by weight or volume via MRI scans, by skull volume, or by neuroimaging intelligence testing. The relationship between brain size and intelligence remains a controversial although frequently investigated question.

Humans

In humans, the right cerebral hemisphere is typically larger than the left, whereas the cerebellar hemispheres are typically closer in size. The adult human brain weighs on average about 1.5 kg (3.3 lb).[1] In men the average weight is about 1370 g and in women about 1200 g.[2] The volume is around 1260 cm3 in men and 1130 cm3 in women, although there is substantial individual variation.[3] Yet another study argued that adult human brain weight is 1300-1400 g for adult humans and 350-400 g for newborn humans. There is a range of volume and weights, and not just one number that one can definitively rely on, as with body mass. It is also important to note that variation between individuals is not as important as variation within species, as overall the differences are much smaller. The mechanisms of interspecific and intraspecific variation also differ.

Variation and evolution

Modern human cranial size over the last 300 ka using data consolidated into 100-year means according to one 2022 study[4]
...and for the last 30 ka[4]

From early primates to hominids and finally to Homo sapiens, the brain is progressively larger, with exception of extinct Neanderthals whose brain size exceeded modern Homo sapiens. The volume of the human brain has increased as humans have evolved (see Homininae), starting from about 600 cm3 in Homo habilis up to 1680 cm3 in Homo neanderthalensis, which was the hominid with the biggest brain size.[5] Some data suggest that the average brain size has decreased since then,[6] including a study concluding the decrease "was surprisingly recent, occurring in the last 3,000 years".[7] [8] However, a reanalysis of the same data [7] suggests that brain size has not decreased, and that the conclusion was made using datasets that are too dissimilar to support quantitative comparison.[9][4]

Proponents of recent changes in brain size draw attention to the gene mutation that causes microcephaly, a neural developmental disorder that affects cerebral cortical volume.[10] Similarly, sociocultural explanations draw attention to externalization of knowledge and group decision-making, partly via the advent of social systems of distributed cognition, social organization, division of labor and sharing of information as possible causes.[11][12][7]

Trends in hominin brain size evolution[7]
Specimens of analysis on human brain size over 9.8-million-years shown in the image above[4]
Brain sizes of hominids
Name Brain size (cm3)[13]
Homo habilis 550–687
Homo ergaster 700–900
Homo erectus 600–1250
Homo heidelbergensis 1100–1400
Homo neanderthalensis 1200–1750
Homo sapiens 1400
Homo floresiensis 417 [14]

H. floresiensis' small brain

Homo floresiensis is a hominin from the island of Flores in Indonesia with fossils dating from 60,000-100,000 years ago.[15] Despite its relatively derived position in the hominin phylogeny, CT imaging of its skull reveals that its brain volume was only 417 cm3,[14] less than that of even Homo habilis, which is believed to have gone extinct far earlier (around 1.65 million years ago.[16]). The reason for this regression in brain size is believed to be island syndrome [17] in which the brains of insular species become smaller due to reduced predation risk. This is beneficial as it reduces the basal metabolic rate without significant increases in predation risk.[18]


Hydrocephalus

Exceptional cases of hydrocephalus, such as what was reported by John Lorber in 1980 and by a study with rats,[19][20] suggest that relatively high levels of intelligence and relatively normal functioning are possible even with very small brains.[21][22] It is unclear what conclusions could be drawn from such reports – such as about brain capacities, redundancies, mechanics and size requirements.

Biogeographic variation

Efforts to find racial or ethnic variation in brain size are generally considered to be a pseudoscientific endeavor[23][24][25] and have traditionally been tied to scientific racism and attempts to demonstrate a racial intellectual hierarchy.[25][26][27][28]

The majority of efforts to demonstrate this have relied on indirect data that assessed skull measurements as opposed to direct brain observations. These are considered scientifically discredited.[26][29]

A large-scale 1984 survey of global variation in skulls has concluded that variation in skull and head sizes is unrelated to race, but rather climatic heat preservation, stating "We find little support for the use of brain size in taxonomic assessment (other than with paleontological extremes over time). Racial taxonomies which include cranial capacity, head shape, or any other trait influenced by climate confound ecotypic and phyletic causes. For Pleistocene hominids, we doubt that the volume of the braincase is any more taxonomically 'valuable' than any other trait."[30]

Sex

Average brain weight for males and females over lifespan. From the study Changes in brain weights during the span of human life.

A human baby's brain at birth averages 369 cm3 and increases, during the first year of life, to about 961 cm3, after which the growth rate declines. Brain volume peaks at the teenage years,[31] and after the age of 40 it begins declining at 5% per decade, speeding up around 70.[32] Average adult male brain weight is 1,345 grams (47.4 oz), while an adult female has an average brain weight of 1,222 grams (43.1 oz).[33] (This does not take into account neuron density nor brain-to-body mass ratio; men on average also have larger bodies than women.) Males have been found to have on average greater cerebral, cerebellar and cerebral cortical lobar volumes, except possibly left parietal.[34] The gender differences in size vary by more specific brain regions. Studies have tended to indicate that men have a relatively larger amygdala and hypothalamus, while women have a relatively larger caudate and hippocampi. When covaried for intracranial volume, height, and weight, Kelly (2007) indicates women have a higher percentage of gray matter, whereas men have a higher percentage of white matter and cerebrospinal fluid. There is high variability between individuals in these studies, however.[3]

However, Yaki (2011) found no statistically significant gender differences in the gray matter ratio for most ages (grouped by decade), except in the 3rd and 6th decades of life in the sample of 758 women and 702 men aged 20–69.[35] The average male in their third decade (ages 20–29) had a significantly higher gray matter ratio than the average female of the same age group. In contrast, among subjects in their sixth decade, the average woman had a significantly larger gray matter ratio, though no meaningful difference was found among those in their 7th decade of life.

Total cerebral and gray matter volumes peak during the ages from 10–20 years (earlier in girls than boys), whereas white matter and ventricular volumes increase. There is a general pattern in neural development of childhood peaks followed by adolescent declines (e.g. synaptic pruning). Consistent with adult findings, average cerebral volume is approximately 10% larger in boys than girls. However, such differences should not be interpreted as imparting any sort of functional advantage or disadvantage; gross structural measures may not reflect functionally relevant factors such as neuronal connectivity and receptor density, and of note is the high variability of brain size even in narrowly defined groups, for example children at the same age may have as much as a 50% differences in total brain volume.[36] Young girls have on average relative larger hippocampal volume, whereas the amygdalae are larger in boys.[3] However, multiple studies[37][38] have found a higher synaptic density in males: a 2008 study reported that men had a significantly higher average synaptic density of 12.9 × 108 per cubic millimeter, whereas in women it was 8.6 × 108 per cubic millimeter, a 33% difference. Other studies have found an average of 4 billion more neurons in the male brain,[39] corroborating this difference, as each neuron has on average 7,000 synaptic connections to other neurons.

Significant dynamic changes in brain structure take place through adulthood and aging, with substantial variation between individuals. In later decades, men show greater volume loss in whole brain volume and in the frontal lobes, and temporal lobes, whereas in women there is increased volume loss in the hippocampi and parietal lobes.[3] Men show a steeper decline in global gray matter volume, although in both sexes it varies by region with some areas exhibiting little or no age effect. Overall white matter volume does not appear to decline with age, although there is variation between brain regions.[40]

Genetic contribution

Adult twin studies have indicated high heritability estimates for overall brain size in adulthood (between 66% and 97%). The effect varies regionally within the brain, however, with high heritabilities of frontal lobe volumes (90-95%), moderate estimates in the hippocampi (40-69%), and environmental factors influencing several medial brain areas. In addition, lateral ventricle volume appears to be mainly explained by environmental factors, suggesting such factors also play a role in the surrounding brain tissue. Genes may cause the association between brain structure and cognitive functions, or the latter may influence the former during life. A number of candidate genes have been identified or suggested, but they await replication.[41][42]

Intelligence

Studies demonstrate a correlation between brain size and intelligence, larger brains predicting higher intelligence. It is however not clear if the correlation is causal.[43] The majority of MRI studies report moderate correlations around 0.3 to 0.4 between brain volume and intelligence.[44][45] The most consistent associations are observed within the frontal, temporal, and parietal lobes, the hippocampus, and the cerebellum, but only account for a relatively small amount of variance in IQ, which suggests that while brain size may be related to human intelligence, other factors also play a role.[45][46] In addition, brain volumes do not correlate strongly with other and more specific cognitive measures.[47] In men, IQ correlates more with gray matter volume in the frontal lobe and parietal lobe, which is roughly involved in sensory integration and attention, whereas in women it correlates with gray matter volume in the frontal lobe and Broca's area, which is involved in language.[3]

Research measuring brain volume, P300 auditory evoked potentials, and intelligence shows a dissociation, such that both brain volume and speed of P300 correlate with measured aspects of intelligence, but not with each other.[48][49] Evidence conflicts on the question of whether brain size variation also predicts intelligence between siblings, as some studies find moderate correlations and others find none.[43] A recent review by Nesbitt, Flynn et al. (2012) points out that crude brain size is unlikely to be a accurate measure of IQ. Brain size is known to differ between men and women, for example (men on average have larger bodies than women), but without well documented differences in IQ.[43]

A discovery in recent years is that the structure of the adult human brain changes when a new cognitive or motor skill, including vocabulary, is learned.[50] Structural neuroplasticity (increased gray matter volume) has been demonstrated in adults after three months of training in a visual-motor skill, as the qualitative change (i.e. learning of a new task) appear more critical for the brain to change its structure than continued training of an already-learned task. Such changes (e.g. revising for medical exams) have been shown to last for at least 3 months without further practicing; other examples include learning novel speech sounds, musical ability, navigation skills and learning to read mirror-reflected words.[51][52]

Other animals

The largest brains are those of sperm whales, weighing about 8 kg (18 lb). An elephant's brain weighs just over 5 kg (11 lb), a bottlenose dolphin's 1.5 to 1.7 kg (3.3 to 3.7 lb), whereas a human brain is around 1.3 to 1.5 kg (2.9 to 3.3 lb). Brain size tends to vary according to body size. The relationship is not proportional, though: the brain-to-body mass ratio varies. The largest ratio found is in the shrew.[53] Averaging brain weight across all orders of mammals, it follows a power law, with an exponent of about 0.75.[54] There are good reasons to expect a power law: for example, the body-size to body-length relationship follows a power law with an exponent of 0.33, and the body-size to surface-area relationship follows a power law with an exponent of 0.67. The explanation for an exponent of 0.75 is not obvious; however, it is worth noting that several physiological variables appear to be related to body size by approximately the same exponent—for example, the basal metabolic rate.[55]

This power law formula applies to the "average" brain of mammals taken as a whole, but each family (cats, rodents, primates, etc.) departs from it to some degree, in a way that generally reflects the overall "sophistication" of behavior.[56] Primates, for a given body size, have brains 5 to 10 times as large as the formula predicts. Predators tend to have relatively larger brains than the animals they prey on; placental mammals (the great majority) have relatively larger brains than marsupials such as the opossum. A standard measure for assessing an animal's brain size compared to what would be expected from its body size is known as the encephalization quotient. The encephalization quotient for humans is between 7.4-7.8.[57]

When the mammalian brain increases in size, not all parts increase at the same rate.[58] In particular, the larger the brain of a species, the greater the fraction taken up by the cortex. Thus, in the species with the largest brains, most of their volume is filled with cortex: this applies not only to humans, but also to animals such as dolphins, whales or elephants. The evolution of Homo sapiens over the past two million years has been marked by a steady increase in brain size, but much of it can be accounted for by corresponding increases in body size.[59] There are, however, many departures from the trend that are difficult to explain in a systematic way: in particular, the appearance of modern man about 100,000 years ago was marked by a decrease in body size at the same time as an increase in brain size. Even so, it is noteworthy that Neanderthals, which became extinct about 40,000 years ago, had larger brains than modern Homo sapiens.[60]

Not all investigators are happy with the amount of attention that has been paid to brain size. Roth and Dicke, for example, have argued that factors other than size are more highly correlated with intelligence, such as the number of cortical neurons and the speed of their connections.[61] Moreover, they point out that intelligence depends not just on the amount of brain tissue, but on the details of how it is structured. It is also well known that crows, ravens, and grey parrots are quite intelligent even though they have small brains.

While humans have the largest encephalization quotient of extant animals, it is not out of line for a primate.[62][63] Some other anatomical trends are correlated in the human evolutionary path with brain size: the basicranium becomes more flexed with increasing brain size relative to basicranial length.[64]

Cranial capacity

Cranial capacity is a measure of the volume of the interior of the skull of those vertebrates who have a brain. The most commonly used unit of measure is the cubic centimetre (cm3). The volume of the cranium is used as a rough indicator of the size of the brain, and this in turn is used as a rough indicator of the potential intelligence of the organism. Cranial capacity is often tested by filling the cranial cavity with glass beads and measuring their volume, or by CT scan imaging.[65][66] A more accurate way of measuring cranial capacity, is to make an endocranial cast and measure the amount of water the cast displaces. In the past there have been dozens of studies done to estimate cranial capacity on skulls. Most of these studies have been done on dry skull using linear dimensions, packing methods or occasionally radiological methods.[citation needed]

Knowledge of the volume of the cranial cavity can be important information for the study of different populations with various differences like geographical, racial, or ethnic origin. Other things can also affect cranial capacity such as nutrition.[67] It is also used to study correlating between cranial capacity with other cranial measurements and in comparing skulls from different beings. It is commonly used to study abnormalities of cranial size and shape or aspects of growth and development of the volume of the brain.[citation needed] Cranial capacity is an indirect approach to test the size of the brain. A few studies on cranial capacity have been done on living beings through linear dimensions.[citation needed]

However, larger cranial capacity is not always indicative of a more intelligent organism, since larger capacities are required for controlling a larger body, or in many cases are an adaptive feature for life in a colder environment. For instance, among modern Homo sapiens, northern populations have a 20% larger visual cortex than those in the southern latitude populations, and this potentially explains the population differences in human brain size (and roughly cranial capacity).[68][69] Neurological functions are determined more by the organization of the brain rather than the volume. Individual variability is also important when considering cranial capacity, for example the average Neanderthal cranial capacity for females was 1300 cm3 and 1600 cm3 for males.[70] Neanderthals had larger eyes and bodies relative to their height, thus a disproportionately large area of their brain was dedicated to somatic and visual processing, functions not normally associated with intelligence. When these areas were adjusted to match anatomically modern human proportions it was found Neanderthals had brains 15-22% smaller than in anatomically-modern humans.[71] When the neanderthal version of the NOVA1 gene is inserted into stem cells it creates neurons with fewer synapses than stem cells containing the human version.[72]

Parts of a cranium found in China in the 1970s show that the young man had a cranial capacity of around 1700 cm3 at least 160,000 years ago. This is greater than the average of modern humans.[73][74]

In an attempt to use cranial capacity as an objective indicator of brain size, the encephalization quotient (EQ) was developed in 1973 by Harry Jerison. It compares the size of the brain of the specimen to the expected brain size of animals with roughly the same weight.[75] This way a more objective judgement can be made on the cranial capacity of an individual animal. A large scientific collection of brain endocasts and measurements of cranial capacity has been compiled by Holloway.[76]

Examples of cranial capacity

Apes

  • Orangutans: 275–500 cm3 (16.8–30.5 cu in)
  • Chimpanzees: 275–500 cm3 (16.8–30.5 cu in)
  • Gorillas: 340–752 cm3 (20.7–45.9 cu in)

Hominids

See also

References

  1. Parent, A; Carpenter MB (1995). "Ch. 1". Carpenter's Human Neuroanatomy. Williams & Wilkins. ISBN 978-0-683-06752-1. 
  2. Harrison, Paul J.; Freemantle, Nick; Geddes, John R. (November 2003). "Meta-analysis of brain weight in schizophrenia". Schizophrenia Research 64 (1): 25–34. doi:10.1016/s0920-9964(02)00502-9. PMID 14511798. 
  3. 3.0 3.1 3.2 3.3 3.4 Cosgrove, Kelly P.; Mazure, Carolyn M.; Staley, Julie K. (October 2007). "Evolving Knowledge of Sex Differences in Brain Structure, Function, and Chemistry". Biological Psychiatry 62 (8): 847–855. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.03.001. PMID 17544382. 
  4. 4.0 4.1 4.2 4.3 Villmoare, Brian; Grabowski, Mark (2022). "Did the transition to complex societies in the Holocene drive a reduction in brain size? A reassessment of the DeSilva et al. (2021) hypothesis". Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution 10. doi:10.3389/fevo.2022.963568. ISSN 2296-701X. 
  5. "Neanderthal man". infoplease. http://www.infoplease.com/encyclopedia/society/neanderthal-man.html. 
  6. McAuliffe, Kathleen (2011-01-20). "If Modern Humans Are So Smart, Why Are Our Brains Shrinking?". DiscoverMagazine.com. https://www.discovermagazine.com/the-sciences/if-modern-humans-are-so-smart-why-are-our-brains-shrinking. 
  7. 7.0 7.1 7.2 7.3 DeSilva, Jeremy M.; Traniello, James F. A.; Claxton, Alexander G.; Fannin, Luke D. (2021). "When and Why Did Human Brains Decrease in Size? A New Change-Point Analysis and Insights From Brain Evolution in Ants". Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution 9: 712. doi:10.3389/fevo.2021.742639. ISSN 2296-701X. 
  8. Henneberg, Maciej (1988). "Decrease of human skull size in the Holocene". Human Biology 60 (3): 395–405. PMID 3134287. 
  9. Corless, Victoria (18 August 2022). "No, the human brain did not shrink". Advanced Science News. https://www.advancedsciencenews.com/no-the-human-brain-did-not-shrink/. 
  10. Kouprina, Natalay; Pavlicek, Adam; Mochida, Ganeshwaran H; Solomon, Gregory; Gersch, William; Yoon, Young-Ho; Collura, Randall; Ruvolo, Maryellen et al. (23 March 2004). "Accelerated Evolution of the ASPM Gene Controlling Brain Size Begins Prior to Human Brain Expansion". PLOS Biology 2 (5): e126. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.0020126. PMID 15045028. 
  11. "When and why did human brains decrease in size 3,000 years ago? Ants may hold clues" (in en). phys.org. https://phys.org/news/2021-10-human-brains-decrease-size-years.html. 
  12. Baraniuk, Chris. "Why human brains were bigger 3,000 years ago" (in en). BBC. https://www.bbc.com/future/article/20220503-why-human-brains-were-bigger-3000-years-ago. 
  13. Brown, Graham; Fairfax, Stephanie; Sarao, Nidhi. "Human Evolution". Tree of Life Project. http://tolweb.org/treehouses/?treehouse_id=3710. 
  14. 14.0 14.1 Falk, Dean; Hildebolt, Charles; Smith, Kirk; Morwood, M. J.; Sutikna, Thomas; Brown, Peter; Jatmiko; Saptomo, E. Wayhu et al. (8 Apr 2005). "The Brain of LB1, Homo floresiensis". Science 308 (5719): 242–245. doi:10.1126/science.1109727. PMID 15749690. Bibcode2005Sci...308..242F. https://www.science.org/doi/10.1126/science.1109727. 
  15. Sutikna, Thomas et al. (30 March 2016). "Revised stratigraphy and chronology for Homo floresiensis at Liang Bua in Indonesia". Nature 532 (7599): 366–9. doi:10.1038/nature17179. PMID 27027286. Bibcode2016Natur.532..366S. https://ro.uow.edu.au/smhpapers/3778. 
  16. F. Spoor; P. Gunz; S. Neubauer; S. Stelzer; N. Scott; A. Kwekason; M. C. Dean (2015). "Reconstructed Homo habilis type OH 7 suggests deep-rooted species diversity in early Homo". Nature 519 (7541): 83–86. doi:10.1038/nature14224. PMID 25739632. Bibcode2015Natur.519...83S. 
  17. Baeckens, Simon; Van Damme, Raoul (20 April 2020). "The island syndrome". Current Biology 30 (8): R329–R339. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2020.03.029. PMID 32315628. 
  18. Herculano-Houzel, Suzana (1 March 2011). "Scaling of Brain Metabolism with a Fixed Energy Budget per Neuron: Implications for Neuronal Activity, Plasticity and Evolution". PLOS ONE 6 (3): e17514. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0017514. PMID 21390261. Bibcode2011PLoSO...617514H. 
  19. Bracci, Aria. "A rat had basically no brain—but it could still see, hear, smell and feel" (in en). Northeastern University. https://medicalxpress.com/news/2020-01-rat-basically-brainbut.html. 
  20. Ferris, C. F.; Cai, X.; Qiao, J.; Switzer, B.; Baun, J.; Morrison, T.; Iriah, S.; Madularu, D. et al. (11 November 2019). "Life without a brain: Neuroradiological and behavioral evidence of neuroplasticity necessary to sustain brain function in the face of severe hydrocephalus" (in en). Scientific Reports 9 (1): 16479. doi:10.1038/s41598-019-53042-3. ISSN 2045-2322. PMID 31712649. Bibcode2019NatSR...916479F. 
  21. Forsdyke, Donald R. (1 December 2015). "Wittgenstein's Certainty is Uncertain: Brain Scans of Cured Hydrocephalics Challenge Cherished Assumptions" (in en). Biological Theory 10 (4): 336–342. doi:10.1007/s13752-015-0219-x. ISSN 1555-5550. 
  22. "Remarkable story of maths genius who had almost no brain" (in en). The Irish Times. https://www.irishtimes.com/news/remarkable-story-of-maths-genius-who-had-almost-no-brain-1.1026845. 
  23. "Lost Research Notes Clear up Racial Bias Debate in Old Skull Size Study". https://www.discovermagazine.com/planet-earth/lost-research-notes-clear-up-racial-bias-debate-in-old-skull-size-study. 
  24. "The disturbing return of scientific racism". Wired UK. https://www.wired.co.uk/article/superior-the-return-of-race-science-angela-saini. 
  25. 25.0 25.1 Mitchell, Paul Wolff (4 October 2018). "The fault in his seeds: Lost notes to the case of bias in Samuel George Morton's cranial race science". PLOS Biology 16 (10): e2007008. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.2007008. PMID 30286069. 
  26. 26.0 26.1 Gould, S. J. (1981). The Mismeasure of Man. New York: W. W. Norton & Company.[page needed]
  27. Graves, Joseph L. (September 2015). "Great Is Their Sin: Biological Determinism in the Age of Genomics". The Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science 661 (1): 24–50. doi:10.1177/0002716215586558. 
  28. Kaplan, Jonathan Michael; Pigliucci, Massimo; Banta, Joshua Alexander (1 August 2015). "Gould on Morton, Redux: What can the debate reveal about the limits of data?". Studies in History and Philosophy of Science Part C: Studies in History and Philosophy of Biological and Biomedical Sciences 52: 22–31. doi:10.1016/j.shpsc.2015.01.001. PMID 25666493. https://philarchive.org/rec/KAPGOM. 
  29. Kamin, Leon J.; Omari, Safiya (September 1998). "Race, Head Size, and Intelligence". South African Journal of Psychology 28 (3): 119–128. doi:10.1177/008124639802800301. 
  30. Beals, Kenneth L.; Smith, Courtland L.; Dodd, Stephen M.; Angel, J. Lawrence; Armstrong, Este; Blumenberg, Bennett; Girgis, Fakhry G.; Turkel, Spencer et al. (June 1984). "Brain Size, Cranial Morphology, Climate, and Time Machines [and Comments and Reply"]. Current Anthropology 25 (3): 312. doi:10.1086/203138. http://syslearn.oregonstate.edu/instruction/anth/smith/TimeMach1984.pdf. 
  31. Giedd, Jay N.; Blumenthal, Jonathan; Jeffries, Neal O.; Castellanos, F. X.; Liu, Hong; Zijdenbos, Alex; Paus, Tomáš; Evans, Alan C. et al. (October 1999). "Brain development during childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal MRI study". Nature Neuroscience 2 (10): 861–863. doi:10.1038/13158. PMID 10491603. 
  32. Peters, R. (2006). "Ageing and the brain". Postgraduate Medical Journal 82 (964): 84–8. doi:10.1136/pgmj.2005.036665. PMID 16461469. PMC 2596698. http://ncbi.nlm/. Retrieved 2019-09-12. 
  33. Kelley Hays; David S. (1998). Reader in Gender archaeology. Routlegde. ISBN 9780415173605. https://books.google.com/books?id=u1bwQj7qdsYC. Retrieved 2014-09-21. 
  34. Carne, Ross P.; Vogrin, Simon; Litewka, Lucas; Cook, Mark J. (January 2006). "Cerebral cortex: An MRI-based study of volume and variance with age and sex". Journal of Clinical Neuroscience 13 (1): 60–72. doi:10.1016/j.jocn.2005.02.013. PMID 16410199. 
  35. Taki, Y.; Thyreau, B.; Kinomura, S.; Sato, K.; Goto, R.; Kawashima, R.; Fukuda, H. (2011). He, Yong. ed. "Correlations among Brain Gray Matter Volumes, Age, Gender, and Hemisphere in Healthy Individuals". PLOS ONE 6 (7): e22734. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0022734. PMID 21818377. Bibcode2011PLoSO...622734T. 
  36. Giedd, Jay N. (April 2008). "The Teen Brain: Insights from Neuroimaging". Journal of Adolescent Health 42 (4): 335–343. doi:10.1016/j.jadohealth.2008.01.007. PMID 18346658. 
  37. Rabinowicz, Theodore; Petetot, Jean MacDonald-Comber; Gartside, Peter S.; Sheyn, David; Sheyn, Tony; de Courten-Myers, Gabrielle M. (January 2002). "Structure of the Cerebral Cortex in Men and Women". Journal of Neuropathology & Experimental Neurology 61 (1): 46–57. doi:10.1093/jnen/61.1.46. ProQuest 229729071. PMID 11829343. 
  38. Alonso-Nanclares, L.; Gonzalez-Soriano, J.; Rodriguez, J. R.; DeFelipe, J. (23 September 2008). "Gender differences in human cortical synaptic density". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 105 (38): 14615–14619. doi:10.1073/pnas.0803652105. PMID 18779570. Bibcode2008PNAS..10514615A. 
  39. Pakkenberg, Bente; Gundersen, Hans Jørgen G. (1997). "Neocortical neuron number in humans: Effect of sex and age". Journal of Comparative Neurology 384 (2): 312–320. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(19970728)384:2<312::AID-CNE10>3.0.CO;2-K. PMID 9215725. 
  40. Good, Catriona D.; Johnsrude, Ingrid S.; Ashburner, John; Henson, Richard N.A.; Friston, Karl J.; Frackowiak, Richard S.J. (July 2001). "A Voxel-Based Morphometric Study of Ageing in 465 Normal Adult Human Brains". NeuroImage 14 (1): 21–36. doi:10.1006/nimg.2001.0786. PMID 11525331. http://pdfs.semanticscholar.org/1043/8bd0e1231dc8bf12ec3f407b5a6a7aa026c9.pdf. 
  41. Peper, Jiska S.; Brouwer, Rachel M.; Boomsma, Dorret I.; Kahn, René S.; Hulshoff Pol, Hilleke E. (June 2007). "Genetic influences on human brain structure: A review of brain imaging studies in twins". Human Brain Mapping 28 (6): 464–473. doi:10.1002/hbm.20398. PMID 17415783. 
  42. Zhang, Jianzhi (December 2003). "Evolution of the human ASPM gene, a major determinant of brain size". Genetics 165 (4): 2063–2070. doi:10.1093/genetics/165.4.2063. PMID 14704186. 
  43. 43.0 43.1 43.2 Nisbett, Richard E.; Aronson, Joshua; Blair, Clancy; Dickens, William; Flynn, James; Halpern, Diane F.; Turkheimer, Eric (February 2012). "Intelligence: New findings and theoretical developments". American Psychologist 67 (2): 130–159. doi:10.1037/a0026699. PMID 22233090. https://pdfs.semanticscholar.org/c03f/f20904c35a370534a9d3710453dd6dc7a2d2.pdf. 
  44. Mcdaniel, M (July 2005). "Big-brained people are smarter: A meta-analysis of the relationship between in vivo brain volume and intelligence". Intelligence 33 (4): 337–346. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2004.11.005. 
  45. 45.0 45.1 Luders, Eileen; Narr, Katherine L.; Thompson, Paul M.; Toga, Arthur W. (March 2009). "Neuroanatomical correlates of intelligence". Intelligence 37 (2): 156–163. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2008.07.002. PMID 20160919. 
  46. Hoppe, Christian; Stojanovic, Jelena (August 2008). "High-Aptitude Minds". Scientific American Mind 19 (4): 60–67. doi:10.1038/scientificamericanmind0808-60. 
  47. Allen, John S.; Damasio, Hanna; Grabowski, Thomas J. (August 2002). "Normal neuroanatomical variation in the human brain: An MRI-volumetric study". American Journal of Physical Anthropology 118 (4): 341–358. doi:10.1002/ajpa.10092. PMID 12124914. 
  48. Egan, Vincent; Chiswick, Ann; Santosh, Celestine; Naidu, K.; Rimmington, J.Ewen; Best, Jonathan J.K. (September 1994). "Size isn't everything: A study of brain volume, intelligence and auditory evoked potentials". Personality and Individual Differences 17 (3): 357–367. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(94)90283-6. 
  49. Egan, Vincent; Wickett, John C.; Vernon, Philip A. (July 1995). "Brain size and intelligence: erratum, addendum, and correction". Personality and Individual Differences 19 (1): 113–115. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(95)00043-6. 
  50. Lee, H.; Devlin, J. T.; Shakeshaft, C.; Stewart, L. H.; Brennan, A.; Glensman, J.; Pitcher, K.; Crinion, J. et al. (31 January 2007). "Anatomical Traces of Vocabulary Acquisition in the Adolescent Brain". Journal of Neuroscience 27 (5): 1184–1189. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4442-06.2007. PMID 17267574. 
  51. Driemeyer, Joenna; Boyke, Janina; Gaser, Christian; Büchel, Christian; May, Arne (23 July 2008). "Changes in Gray Matter Induced by Learning—Revisited". PLOS ONE 3 (7): e2669. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0002669. PMID 18648501. Bibcode2008PLoSO...3.2669D. 
  52. Ilg, R.; Wohlschlager, A. M.; Gaser, C.; Liebau, Y.; Dauner, R.; Woller, A.; Zimmer, C.; Zihl, J. et al. (16 April 2008). "Gray Matter Increase Induced by Practice Correlates with Task-Specific Activation: A Combined Functional and Morphometric Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study". Journal of Neuroscience 28 (16): 4210–4215. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5722-07.2008. PMID 18417700. 
  53. Kevin Kelly. "The Technium: Brains of White Matter". kk.org. http://www.kk.org/thetechnium/archives/2004/11/brains_of_white.php. 
  54. Armstrong, E (17 June 1983). "Relative brain size and metabolism in mammals". Science 220 (4603): 1302–1304. doi:10.1126/science.6407108. PMID 6407108. Bibcode1983Sci...220.1302A. 
  55. Savage, V. M.; Gillooly, J. F.; Woodruff, W. H.; West, G. B.; Allen, A. P.; Enquist, B. J.; Brown, J. H. (April 2004). "The predominance of quarter-power scaling in biology". Functional Ecology 18 (2): 257–282. doi:10.1111/j.0269-8463.2004.00856.x. 
  56. Jerison, Harry J. (1973). Evolution of the Brain and Intelligence. Academic Press. ISBN 978-0-12-385250-2. [page needed]
  57. "Evolution of the brain and intelligence". Trends Cogn. Sci. (Regul. Ed.) 9 (5): 250–7. May 2005. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2005.03.005. PMID 15866152. 
  58. Finlay, Barbara L.; Darlington, Richard B.; Nicastro, Nicholas (April 2001). "Developmental structure in brain evolution". Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24 (2): 263–278. doi:10.1017/S0140525X01003958. PMID 11530543. http://pdfs.semanticscholar.org/61f5/d2349949e6171891491b45829c09c173b36a.pdf. 
  59. Kappelman, John (March 1996). "The evolution of body mass and relative brain size in fossil hominids". Journal of Human Evolution 30 (3): 243–276. doi:10.1006/jhev.1996.0021. 
  60. Holloway, Ralph L. (1996). "Toward a synthetic theory of human brain evolution". Origins of the Human Brain. pp. 42–54. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198523901.003.0003. ISBN 978-0-19-852390-1. 
  61. Roth, G; Dicke, U (May 2005). "Evolution of the brain and intelligence". Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9 (5): 250–257. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2005.03.005. PMID 15866152. 
  62. Motluk, Alison (28 July 2010). "Size isn't everything: The big brain myth". New Scientist. https://www.newscientist.com/article/mg20727711-300-size-isnt-everything-the-big-brain-myth/. 
  63. Azevedo, Frederico A.C.; Carvalho, Ludmila R.B.; Grinberg, Lea T.; Farfel, José Marcelo; Ferretti, Renata E.L.; Leite, Renata E.P.; Filho, Wilson Jacob; Lent, Roberto et al. (10 April 2009). "Equal numbers of neuronal and nonneuronal cells make the human brain an isometrically scaled-up primate brain". The Journal of Comparative Neurology 513 (5): 532–541. doi:10.1002/cne.21974. PMID 19226510. "We find that the adult male human brain contains on average 86.1 ± 8.1 billion NeuN-positive cells (“neurons”) and 84.6 ± 9.8 billion NeuN-negative (“nonneuronal”) cells. [...] These findings challenge the common view that humans stand out from other primates in their brain composition and indicate that, with regard to numbers of neuronal and nonneuronal cells, the human brain is an isometrically scaled-up primate brain.". 
  64. Ross, Callum; Henneberg, Maciej (December 1995). "Basicranial flexion, relative brain size, and facial kyphosis inHomo sapiens and some fossil hominids". American Journal of Physical Anthropology 98 (4): 575–593. doi:10.1002/ajpa.1330980413. PMID 8599387. 
  65. Logan, Corina J.; Clutton-Brock, Tim H. (January 2013). "Validating methods for estimating endocranial volume in individual red deer (Cervus elaphus)". Behavioural Processes 92: 143–146. doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2012.10.015. PMID 23137587. https://www.repository.cam.ac.uk/handle/1810/261261. 
  66. Logan, Corina J.; Palmstrom, Christin R. (11 June 2015). "Can endocranial volume be estimated accurately from external skull measurements in great-tailed grackles (Quiscalus mexicanus)?". PeerJ 3: e1000. doi:10.7717/peerj.1000. PMID 26082858. 
  67. Rushton, J. Philippe; Jensen, Arthur R. (2005). "Thirty years of research on race differences in cognitive ability". Psychology, Public Policy, and Law 11 (2): 235–294. doi:10.1037/1076-8971.11.2.235. 
  68. "BBC News - Dark winters 'led to bigger human brains and eyeballs'". BBC News. 27 July 2011. http://www.bbc.co.uk/news/science-environment-14279729. 
  69. Alok Jha (27 July 2011). "People at darker, higher latitudes evolved bigger eyes and brains". the Guardian. https://www.theguardian.com/science/2011/jul/27/higher-latitudes-bigger-eyes-brains. 
  70. Stanford, C., Allen, J.S., Anton, S.C., Lovell, N.C. (2009). Biological Anthropology: the Natural History of Humankind. Toronto: Pearson Canada. p. 301
  71. 71.0 71.1 Pearce, Eiluned; Stringer, Chris; Dunbar, R. I. M. (7 May 2013). "New insights into differences in brain organization between Neanderthals and anatomically modern humans". Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 280 (1758): 20130168. doi:10.1098/rspb.2013.0168. PMID 23486442. 
  72. Cohen, Jon (20 June 2018). "Exclusive: Neanderthal 'minibrains' grown in dish". Science. https://www.science.org/content/article/exclusive-neanderthal-minibrains-grown-dish. 
  73. 73.0 73.1 Michael Marshall (Feb 5, 2022). "160,000-year-old fossil may be the first Denisovan skull we've found". New Scientist. https://www.newscientist.com/article/2305830-160000-year-old-fossil-may-be-the-first-denisovan-skull-weve-found/. 
  74. 74.0 74.1 Xiu-Jie Wu (Feb 2022). "Evolution of cranial capacity revisited: A view from the late Middle Pleistocene cranium from Xujiayao, China". Journal of Human Evolution 163: 103119. doi:10.1016/j.jhevol.2021.103119. PMID 35026677. 
  75. Campbell, G.C., Loy, J.D., Cruz-Uribe, K. (2006). Humankind Emerging: Ninth Edition. Boston: Pearson. p346
  76. Holloway, Ralph L., Yuan, M. S., and Broadfield, D.C. (2004). The Human Fossil Record: Brain Endocasts: The Paleoneurological Evidence. New York. John Wiley & Sons Publishers (http://www.columbia.edu/~rlh2/PartII.pdf and http://www.columbia.edu/~rlh2/available_pdfs.html for further references).
  77. Haile-Selassie, Yohannes; Melillo, Stephanie M.; Vazzana, Antonino; Benazzi, Stefano; Ryan, Timothy M. (12 September 2019). "A 3.8-million-year-old hominin cranium from Woranso-Mille, Ethiopia". Nature 573 (7773): 214–219. doi:10.1038/s41586-019-1513-8. PMID 31462770. Bibcode2019Natur.573..214H. 
  78. Lieberman, Daniel. THE EVOLUTION OF THE HUMAN HEAD. pp. 433. 
  79. Lieberman, Daniel. THE EVOLUTION OF THE HUMAN HEAD. pp. 435. 

Further reading