Philosophy:Dishabituation
Part of a series on |
Psychology |
---|
|
Dishabituation (or dehabituation) is a form of recovered or restored behavioral response wherein the reaction towards a known stimulus is enhanced, as opposed to habituation.[1] Initially, it was proposed as an explanation to increased response for a habituated behavior by introducing an external stimulus;[2] however, upon further analysis, some have suggested that a proper analysis of dishabituation should be taken into consideration only when the response is increased by implying the original stimulus.[3]
Based on studies conducted over habituation's dual-process theory which attributed towards dishabituation, it is also determined that the latter was independent of any behavioral sensitization.[4]
An example of dishabituation is the response of a receptionist in a scenario where a delivery truck arrives at 9:00AM every morning. The first few times it arrives it is noticed by the receptionist, and after weeks, the receptionist does not respond as strongly. One day the truck does not arrive, and the receptionist notices its absence. When it arrives the next day, the receptionist's response is stronger when it arrives as expected.
History
The phenomenon was studied by an early scientist Samuel Jackson Holmes in 1912, while he was studying the animal behavior in sea urchins. Later in 1933, George Humphrey—while studying the same effects in human babies and extensively over lower vertebrates—argued that dishabituation is in fact the removal of habituation altogether, to a behavior that was not conditioned to begin with.[5]
Mechanism
In humans
According to the dual-process theory of habituation, dishabituation is characterized by an increase in responding to a habituated stimulus after introducing a deviant, to sensitize a change in arousal.[6][4] For example, when hearing the ticking of a clock and the clock makes a louder ticking sound, you pay more attention to the clock even though you are already familiar with a clock. Further investigations into elicitation and habituation of the electrodermal orienting reflex also showed that dishabituation is independent of sensitization for indifferent stimuli.[7]
A meta-analysis shows that dishabituation is improvised on preterm infants as compared to term infants based on the magnitude of stimulus sensitized.[8][9]
Biological basis
As per the Center for Neural Engineering, University of Southern California (Los Angeles ), the primordial hippocampus plays an important role in modeling the dishabituation of behavioral response. According to this, the interaction of two processes is dynamically postulated based on synaptic plasticity, which acquires both long and short-term forgetting. Along with that, cumulative shrinking is proposed to map responses from the temporal region of the anterior thalamus that references the spatial positions. The plasticity model combined with the structure of medial pallium model provides a structured network of neural mechanisms, contributing towards dishabituation and habituation alike.[10]
Accordingly, this phenomenon is neither indicative to counteract the emphasis of an existing habituation but instead, organizes an independent neuronal process, nor resulted by facilitation, as the etymology may indicate.[11]
In animals
- Studies done on the Aplysia to establish dishabituation or superimposed sensitization,[12] to understand their neuronal mechanisms of their gill-withdrawal reflex,[13][14] and dishabituation processes to establish large-scale methodology to solve major limitation problem.[15]
- The Tritonia diomedea for its escape swim (the number of cycles per swim)[16] and the Drosophila melanogaster[17]
- Auditory stimulation to understand the escape behavior in hermit crabs[18]
- Exercise as a dishabituating stimulus on hypoglycemic rodents[19]
All the above establish the process of dishabituation, where responding to a repetitive stimulus increases and has been documented in a wide range of organisms - from single-celled animals to primates - which is thought to allow an organism to reflexively either filter out or consider, all forms of information.[20]
It is also characterized as an emancipation of an existing prey-catching behavior. Sometimes however, the inconsistency in dishabituation of behavioral response is brought-on by mismatch between the 1st and 2nd stimuli, which in-turn is due to the occurrence of inhibition by habituation, to the existing stimulus.[11]
Application
Dishabituation shows an increase in reward effectiveness as it produces a heightened behavioral response to sensitization of arousal.[6] Other studies also show that it is caused by mind-wandering, where with distributed working process as opposed to practising in mass, the learning behavior is enhanced.[21]
In the development of preterm infants, the dishabituation process also provides with an approach for the early diagnosis of cognitive status and most importantly, their mental faculties performances.[8][22]
See also
- Adaptive behaviors
- Operant conditioning
- Perceptual learning
- Reinforcement
References
- ↑ Steiner, Genevieve Z.; Barry, Robert J. (2014-02-14). "The mechanism of dishabituation". Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience 8: 14. doi:10.3389/fnint.2014.00014. ISSN 1662-5145. PMID 24592215.
- ↑ "Classical Conditioning | Learning, Memory, & Attention". http://www.cogsci.ucsd.edu/~coulson/101b/Learning1.pdf.
- ↑ Rankin, Catharine H.; Abrams, Thomas; Barry, Robert J.; Bhatnagar, Seema; Clayton, David; Colombo, John; Coppola, Gianluca; Geyer, Mark A. et al. (2017-03-28). "Habituation Revisited: An Updated and Revised Description of the Behavioral Characteristics of Habituation". Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 92 (2): 135–138. doi:10.1016/j.nlm.2008.09.012. ISSN 1074-7427. PMID 18854219.
- ↑ 4.0 4.1 Steiner, Genevieve Z.; Barry, Robert J. (2014-01-01). "The mechanism of dishabituation" (in en). Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience 8: 14. doi:10.3389/fnint.2014.00014. PMID 24592215. PMC 3924047. http://ro.uow.edu.au/sspapers/782/.
- ↑ Thompson, Richard F (2017-03-28). "Habituation: A History". Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 92 (2): 127–134. doi:10.1016/j.nlm.2008.07.011. ISSN 1074-7427. PMID 18703156.
- ↑ 6.0 6.1 Klein, Stephen B. (2011-04-04) (in en). Learning: Principles and Applications. SAGE Publications. ISBN 9781412987349. https://books.google.com/books?id=OT-rUMExGrAC&q=dishabituation&pg=PA53.
- ↑ Steiner, Genevieve Z.; Barry, Robert J. (2011-01-01). "Exploring the mechanism of dishabituation" (in en). Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 95 (4): 461–466. doi:10.1016/j.nlm.2011.02.007. ISSN 1074-7427. PMID 21329761. http://researchdirect.uws.edu.au/islandora/object/uws:30621.
- ↑ 8.0 8.1 Kavšek, Michael; Bornstein, Marc H. (2010-01-01). "Visual Habituation and Dishabituation in Preterm Infants: A Review and Meta-analysis". Research in Developmental Disabilities 31 (5): 951–975. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2010.04.016. ISSN 0891-4222. PMID 20488657.
- ↑ "Infant Perception and Cognition". http://web.mnstate.edu/malonech/Psy611/Notes/Bjorklund/Perception%20and%20Cognition%20BjCh4.htm.
- ↑ Wang, D.; Arbib, M. A. (1992-01-01). "Modeling the dishabituation hierarchy: the role of the primordial hippocampus". Biological Cybernetics 67 (6): 535–544. doi:10.1007/BF00198760. ISSN 0340-1200. PMID 1472577.
- ↑ 11.0 11.1 "Modeling the dishabituation hierarchy: The role of the primordial hippocampus". http://web.cse.ohio-state.edu/~wang.77/papers/Wang-Arbib.biocyb92.pdf.
- ↑ Hawkins, Robert D.; Cohen, Tracey E.; Kandel, Eric R. (2017-03-29). "Dishabituation in Aplysia can involve either reversal of habituation or superimposed sensitization". Learning & Memory 13 (3): 397–403. doi:10.1101/lm.49706. ISSN 1072-0502. PMID 16705138.
- ↑ "Neuronal Mechanisms of Habituation and Dishabituation of the Gill-Withdrawal Reflex in Aplysia". http://uoneuro.uoregon.edu/wehr/coursepapers/Castellucci-Kandel-1970.pdf.
- ↑ Carew, Thomas J.; Castellucci, Vincent F.; Kandel, Eric R. (1971-01-01). "An Analysis of Dishabituation and Sensitization of The Gill-Withdrawal Reflex In Aplysia" (in en). International Journal of Neuroscience 2 (2): 79–98. doi:10.3109/00207457109146995. ISSN 0020-7454. PMID 4347410. https://nyuscholars.nyu.edu/en/publications/an-analysis-of-dishabituation-and-sensitization-of-the-gill-withd.
- ↑ "Slimelines Vol-1, Page 3". http://yyy.rsmas.miami.edu/groups/sea-hares/slimelines/slimelines3.html.
- ↑ Mongeluzi, Donna L.; Frost, William N. (2017-03-29). "Dishabituation of the Tritonia Escape Swim". Learning & Memory 7 (1): 43–47. doi:10.1101/lm.7.1.43. ISSN 1072-0502. PMID 10706601.
- ↑ Asztalos, Zoltan; Baba, Kotaro; Yamamoto, Daisuke; Tully, Tim (2007-01-01). "The fickle Mutation of a Cytoplasmic Tyrosine Kinase Effects Sensitization but not Dishabituation in Drosophila Melanogaster". Journal of Neurogenetics 21 (1): 59–71. doi:10.1080/01677060701249488. ISSN 0167-7063. PMID 17464798.
- ↑ "Auditory stimulation dishabituates anti-predator escape behavior in hermit crabs (Coenobita clypeatus)". http://pigeonrat.psych.ucla.edu/resources/6/Stahlman%20et%20al%20Auditory%20stim%20dishabituation%20in%20hermit%20crabs%20BP%202011.pdf.
- ↑ "High intensity exercise as a dishabituating stimulus restores counterregulatory responses in recurrently hypoglycemic rodents". http://discovery.dundee.ac.uk/portal/files/12144486/Dishabituation_Diabetes_Accepted_Manuscript_2017.pdf.
- ↑ Hauser, Marc D.; Konishi, Mark (2017-03-29) (in en). The Design of Animal Communication. MIT Press. ISBN 9780262582230. https://books.google.com/books?id=caf8iat1C7sC&q=dishabituation&pg=PA568.
- ↑ "The Costs and Benefits of Mind-Wandering: A Review". http://www.apa.org/pubs/journals/features/cep-a0031569.pdf.
- ↑ Kavsek, Michael (2004). "Predicting Later IQ from Infant Visual Habituation and Dishabituation: A Meta-Analysis" (in en). Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 25 (3): 369–393. doi:10.1016/j.appdev.2004.04.006. ISSN 0193-3973. https://eric.ed.gov/?id=EJ731762.
External links
- Video of a habituated rat demonstrating dishabituation
- Olfactory Habituation and Dishabituation experiment conducted at Stanford Medicine
- Generalization and dishabituation of the orienting response to a stimulus of lower intensity
- Brody, Leslie R.; Zelazo, Philip R.; Chaika, Helene (1984). "Habituation-Dishabituation to speech in the neonate". Developmental Psychology 20: 114–119. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.20.1.114. http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/download?doi=10.1.1.1012.8344&rep=rep1&type=pdf.
- Habituation and dishabituation to speech and office noise
- Advances in Child Development and Behavior (vol. 6)
Original source: https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Dishabituation.
Read more |