Biology:Memory improvement

From HandWiki
Short description: Act of improving one's memory
The hippocampus regulates memory function.

Memory improvement is the act of enhancing one's memory. Research on improving memory is driven by amnesia, age-related memory loss, and people’s desire to enhance their memory. Research involved in memory improvement has also worked to determine what factors influence memory and cognition. There are many different techniques to improve memory some of which include cognitive training, psychopharmacology, diet, stress management, and exercise. Each technique can improve memory in different ways.

Memory function factors

Neuroplasticity

Neuroplasticity is the mechanism by which the brain encodes experience, learns new behaviors, and can relearn behaviors lost due to brain damage.[1]

London Taxicab

Experience-dependent neuroplasticity suggests that the brain changes in response to experiences. London taxicab drivers provide a great example of this dynamic. They undergo extensive training for 2–4 years, learning and memorizing street names, layout of streets within the city and the quickest cross-city routes. After studying London taxicab drivers over a period of time, it was found that the grey matter volume increased over time in the posterior hippocampus, an area in the brain involved heavily in memory. The longer taxi drivers navigated the streets of London, it was found that they had more gray matter volume in their posterior hippocampus. This suggests a correlation between mental training or exercise and the brains capacity to manage greater volume and more complex information. The increase in volume led to a decrease in the taxi drivers' ability to acquire new visuo-spatial information.[2]

Stress

Research has found that chronic and acute stress have adverse effects on memory processing systems. Therefore, it is important to find mechanisms in which one can reduce the amount of stress in their lives when seeking to improve memory.

  • Chronic stress has been shown to have negative impacts on the brain, especially in memory processing systems.[3] The hippocampus is vulnerable to repeated stress due to adrenal steroid stress hormones.[4] Elevated glucocorticoids, a class of adrenal steroid hormones, results in increased cortisol, a well known stress response hormone in the brain,[5] and glucocorticoids are known to affect memory.[6] Prolonged high cortisol levels, as seen in chronic stress, have been shown to result in reduced hippocampal volume as well as deficits in hippocampal-dependent memory, as seen in impaired declarative, episodic, spatial, and contextual memory performance.[6] Chronic, long-term high cortisol levels affect the degree of hippocampal atrophy, resulting in as much as a 14% hippocampal volume reduction and impaired hippocampus-dependent memory when compared to elderly subjects with decreased or moderate cortisol levels.[6][7][8] Relative to other brain regions, the hippocampus has a high concentration of glucocorticoid receptors. An example may be found in the London taxi drivers, as the anterior hippocampus was hypothesized to decrease in volume as a result of elevated cortisol levels from stress.[2][nb 1]
  • Acute stress, a more common form of stress, results in the release of adrenal steroids resulting in impaired short-term and working memory processes such as selective attention, memory consolidation, as well as long-term potentiation.[9][10] The human brain has a limited short-term memory capacity to process information, which results in constant competition between stimuli to become processed. Cognitive control processes such as selective attention reduce this competition by prioritizing where attentional resources are distributed. Attention is crucial in memory processing and enhances encoding and strength of memory traces.[11] It is therefore important to selectively attend to relevant information and ignore irrelevant information in order to have the greatest success at remembering.[12]
Animal and human studies provide evidence as they report that acute stress impairs the maintenance of short-term memory and working memory and aggravates neuropsychiatric disorders involved in short-term and working memory such as depression and schizophrenia.[3] Animal studies with rats have also shown that exposure to acute stress reduces the survival of hippocampal neurons.[13]
One of the roles of the central nervous system (CNS) is to help adapt to stressful environments.[3] It has been suggested that acute stress may have a protective function for individuals more vulnerable to their own stress hormones. Some individuals, for example, are not able to decrease or habituate their cortisol elevation, which plays a major role in hippocampal atrophy.[14] This over-response of the central nervous system to stress therefore causes maladaptive chronic stress-like effects to memory processing systems.[3]

Strategies

Cognitive training

Discovering that the brain can change as a result of experience has resulted in the development of cognitive training. Cognitive training improves cognitive functioning, which can increase working memory capacity and improve cognitive skills and functions in clinical populations with working memory deficiencies.[15] Cognitive training may focus on attention, speed of processing, neurofeedback, dual-tasking and perceptual training.[15]

Cognitive training has been shown to improve cognitive abilities for up to five years. In one experiment, the goal was to prove that cognitive training would increase the cognitive functions in older adults by using three types of training (memory, reasoning and speed of processing). It was found that improvements in cognitive ability not only was maintained over time but had a positive transfer effect on everyday functioning. Therefore, these results indicate that each type of cognitive training can produce immediate and lasting improvements in each kind of cognitive ability, thus suggesting that training can be beneficial to improving memory.[16]

Cognitive training in areas other than memory has actually been seen to generalize and transfer to memory systems. For example, the Improvement in Memory with Plasticity-based Adaptive Cognitive Training (IMPACT) study by the American Geriatrics Society in 2009 demonstrated that cognitive training designed to improve accuracy and speed of the auditory system presented improvements in memory and attention system functioning as well as auditory functioning.[17]

Human Brain

Two cognitive training methods are:

  • Strategy training is used to help individuals remember increasing amounts of information of a particular type. It involves teaching effective approaches to encoding, maintenance, and/or recall from working memory. The main goal of strategy training is to increase performance in tasks requiring retention of information. Studies strongly support the claim that the amount of information remembered can be increased by rehearsing out loud, telling a story with stimuli, or using imagery to make stimuli stand out. Strategy training has been used in children with Down syndrome and also in older adult populations.[15]
  • Core training involves repetition of demanding working memory tasks. Some core training programs involve a combination of several tasks with widely varying stimulus types. The diversity of exercises increase the chance that one of, or some combination of the training tasks, will produce desired training-related gains. A goal of cognitive training is to impact the ease and success of cognitive performance in one's daily life. Core training can reduce the symptoms of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and improve the quality of life involving patients with multiple sclerosis, schizophrenia and also, those who have suffered from stroke.[15]

The manner in which a training study is conducted could affect the outcomes or perception of the outcomes. Expectancy/effort effects occur when the experimenter subconsciously influences the participants to perform a desired result. One form of expectancy bias relates to placebo effects, which is the belief that training should have a positive influence on cognition. A control group may help to eliminate this bias because this group would not expect to benefit from the training. Researchers sometimes generalize their results, which can be misleading and incorrect. An example is to generalize findings of a single task and interpret the observed improvements as a broadly defined cognitive ability. The study may result in inconsistency if there are a variety of comparison groups used in working memory training, which is impacted by: training and assessment timeline, assessment conditions, training setting and control group selection.[15]

The Five x Five System is a set of memory enhancement tools that are scientifically validated. The system was created by Dr. Peter Marshall for research purposes at Royal Holloway, University of London. The system involves 5 groups of 5 tactics designed to maximize storage and recall at each stage of the process of registering, short-term storage, long-term storage, consolidation and retrieval and was designed to test efficacy of including memory training in school curricula. Each section is of equal text length so that it can be taught verbatim in the same amount of time by all competent teachers.[18]

Personal Application & Intellectual Conception

Generation effect

Generation effect relies on the involvement of the individual in creating/generating their own study materials in order to enhance encoding and long-term retrieval. [19] Though, the underlying mechanisms of the generation effect are not fully understood, a analysis concluded that the effect is real.[20]

Testing effect

Testing effect is a derivative of generation effect as it involves generating the self-testing material. Moreover, it is known that repeatedly testing yourself enhances encoding, thus improving memory.[19] Testing Effect is when most of the learning is allocated to declarative knowledge long term memory is enhanced.[21] In order to retrieve information from your memory you must practice doing it.[22]  The more frequent practicing memorizing the more capable and likely you are to remember it later.[22] The development of an effective retrieval structure that makes it easier to access information that has been stored in long-term memory is facilitated by using repeated retrieval practice.[21] Testing effect occurs because of  the development of an adequate retrieval structure.[21] The testing effect is different from re-reading because the information being learned is being practiced and tested which forces the information to be drawn from memory to recall.[22] The testing effect allows for information to be recalled over a longer period as it is used as a self-testing tool and aids in having the ability to recall information in the future.[23] This strategy is effective when using memory recall especially for information that is being tested on and needs to be in long-term memory.[21]

Spacing effect

Taking scheduled breaks, and doing short study sessions has proven to be more helpful for memory compared to one long study session. It is also known that memory can be improved by getting quality sleep after learning.[19] [24] Longer breaks between study sessions have been associated with better learning and retention. Encountering previously learned information after a break, helps improve long-term learning as well, not just short-term retention.[25]

Illusion of learning

Illusion of learning should be avoided to achieve best outcomes. Some learning and studying strategies people use may seem more effective than they actually are. This creates a problem where the individual thinks they know the material, when they don't necessarily. This could be caused by fluency and the familiarity effect. As people reread the material over and over, it becomes easier to read, creating a sense of fluency. However, this fluency does not indicate that encoding and/or retrieval of the material is being enhanced. Familiarity effects creates illusion of learning, as when the individual recognizes a word or concept to be familiar, they may interpret that as knowing and understanding the material.[19]

State-Dependent Learning

Retrieval is known to be improved when the environment/mood state that the encoding happened in, matches the environment/mood state at the time of retrieval. [26]

Concept Maps “are diagrams that link word concepts in a fluid manner to central key concepts.” [21] They center around a main topic or idea, with lines protruding from the center with related information.[27] Other concepts and ideas are then written at the end of each of the lines with new, related information. These related ideas are usually one or two words in length, giving only the essence of what is needed for memory retrieval.[21] Related ideas can also be drawn at the ends of the lines. This may be especially useful, given the drawing effect (people remember images better than words).[28] These diagrams are beneficial because they require the creator to link and integrate different ideas, which improve critical thinking and leads to more meaningful learning.[29] Concept maps also help to facilitate the storage of material in long term memory, as well as help to show visually any knowledge gaps that may be present.[21] Concept maps have been shown to improve people's ability to complete novel problem solving tasks.[30]

The Drawing Effect is another way to improve memory. Studies show that images are better remembered than words, something that is now known as the picture-superiority effect.[28] Furthermore, another study found that when people are studying vocabulary, they remember more when they draw the definition, in comparison to writing it.[31] This is thought to be because drawing uses 3 different types of memory- elaborative, motor, and pictorial.[32] The benefit of using pictures to enhance memory is even seen at an older age, including in dementia patients.[32]

Techniques to improve memory: visual memory

Method of loci is a technique utilized for memory recall when to-be-remembered items are associated with different locations that are well known to the learner.[21] Method of loci is one of the oldest and most effective mnemonics based on visual imagery.[21] The more you exercise your visual memory through using objects to recall information better memory recall you will have.[33] The locations that are utilized when using the method of loci aids in the effectiveness of memory recall.[21] For example, using the location of a driving route to work is more effective than using a room within a home because items in a room can be moved around while a route to work is more constant without items being moved around.[21] There are limitations when using method of loci, it is difficult to recall any given item without working your way through the list sequence, which can be time consuming.[21] Another limitation is that it is not useful when an individual is trying to learn and remember the real world.[21] This mnemonic technique plus others are effective because they allow the learner to apply their own knowledge to enhance their memory recall.[21]

Psychopharmacology

Psychopharmacology is the scientific study of the actions of drugs and their effects on mood, sensation, thought, and behavior.

Evidence that aspects of memory can be improved by action on selective neurotransmitter systems, such as the cholinergic system which releases acetylcholine, has possible therapeutic benefits for patients with cognitive disorders.[34]

Findings from studies have indicated that acute administration of nicotine can improve cognitive performance (particularly tasks that require attention), short-term episodic memory and prospective memory task performance. Chronic usage of low-dose nicotine in animals has been found to increase the number of neuronal nicotinic acetylcholine receptors (nAChRs) and improve performance on learning and memory tasks.[35]

Short-term nicotine treatment, utilizing nicotine skin patches, have shown that it may be possible to improve cognitive performance in a variety of groups such as normal non-smoking adults, Alzheimer's disease patients, schizophrenics, and adults with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder.[36] Similarly, evidence suggests that smoking improves visuospatial working memory impairments in schizophrenic patients, possibly explaining the high rate of tobacco smoking found in people with schizophrenia.[37]

Stress management

Meditation:attending to a flame

Meditation, a form of mental training to focus attention,[12] has been shown to increase the control over brain resource distribution, improving both attention and self-regulation.[13] The changes are potentially long-lasting as meditation may have the ability to strengthen neuronal circuits as selective attentional processes improve.[38] Meditation may also enhance cognitive limited capacity, affecting the way in which stimuli are processed.[12]

Meditation practice has also been associated with physical changes in brain structure. Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) of Buddhist insight meditation practitioners who practiced mindfulness meditation were found to have an increase in cortical thickness and hippocampus volume compared to the control group.[39] This research provides structural evidence that meditation practice promotes neural plasticity and experience-dependent cortical plasticity.[40] Mindfulness, also known to assist in furthering openness to experiences out of curiosity, interest and acceptance,[41] can increase one's capacity to attend and increase awareness in a moment's experience. Research illustrates that this technique for stress management can increase memory by doing so, allowing for influence on stress processing pathways in the amygdala and prefrontal cortex.[42] In concordance, mindfulness meditation works in association with the sympathetic nervous system (SNS) to regulate the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) system and the sympathomedullary pathway (SAM) to maintain homeostasis on stress reactive physiology.[43]

Exercise

In both human and animal studies, exercise has been shown to improve cognitive performance on encoding and retrieval tasks. Morris water maze and radial arm water maze studies of rodents found that, when compared to sedentary animals, exercised mice showed improved performance traversing the water maze and displayed enhanced memory for the location of an escape platform.[44] Likewise, human studies have shown that cognitive performance is improved due to physiological arousal, which speeded mental processes and enhanced memory storage and retrieval.[45] Ongoing exercise interventions have been found to favorably impact memory processes in older adults[46] and children.[47]

Exercise has been found to positively regulate hippocampal neurogenesis,[48] which is considered an explanation for the positive influence of physical activities on memory performance. Hippocampus-dependent learning, for example, can promote the survival of newborn neurons which may serve as a foundation for the formation of new memories.[49] Exercise has been found to increase the level of brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) protein in rats, with elevated BDNF levels corresponding with strengthened performance on memory tasks. Data also suggests that BDNF availability at the beginning of cognitive testing is related to the overall acquisition of a new cognitive task and may be important in determining the strength of recall in memory tasks.[44]

A meta-analysis concluded that specifically resistance training, as compared to cardiovascular exercise, had no measurable effect on working memory.[50]

There is some evidence that also shows that the amount of effort put into exercising is positively correlated with the level of cognitive performance after working out both in the short term and long term.[51]

Mental exercise

Aristotle wrote a treatise about memory: De memoria et reminiscentia. To improve recollection, he advised that a systematic search should be made and that practice was helpful. He suggested grouping the items to be remembered in threes and then concentrating upon the central member of each triad (group of three).[52]

Music playing has recently gained attention as a possible way to promote brain plasticity. Promising results have been found suggesting that learning music can improve various aspects of memory. For instance, children who participated in one year of instrumental musical training showed improved verbal memory, whereas no such improvement was shown in children who discontinued musical training.[53] Similarly, adults with no previous musical training who participated in individualized piano instruction showed significantly improved performance on tasks designed to test attention and working memory compared to a healthy control group.[54] Evidence suggests that the improvements to verbal, working and long-term memory associated to musical training are a result of the enhanced verbal rehearsal mechanisms musicians possess.[55]

Another study tested elderly participants in how learning a new activity impacts their memory and mental control.[56] They were divided into 5 groups that each spent 15 hours a week doing one of 5 different scenarios: learning digital photography, learning to quilt, learning both digital photography and how to quilt, socializing with others, or doing solitary activities by themselves. It was found that all groups improved with regard to mental control, however learning a new skill(s) led to improved episodic memory.[56]

Memory aids

String around finger used as a memory aid

Physical memory aids, typically worn on the wrist or finger, can help the user remember something they might otherwise forget. Common aids such as this are used by people with memory loss. Typical memory aids for people with Alzheimer's includes sticky notes and color-coded memory aids.[57] Tying a string around one's finger to remember something important is both a literary device,[58] and an actual practice.[59] One school yearbook from 1849 suggests using either a string tied around a finger or a knot tied in the corner of a handkerchief to remember something important to the student.[60] The oldest documented legend of a string used as a memory aid was in the myth Ariadne's thread, where a thread was presented by Ariadne to her lover Theseus to find his way out of the minotaur's labyrinth. The knot-in-the-handkerchief memory aid was used by German philosopher Martin Heidegger.[61]

Memory clamp in use to remember a small child in the back seat of a car on a hot day.

A memory clamp (also called a "reality clamp") is a generic name for a type of physical memory aid designed be worn on the wrist or finger to help the user remember something they might otherwise forget, and was originally invented by physicist Rick Yukon to create difficult-to-ignore visuals and a deliberately intrusive shape and size.[62][63] (For example, a child in a car seat, an important meeting, or the need to take one's own medicine.) A well designed memory clamp is designed to be difficult to ignore visually, typically with bright colors and sometimes contrasting base colors. A memory clamp is designed to cause a slight amount of visual discomfort and a slight amount of physical discomfort, so that the user maintains at least partial awareness of the intrusion, and is thus designed to be worn only intermittently, so the user doesn't become accustomed to it.[62]

Other memory methods include writing on one's own hand, sending a text message to oneself, or using sticky notes.[64] Wrist-worn, finger-worn and ankle-worn memory aids have apparently been used for hundreds of years.[65]

See also

Notes

  1. Quote from Maguire: "Increases in plasma or salivary cortisol are often used as a biochemical marker of stress. Elevated cortisol levels have been associated with poor performance on memory tasks (Newcomer et al., 1999; Cho et al., 2000) and reduced hippocampal volume (Lupien et al., 1998). Thus, stress may have induced the anterior hippocampal volume reduction in the taxi drivers."[2]

References

  1. Kleim, JA., & Jones, TA. (2008). Principles of experience-dependent neural plasticity: implications for rehabilitation after brain damage. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 51, S225-S239.
  2. 2.0 2.1 2.2 Maguire, EA., Woollett, K., & Spiers, HJ. (2006). London taxi drivers and bus drivers: a structural MRI and neuropsychological analysis. Hippocampus, 16, 1091-1101.
  3. 3.0 3.1 3.2 3.3 Mizoguchi, Kazushige; Yuzurihara, Mitsutoshi; Ishige, Atsushi; Sasaki, Hiroshi; Chui, De-Hua; Tabira, Takeshi (2000). "Chronic Stress Induces Impairment of Spatial Working Memory Because of Prefrontal Dopaminergic Dysfunction". The Journal of Neuroscience 20 (4): 1568–1574. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-04-01568.2000. PMID 10662846. 
  4. Jacobson, Lauren; Sapolsky, Robert (1991). "The Role of the Hippocampus in Feedback Regulation of the Hypothalamic-Pituitary-Adrenocortical Axis". Endocrine Reviews 12 (2): 118–134. doi:10.1210/edrv-12-2-118. PMID 2070776. 
  5. Starkman, Monica N.; Gebarski, Stephen S.; Berent, Stanley; Schteingart, David E. (1992). "Hippocampal formation volume, memory dysfunction, and cortisol levels in patients with Cushing's syndrome". Biological Psychiatry 32 (9): 756–765. doi:10.1016/0006-3223(92)90079-f. PMID 1450290. https://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/handle/2027.42/29769/0000107.pdf?sequence=1&isAllowed=y. 
  6. 6.0 6.1 6.2 McEwen, Bruce S. (1999). "Stress and Hippocampal Plasticity". Annual Review of Neuroscience 22: 105–122. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.22.1.105. PMID 10202533. 
  7. Squire, Larry R. (1992). "Memory and the hippocampus: A synthesis from findings with rats, monkeys, and humans". Psychological Review 99 (2): 195–231. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.99.2.195. PMID 1594723. http://whoville.ucsd.edu/PDFs/188_Squire_PsychRev_1992.pdf. 
  8. Sapolsky, Robert M.; Krey, Lewis C.; McEwen, Bruce S. (1986). "The Neuroendocrinology of Stress and Aging: The Glucocorticoid Cascade Hypothesis". Endocrine Reviews 7 (3): 284–301. doi:10.1210/edrv-7-3-284. PMID 3527687. 
  9. Lupien, S.; McEwen, B. (1997). "The acute effects of corticosteroids on cognition: Integration of animal and human model studies". Brain Research Reviews 24 (1): 1–27. doi:10.1016/s0165-0173(97)00004-0. PMID 9233540. 
  10. McEwen, Bruce S. (2006). "Protective and damaging effects of stress mediators: Central role of the brain". Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience 8 (4): 367–381. doi:10.31887/DCNS.2006.8.4/bmcewen. PMID 17290796. 
  11. Shiffrin, R.M. (1976). "Capacity limitations in information processing, attention, and memory". In W.K. Estes (Ed.), Handbook of learning and cognitive processes (Vol. 4). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. doi:10.4324/9781315770338. ISBN:9781315770338
  12. 12.0 12.1 12.2 Slagter, Heleen A.; Lutz, Antoine; Greischar, Lawrence L.; Francis, Andrew D.; Nieuwenhuis, Sander; Davis, James M.; Davidson, Richard J. (2007). "Mental Training Affects Distribution of Limited Brain Resources". PLOS Biology 5 (6): e138. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.0050138. PMID 17488185. 
  13. 13.0 13.1 Tang, Yi-Yuan; Ma, Yinghua; Wang, Junhong; Fan, Yaxin; Feng, Shigang; Lu, Qilin; Yu, Qingbao; Sui, Danni et al. (2007). "Short-term meditation training improves attention and self-regulation". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 104 (43): 17152–17156. doi:10.1073/pnas.0707678104. PMID 17940025. Bibcode2007PNAS..10417152T. 
  14. Kirschbaum, Clemens; Prussner, Jens C.; Stone, Arthur A.; Federenko, Ilona; Gaab, Jens; Lintz, Doris; Schommer, Nicole; Hellhammer, Dirk H. (1995). "Persistent High Cortisol Responses to Repeated Psychological Stress in a Subpopulation of Healthy Men". Psychosomatic Medicine 57 (5): 468–474. doi:10.1097/00006842-199509000-00009. PMID 8552738. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/14647264. 
  15. 15.0 15.1 15.2 15.3 15.4 Morrison, AB., & Chein, JM. (2010). Does working memory training work? the promise and challenges of enhancing cognition by training working memory. Psychonomic Society, Inc.
  16. Willis, Sherry L.; Tennstedt, Sharon L.; Marsiske, Michael; Ball, Karlene; Elias, Jeffrey; Koepke, Kathy Mann; Morris, John N.; Rebok, George W. et al. (2006). "Long-term Effects of Cognitive Training on Everyday Functional Outcomes in Older Adults". JAMA 296 (23): 2805–2814. doi:10.1001/jama.296.23.2805. PMID 17179457. 
  17. Smith, Glenn E.; Housen, Patricia; Yaffe, Kristine; Ruff, Ronald; Kennison, Robert F.; Mahncke, Henry W.; Zelinski, Elizabeth M. (2009). "A Cognitive Training Program Based on Principles of Brain Plasticity: Results from the Improvement in Memory with Plasticity-based Adaptive Cognitive Training (IMPACT) Study". Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 57 (4): 594–603. doi:10.1111/j.1532-5415.2008.02167.x. PMID 19220558. 
  18. Marshall, P (2012) Improving Your Memory: The Unique 5 x 5 System, Bexley-on-Sea. Oakley Books ISBN:978-0-9569784-6-2
  19. 19.0 19.1 19.2 19.3 Goldstein, Bruce (2021). Cognitive psychology:connecting mind, research, and everyday experience (5th ed.). Cengage. ISBN 978-1-337-40827-1. 
  20. Bertsch, Sharon; Pesta, Bryan J.; Wiscott, Richard; McDaniel, Michael A. (2007). "The generation effect: A meta-analytic review". Memory & Cognition 35 (2): 201–210. doi:10.3758/BF03193441. PMID 17645161. 
  21. 21.00 21.01 21.02 21.03 21.04 21.05 21.06 21.07 21.08 21.09 21.10 21.11 21.12 21.13 Baddeley, Alan D.; Eysenck, Michael W.; Anderson, Michael C. (2009). Memory. Hove [England]: Psychology Press. ISBN 978-1-84872-000-8. OCLC 237880710. 
  22. 22.0 22.1 22.2 Lang, James M. (2016). Small teaching : everyday lessons from the science of learning. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. ISBN 978-1-118-94449-3. OCLC 955939138. 
  23. Madigan, Robert. (2015). How memory works-and how to make it work for you. Guilford Publications. ISBN 978-1-4625-2037-4. OCLC 958867556. 
  24. Goldstein, Bruce (2021). Cognitive psychology:connecting mind, research, and everyday experience (5th ed.). Cengage. ISBN 978-1-337-40827-1. 
  25. Carpenter, Shana K.; Cepeda, Nicholas J.; Rohrer, Doug; Kang, Sean H. K.; Pashler, Harold (2012). "Using Spacing to Enhance Diverse Forms of Learning: Review of Recent Research and Implications for Instruction". Educational Psychology Review 24 (3): 369–378. doi:10.1007/s10648-012-9205-z. 
  26. Goldstein, Bruce (2021). Cognitive psychology:connecting mind, research, and everyday experience (5th ed.). Cengage. ISBN 978-1-337-40827-1. 
  27. Budd, John W. (2010). "Mind Maps As Classroom Exercises". The Journal of Economic Education 35: 35–46. doi:10.3200/JECE.35.1.35-46. 
  28. 28.0 28.1 Childers, Terry L.; Houston, Michael J. (1984). "Conditions for a Picture-Superiority Effect on Consumer Memory". Journal of Consumer Research 11 (2): 643–654. doi:10.1086/209001. 
  29. D'Antoni, Anthony V.; Zipp, Genevieve Pinto; Olson, Valerie G.; Cahill, Terrence F. (2010). "Does the mind map learning strategy facilitate information retrieval and critical thinking in medical students?". BMC Medical Education 10 (1): 61. doi:10.1186/1472-6920-10-61. PMID 20846442. 
  30. Novak, Joseph D. (1990). "Concept maps and Vee diagrams: two metacognitive tools to facilitate meaningful learning". Instructional Science 19 (1): 29–52. doi:10.1007/BF00377984. 
  31. Wammes, Jeffrey D.; Meade, Melissa E.; Fernandes, Myra A. (2017). "Learning terms and definitions: Drawing and the role of elaborative encoding". Acta Psychologica 179: 104–113. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2017.07.008. PMID 28756291. 
  32. 32.0 32.1 Fernandes, Myra A.; Wammes, Jeffrey D.; Meade, Melissa E. (2018). "The Surprisingly Powerful Influence of Drawing on Memory". Current Directions in Psychological Science 27 (5): 302–308. doi:10.1177/0963721418755385. 
  33. Kay, David (1888). Memory : what it is and how to improve it. D. Appleton and Co. OCLC 881360195. 
  34. Robbins, T. W. (2000). "NEUROSCIENCE: Boosting Working Memory". Science 290 (5500): 2275–2276. doi:10.1126/science.290.5500.2275. PMID 11188728. 
  35. Heishman, S. J.; Kleykamp, B. A.; Singleton, E. G. (2010). "Meta-analysis of the acute effects of nicotine and smoking on human performance". Psychopharmacology 210 (4): 453–469. doi:10.1007/s00213-010-1848-1. PMID 20414766. 
  36. Swan, Gary E.; Lessov-Schlaggar, Christina N. (2007). "The Effects of Tobacco Smoke and Nicotine on Cognition and the Brain". Neuropsychology Review 17 (3): 259–273. doi:10.1007/s11065-007-9035-9. PMID 17690985. 
  37. Sacco, Kristi A.; Termine, Angelo; Seyal, Aisha; Dudas, Melissa M.; Vessicchio, Jennifer C.; Krishnan-Sarin, Suchitra; Jatlow, Peter I.; Wexler, Bruce E. et al. (2005). "Effects of Cigarette Smoking on Spatial Working Memory and Attentional Deficits in Schizophrenia". Archives of General Psychiatry 62 (6): 649–659. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.62.6.649. PMID 15939842. 
  38. Xiong, Glen L.; Doraiswamy, P. Murali (2009). "Does Meditation Enhance Cognition and Brain Plasticity?". Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1172 (1): 63–69. doi:10.1196/annals.1393.002. PMID 19743551. Bibcode2009NYASA1172...63X. http://dl.dropbox.com/u/5317066/2009-xiong.pdf. 
  39. Luders, Eileen; Toga, Arthur W.; Lepore, Natasha; Gaser, Christian (2009). "The underlying anatomical correlates of long-term meditation: Larger hippocampal and frontal volumes of gray matter". NeuroImage 45 (3): 672–678. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.12.061. PMID 19280691. 
  40. Lazar, Sara W.; Kerr, Catherine E.; Wasserman, Rachel H.; Gray, Jeremy R.; Greve, Douglas N.; Treadway, Michael T.; McGarvey, Metta; Quinn, Brian T. et al. (2005). "Meditation experience is associated with increased cortical thickness". NeuroReport 16 (17): 1893–1897. doi:10.1097/01.wnr.0000186598.66243.19. PMID 16272874. 
  41. Quaglia, J. T.; Brown, K. W.; Lindsay, E. K.; Creswell, J. D.; Goodman, R. J.. "From conceptualization to operationalization of mindfulness". Handbook of mindfulness: Theory, research, and practice. New York: Guilford Press. 
  42. Holzel, B. K.; Hoge, E. A.; Greve, D. N.; Gard, T.; Creswell, J. D.; Brown, K. W.; Lazar, S. W. (2013). "Neural mechanisms of symptom improvements in generalized anxiety disorder following mindfulness training". Neuroimage: Clinical 2: 448–458. doi:10.1016/j.nicl.2013.03.011. PMID 24179799. 
  43. Creswell, J. David; Lindsay, Emily K. (2014). "How Does Mindfulness Training Affect Health? A Mindfulness Stress Buffering Account". Current Directions in Psychological Science 23 (6): 401–407. doi:10.1177/0963721414547415. 
  44. 44.0 44.1 Berchtold, N.C.; Castello, N.; Cotman, C.W. (2010). "Exercise and time-dependent benefits to learning and memory". Neuroscience 167 (3): 588–597. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.02.050. PMID 20219647. 
  45. Lambourne, Kate; Tomporowski, Phillip (2010). "The effect of exercise-induced arousal on cognitive task performance: A meta-regression analysis". Brain Research 1341: 12–24. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2010.03.091. PMID 20381468. 
  46. Colcombe, Stanley; Kramer, Arthur F. (2003). "Fitness Effects on the Cognitive Function of Older Adults". Psychological Science 14 (2): 125–130. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.t01-1-01430. PMID 12661673. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/10833611. 
  47. Hillman, Charles H.; Castelli, Darla M.; Buck, Sarah M. (2005). "Aerobic Fitness and Neurocognitive Function in Healthy Preadolescent Children". Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise 37 (11): 1967–1974. doi:10.1249/01.mss.0000176680.79702.ce. PMID 16286868. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/7481314. 
  48. Van Praag, Henriette; Kempermann, Gerd; Gage, Fred H. (1999). "Running increases cell proliferation and neurogenesis in the adult mouse dentate gyrus". Nature Neuroscience 2 (3): 266–270. doi:10.1038/6368. PMID 10195220. 
  49. Van Der Borght, Karin; Havekes, Robbert; Bos, Thomas; Eggen, Bart J. L.; Van Der Zee, Eddy A. (2007). "Exercise improves memory acquisition and retrieval in the Y-maze task: Relationship with hippocampal neurogenesis". Behavioral Neuroscience 121 (2): 324–334. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.121.2.324. PMID 17469921. https://www.rug.nl/research/portal/files/3623253/2007BehavNeuroscivdBorght.pdf. 
  50. "Lifting cognition: a meta-analysis of effects of resistance exercise on cognition". Psychol Res 84 (5): 1167–1183. 2019. doi:10.1007/s00426-019-01145-x. PMID 30627769. https://www.pure.ed.ac.uk/ws/files/121873388/exercise_accepted_ms_1_.pdf. 
  51. "Regular exercise adds up to big memory boosts". July 2019. https://www.health.harvard.edu/mind-and-mood/regular-exercise-adds-up-to-big-memory-boosts. 
  52. Whitehead, Anne, "Memory and Inscription", Memory, pp. 15–49 
  53. Wan, Catherine Y.; Schlaug, Gottfried (2010). "Music Making as a Tool for Promoting Brain Plasticity across the Life Span". The Neuroscientist 16 (5): 566–577. doi:10.1177/1073858410377805. PMID 20889966. 
  54. Bugos, J. A.; Perlstein, W. M.; McCrae, C. S.; Brophy, T. S.; Bedenbaugh, P. H. (2007). "Individualized Piano Instruction enhances executive functioning and working memory in older adults". Aging & Mental Health 11 (4): 464–471. doi:10.1080/13607860601086504. PMID 17612811. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/6224665. 
  55. Franklin, Michael S.; Sledge Moore, Katherine; Yip, Chun-Yu; Jonides, John; Rattray, Katie; Moher, Jeff (2008). "The effects of musical training on verbal memory". Psychology of Music 36 (3): 353–365. doi:10.1177/0305735607086044. http://tanclab.org/wp-content/uploads/2014/08/7_POM_Franklin_etal2008.pdf. 
  56. 56.0 56.1 Park, Denise C.; Lodi-Smith, Jennifer; Drew, Linda; Haber, Sara; Hebrank, Andrew; Bischof, Gérard N.; Aamodt, Whitley (January 2014). "The Impact of Sustained Engagement on Cognitive Function in Older Adults: The Synapse Project". Psychological Science 25 (1): 103–112. doi:10.1177/0956797613499592. PMID 24214244. 
  57. "Traditional memory aids". https://www.alzheimers.org.uk/get-support/staying-independent/traditional-memory-aids. 
  58. Boys' Life. Boy Scouts of America, Inc.. 1961. https://books.google.com/books?id=9NWHIWE6WIwC&q=string+tied+around+finger+remember+history&pg=PA51. 
  59. Herald and Presbyter. Monfort & Company. 1912. https://books.google.com/books?id=m6CaEvlfU4gC&q=string+tied+around+finger+remember+history&pg=PA20. 
  60. The Ragged School Union Magazine. Partridge & Oakey. 1849. p. 150. https://archive.org/details/bub_gb_6k5AAAAAcAAJ/page/n163. 
  61. Bass, Alan (2017). Fetishism, Psychoanalysis, and Philosophy: The Iridescent Thing. Routledge. ISBN 978-1-351-36865-0. https://books.google.com/books?id=w25ADwAAQBAJ&q=knot+handkerchief+remember&pg=PT74. 
  62. 62.0 62.1 Ukon, Ricky. "Reality Clamp". https://spaceforce7.com/?p=81. 
  63. Ukon, Ricky. "Proclamation of Rick Yukon's Intellectual Property in Reality Clamp discovery". https://spaceforce7.com/?p=87. 
  64. Co, Emily (2012-03-14). "10 Smart Ways to Remember Things". http://www.savvysugar.com/How-Remember-Things-22188563. 
  65. "A String Around Your Finger". 2015-06-11. https://www.myjewishlearning.com/rabbis-without-borders/a-string-around-your-finger/.