Philosophy:Sexual selection in humans
Sexual selection in humans concerns the concept of sexual selection, introduced by Charles Darwin as an element of his theory of natural selection,[1] as it affects humans. Sexual selection is a biological way one sex chooses a mate for the best reproductive success. Most compete with others of the same sex for the best mate to contribute their genome for future generations. This has shaped human evolution for many years, but reasons why humans choose their mates are not fully understood. Sexual selection is quite different in non-human animals than humans as they feel more of the evolutionary pressures to reproduce and can easily reject a mate.[2] The role of sexual selection in human evolution has not been firmly established although neoteny has been cited as being caused by human sexual selection.[3] It has been suggested that sexual selection played a part in the evolution of the anatomically modern human brain, i.e. the structures responsible for social intelligence underwent positive selection as a sexual ornamentation to be used in courtship rather than for survival itself,[4] and that it has developed in ways outlined by Ronald Fisher in the Fisherian runaway model.[5][6][7][8][9] Fisher also stated that the development of sexual selection was "more favourable" in humans.[10]
General hypotheses
Some hypotheses about the evolution of the human brain argue that it is a sexually selected trait, as it would not confer enough fitness in itself relative to its high maintenance costs (a fifth to a quarter of the energy and oxygen consumed by a human).[11] Current consensus about the evolutionary development of the human brain accepts sexual selection as a potential contributing factor but maintains that human intelligence and the ability to store and share cultural knowledge would have likely carried high survival value as well.[12]
Sexual selection's role in human evolution cannot be definitively established, as features may result from an equilibrium among competing selective pressures, some involving sexual selection, others natural selection, and others pleiotropy. Richard Dawkins argued that
- "When you notice a characteristic of an animal and ask what its Darwinian survival value is, you may be asking the wrong question. It could be that the characteristic you have picked out is not the one that matters. It may have "come along for the ride", dragged along in evolution by some other characteristic to which it is pleiotropically linked."[13]
Darwin's sexual selection hypothesis
Charles Darwin described sexual selection as depending on "the advantage which certain individuals have over others of the same sex and species, solely in respect of reproduction".[14] Darwin noted that sexual selection is of two kinds and concluded that both kinds had operated on humans:[15] "The sexual struggle is of two kinds; in the one it is between the individuals of the same sex, generally the male sex, in order to drive away or kill their rivals, the females remaining passive; whilst in the other, the struggle is likewise between the individuals of the same sex, in order to excite or charm those of the opposite sex, generally the females, which no longer remain passive, but select the more agreeable partners."[16]
Charles Darwin conjectured that the male beard, as well as the hairlessness of humans compared to nearly all other mammals, were results of sexual selection. He reasoned that since the bodies of females are more nearly hairless, the loss of fur was due to sexual selection of females at a remote prehistoric time when males had overwhelming selective power, and that it nonetheless affected males due to genetic correlation between the sexes. He also hypothesized that contrasts in sexual selection acting along with natural selection were significant factors in the geographical differentiation in human appearance of some isolated groups, as he did not believe that natural selection alone provided a satisfactory answer. Although not explicit, his observation that in Khoisan women "the posterior part of the body projects in a most wonderful manner" (known as steatopygia)[17] implies sexual selection for this characteristic. In The Descent of Man, and Selection in Relation to Sex, Darwin viewed many physical traits which vary around the world as being so trivial to survival[18] that he concluded some input from sexual selection was required to account for their presence. He noted that variation in these features among the various peoples of the world meant human mate-choice criteria would also have to be quite different if the focus was similar, and he himself doubted that, citing[19] reports indicating that ideals of beauty did not, in fact, vary in this way around the world.
Sexual dimorphism
The effects on the human brain formation during puberty is directly linked to hormones changes. The mismatch timing between biological puberty and age of social maturity in western society has a psychological expectation on children.[20] With puberty, men are generally hairier than women, and Darwin was of the opinion that hairlessness was related to sexual selection; however, several other explanations have been advanced to explain human hairlessness; a leading one being that loss of body hair facilitated sweating.[21] This idea closely relates to that of the suggested need for increased photoprotection and is part of the most-commonly-accepted scientific explanation for the evolution of pigmentary traits.[22]
Sexual dimorphism suggests the presence of sexual selection. The earliest homininae were highly dimorphic and that this tendency lessened over the course of human evolution, suggesting humans have become more monogamous. In contrast, gorillas living in harems exhibit a much stronger sexual dimorphism (see: homininae).[23]
Sexual anatomy
The theory of sexual selection has been used to explain a number of human anatomical features. These include rounded breasts, facial hair, pubic hair and penis size. The breasts of primates are flat, yet are able to produce sufficient milk for feeding their young. The breasts of non-lactating human females are filled with fatty tissue and not milk. Thus it has been suggested the rounded female breasts are signals of fertility.[24] Richard Dawkins has speculated that the loss of the penis bone in humans, when it is present in other primates, may be due to sexual selection by females looking for a clear sign of good health in prospective mates. Since a human erection relies on a hydraulic pumping system, erection failure is a sensitive early warning of certain kinds of physical and mental ill health.[25]
Homo has a thicker penis than the other great apes, though it is on average no longer than the chimpanzee's.[26] It has been suggested the evolution of the human penis towards larger size was the result of female choice rather than sperm competition, which generally favors large testicles.[27] However, penis size may have been subject to natural selection, rather than sexual selection, due to a larger penis' efficiency in displacing the sperm of rival males during sexual intercourse. A model study showed displacement of semen was directly proportional to the depth of pelvic thrusting, as an efficient semen displacement device.[28]
Selection preferences and biological drivers
There are a variety of factors that drive sexual selection in humans. Current available research indicates that selection preferences are biologically driven,[29] that is, by the display of phenotypic traits that can be both consciously and unconsciously evaluated by the opposite sex to determine the health and fertility of a potential mate.[30] This process can be affected, however, by social factors, including in cultures where arranged marriage is practiced, or psychosocial factors, such as valuing certain cultural traits of a mate, including a person's social status, or what is perceived to be an ideal partner in various cultures.[31]
Selection preferences in females
Some of the factors that affect how females select their potential mates for reproduction include voice pitch, facial shape, muscular appearance, and height.[32][33] Several studies suggest that there is a link between hormone levels and partner selection among humans. In a study measuring female attraction to males with varying levels of masculinity, it was established that women had a general masculinity preference for men's voices, and that the preference for masculinity was greater in the fertile phase of the menstrual cycle than in the non-fertile phase.[33] There is further evidence from the same study that in fertile stages of the menstrual cycle, women also had a preference for other masculine traits such as body size, facial shape, and dominant behavior, which are indicators of both fertility and health.[33] This study did not exclude males with feminine traits from being selected, however, as feminine traits in men indicate a higher probability of long-term relationship commitment,[33] and may be one of several evolutionary strategies.[34] Further research also backs up the idea of using phenotypic traits as a means of assessing a potential mate's fitness for reproduction as well as assessing whether a partner has high genetic quality.[35]
Another factor affecting the selection process is the environment which the person inhabits. In biological terms, certain environmental conditions may bring about demands for or the disregarding of certain traits. One such example is a preference for males whose facial structure indicates certain hormonal ratios, such as testosterone-cortisol levels (sex and stress hormones). Research shows that, for example, in countries with varying Human Development Index (HDI) levels, females have different preferences for sex-stress hormone ratios, as expressed in the male's face. A Royal Society research showed a significant correlation between a measure of societal development and preferences for indication of lower testosterone levels, as manifested in facial features, and the interaction between preferences for testosterone and cortisol.[36] It was concluded that societal-level ecological factors impact the valuation of traits by combinations of sex- and stress-hormones.[36]
A 2020 study reported that women tend to find a man more attractive if the man's previous relationships ended mutually, and less attractive if the man was dumped.[37]
Selection preferences in males
Like their female counterparts, males also use visual information about a potential mate, as well as voice, body shape, and an assortment of other factors in selecting a partner. Research shows that males tend to prefer feminine women's faces and voices as opposed to women with masculine features in these categories.[38] Furthermore, males also evaluate skin coloration, symmetry, and apparent health, as a means by which they select a partner for reproductive purposes.[38] Males are particularly attracted to femininity in women's faces when their testosterone levels are at their highest, and the level of attraction to femininity may fluctuate as hormone levels fluctuate.[39] Studies on men have also been done to show the effects of exogenous testosterone and its effects on attraction to femininity, and the results concluded that throughout several studies, men have shown decreased preference for feminine female faces in the long-term context, when given exogenous testosterone, but this difference did not occur with placebo.[40]
Common preferences in either sex
Sexual selection preferences are general terms by which the mating and reproductive process are understood. As one article states, sexual selection is in essence a process which favors sexual displays for attraction, aggressiveness, dominance, size, and strength, and the ability to exclude competitors by force if necessary, or by using resources to win.[41] Both male and female use voice, face, and other physical characteristics[31] to assess a potential mate's ability to reproduce, as well as their health.[30] Together with visual and chemical signals, these crucial characteristics which are likely to enhance the ability to produce offspring, as well as long-term survival prospects, can be assessed and selections made.[29][42]
Sexual selection in males
Contest competition
Contest competition is form of sexual selection in which mating is obtained by using force or the threat of force to exclude same-sex competitors from mates.[43] Male contest competition favors large body size, which is seen in the sexual dimorphism of human males and females.[44] In all living hominid species, males are more muscular, allowing them to have more strength and power. Human males have 61% more overall muscle mass compared to females.[45] This greater muscle mass allows males to gain greater acceleration, speed, and more powerful striking movements.[46] Compared to females, human males exhibit more same-sex aggression, which peaks in young adulthood.[47][48][49][50]
Male contest competition also often favors threat displays, which allow one competitor to submit without a costly fight.[51] Low vocalization fundamental frequencies (perceived as vocal pitch) increase the perception of threat among human males.[52][53][54] Controlling for body size, lower male fundamental frequency relative to females tends to evolve in polygynous anthropoid primates, where males compete more intensely for mates.[55] Chimpanzees and humans have the greatest sexual dimorphism in fundamental frequency of all hominids.[55] Males are also more likely to engage in physical risks in front of competitors, and males who take more physical risks are perceived as being stronger.[56] Status badges such as facial hair are generally related to men being perceived as more dominant.[43] Facial hair makes the jaw appear more prominent and shows emotions like anger clearly which makes a male appear more threatening.[57][58] Dominance has been associated with increased male mating success.[59][60][61]
Often contest competition produces anatomical weapons such as antlers or large canine teeth; however, hominids lack canine weaponry typical of other primates.[43] Reduced canine size may be due to bipedalism and adaptations of the hand.[62][63] Bipedalism is not a common trait, yet many species like the great apes stand on their hind legs when fighting, which increases power behind blows.[64][43] Hominin hands are adapted for gripping tools or hurling objects like stones.[65][66][67][68] Bipedalism and utilizing handheld objects such as weapons may have aided early hominins in contest competition, reducing sexual selection pressures of maintaining large canine teeth.[62][67][69]
Several other traits in human males may have been selected for contest competition. Males exhibit a more robust face compared to females.[43] This may have provided protection against blows to the face during contest competitions as the areas on the skull that have increased robusticity are parts that are more likely to suffer from injury.[70] Additionally, there are 23% more lefthanded males than females.[71] Although left-handedness is heritable and associated with survival disadvantages, the rarity of left-handedness may have given ancestral males a fighting advantage in competitions keeping this trait in the gene pool via negative frequency-dependent selection.[43][72][73][74] Many combat sports such as boxing have higher-than-chance frequencies of left-handed individuals among the top competitors.[75] Human males are also able to tolerate pain longer than females, especially during competition.[43][76][77] A higher pain tolerance allows for males to remain aggressive during contests along with an increased aerobic capacity.[43] Males have an oxygen capacity rate that is 25–30% higher than females.[78][79] This aerobic capacity increases during puberty when males are sexually maturing and preparing to mate.[43]
Human males engage in both within-group contest competition and coalitional aggression.[43] The latter form competition may be supported by males tending to contribute more to a group task when competing against other groups and to discriminate more strongly against outgroup members.[80][81][82][83]
Traits that evolve during contest competition, such as large body size and physical aggression, are often costly to produce and maintain.[84] These traits may therefore be indicators of male genetic quality and/or ability to provide resources and other direct benefits.[84] Consequently, human females may evolve preferences for these traits, which then comprise an additional selection pressure. However, secondary sexual characteristics in human males do not always enhance overall attractiveness to females.[85][86][87] Some traits of human males that function in contests, such as body size, strength, and weaponry usage, may also have been selected to aid in hunting.[43] However, contest competition is observed in all great apes and thus likely preceded hunting as a selective pressure.[43]
Sexual selection in females
Human female mating competition is complex and multifaceted and varies across cultures, societies, and individuals.[88] Females may compete for high-quality mates who possess traits that indicate underlying genetic quality, possibly including physical attractiveness and intelligence,[89] or material resources that can enhance the survival and reproductive success of the female and her offspring.[90][89]
Females may also compete for leadership and reputation in social alliances and networks that can provide support, protection, and mating opportunities.[91][92] Human females compete with other females, sometimes including co-wives, to obtain and retain investment from mates, while managing cooperative same-sex relationships.[93]
Female mate competition
Over human evolution, the cost of aggressive and physical contests in females may have been high given that females were the primary caregivers and protectors of offspring, so a mother's death greatly impacts infant mortality.[91][92] Some behaviors from mothers competing with other females at a similar life stage over resources include self-promotion and competitor derogation.[92] However, maternal competition remains understudied. Compared to male aggression, female aggression tends to be more indirect. Females tend to engage in more subtle and indirect aggression, such as gossip, as a competitive tool to harm same-sex rivals' social opportunities[94] and partake in competitor derogation to prevent female rivals from getting male attention.[92] Gossip, derogation, and social exclusion grant the aggressor the chance to go undetected and avoid retaliation. Derogation, for example, can eliminate same-sex rivals by reducing their ability to compete; it was found that girls' suicide attempts were associated with any amount of indirect peer victimization, whereas only frequent indirect peer victimization was associated with boys' suicide attempts.[92] Furthermore, same-sex harassment in some nonhuman animals impacted females' ovulation capabilities, which suggests that human females' reproductive success could be influenced by the stress induced by indirect or direct peer victimization.[92] Males pursue both sexually attractive and faithful long-term partners, which might be the source of female mating competition greatly revolving around denigrating same-sex rivals' attractiveness and reputation through accusations of promiscuity and infidelity.[92] Competitive women are more likely to spread reputation-harming information about other women, suggesting that reputation manipulation is a form of female competition for romantic partners.[95] Women are more likely to compete for desirable mates when maternal investment levels are high, and their social groups are largely composed of mothers,[96] as more women living closer together are looking for similar resources that benefit their own survival and that of their children.[92]
Sexually selected female traits
Competition for mates among human females may take multiple forms. Contests tend to be less frequent, aggressive, and injurious than male-male contests.[97] This leads to a difference in the traits selected. The indirect aggression in which females engage can take the form of damaging the reputation of other women (e.g., via gossip), potentially influencing their sexual behavior and opportunities.[98] Additionally, females compete with one another through male mate choice, e.g., by enhancing their own physical attractiveness.[98] Some female anatomical traits are targets of male mate choice and possibly represent female sexual ornaments shaped by selection. Femininity in the female face and voice provide cues to female reproductive hormones and reproductive potential.[99] Males tend to have lower pitched voices than females, likely due to male intrasexual competition,[100] but some evidence suggests that high female voice pitch may also be favored by male mate choice and function in intrasexual competition among females.[101]
Deposition of fat on the hips, buttocks, and breasts in human females may also be an outcome of female sexual selection, signaling the ability to support gestation and lactation for offspring in environments where resources may be low.[102][103] However, in the Western World, women with larger breasts are seen as more likely to commit infidelity and more likely to participate in intra-sexual competition with other females.[102] Greater overall body fat percentage in human females appears to be unique among primates and may function in storing resources needed to gestate and support large-brained offspring[104] as well as in sexual selection.[105] For example, higher female body mass index (BMI) is associated with increased fertility in young women, particularly those in subsistence societies.[106] Lower WHR, lower BMI, and smaller waist sizes are also associated with lower birth weights and higher infant mortality.[107] Such traits, particularly body fat distribution, may represent sexual ornamentation, which is important in mating throughout the animal kingdom, for example, in birds.[108][109] Humans also use bodily decoration, including jewelry, tattoos, scarification, and makeup to enhance appearance and desirability to potential mates.[102][110]
In some non-human primates, red coloration may act as a sexual signal.[111][112] For example, female mandrills have varying red facial colors that appear to indicate number of offspring and stage of ovulatory cycle, thereby advertising their apparent fertility.[113] Female rhesus macaques experience changes in facial redness during different times in their cycle, which is used by male macaques in mate choice, which is consistent with the proposal that this characteristic is sexually selected.[114] Red has also been linked to human female sexuality.[111] Some studies show that women who displayed themselves in red on dating websites were more likely to be interested in sex.[111] Research has also shown that men perceive women in red as more sexually attractive than women who were not,[115][116] particularly young, fertile women.[117]
It has also been suggested that women who are nearing ovulation were more likely to be judged as more attractive than their counterparts who were in different stages of their cycle.[118] Facial and vocal attractiveness have been observed to change with estradiol and progesterone in pattens consistent with fertility-related increases,[119] although some data challenge this interpretation.[120] In general, ovulatory cycle changes are more subtle than in non-human primates, perhaps representing leakage of information on fertility and hormonal status rather than signals functioning to convey this information.[121]
Phenotype
Sexual selection has continued to be suggested as a possible explanation for geographical variation in appearance within the human species; in modern hypotheses, marriage practices are proposed as the main determinant of sexual selection. John Manning[122] suggests that where polygyny is common (e.g. as result of women emancipation), men face intense competition for wives and are more likely to be completely unsuccessful in reproducing, and the result is strong selection of males for traits which are adaptive for successful reproduction. He proposes a link to skin color through selection of males for testosterone-mediated traits which confer an ability to successfully compete for females. He suggests testosterone makes the human immune system less competent to resist pathogens. In this view the antimicrobial properties of melanin help mitigate the susceptibility to disease that polygyny induces by increasing testosteronization. According to this argument, the anti-infective qualities of melanin were more important than protection from ultraviolet light in the evolution of the darkest skin types. Manning asserts that skin color is more correlated with the occurrence of polygyny – explicable by it having an antimicrobial function – than the latitudinal gradient in intensity of ultraviolet radiation, and he points to the lack of very dark skin at equatorial latitudes of the New World and the relatively light skin of Khoisan people in Africa.[122][123]
Research seems to contradict Manning's explanation about skin color. In 1978, NASA launched the Total Ozone Mapping Spectrometer, which was able to measure the ultraviolet radiation reaching Earth's surface. Jablonski and Chaplin took the spectrometer's global ultraviolet measurements and compared them with published data on skin color in indigenous populations from more than 50 countries. There was an unmistakable correlation: The weaker the ultraviolet light, the fairer the skin.[124] Rogers et al. (2004) performed an examination of the variation in MC1R nucleotide sequences for people of different ancestry and compared the sequences of chimpanzees and humans from various regions of the Earth. Rogers concluded that, at the time of the evolutionary separation of chimpanzees and humans, the common ancestors of all humans had light skin that was covered by dark hair. Additionally, our closest extant relative, the chimpanzee, has light skin covered by thick body hair.[125] Over time human hair disappeared to allow better heat dissipation through sweating[126] and the skin tone grew darker to increase the epidermal permeability barrier[127] and protect from folate depletion due to the increased exposure to sunlight.[128] When humans started to migrate away from the tropics, there was less-intense sunlight, partly due to clothing to protect against cold weather. Under these conditions there was less photodestruction of folate, and so the evolutionary pressure stopping lighter-skinned gene variants from surviving was reduced. In addition, lighter skin is able to generate more vitamin D (cholecalciferol) than darker skin, so it would have represented a health benefit in reduced sunlight if there were limited sources of vitamin D.[126] The genetic mutations leading to light skin may have experienced selective pressure due to the adoption of farming and settlement in northern latitudes.[129]
Anthropologist Peter Frost has proposed that sexual selection was responsible for the evolution of pigmentary traits of women in Northern and Eastern European populations. He contends that the diversity of hair and eye color in Northeast European populations originated as a consequence of intense female intrasexual competition, and is an adaptation for reproductive success in women.[130][131]
Geoffrey Miller hypothesis
Geoffrey Miller, drawing on some of Darwin's largely neglected ideas about human behavior, has hypothesized that many human behaviors not clearly tied to survival benefits, such as humor, music, visual art, some forms of altruism, verbal creativity, or the fact that most humans have a far greater vocabulary than that which is required for survival, can nevertheless play a role.[132] Miller (2000) has proposed that this apparent redundancy is due to individuals using vocabulary to demonstrate their intelligence, and consequently their "fitness", to potential mates. This has been tested experimentally, and it appears that males do make greater use of lower-frequency (more unusual) words when in a romantic mindset compared to a non-romantic mindset, suggesting that vocabulary is likely to be used as a sexual display (Rosenberg & Tunney, 2008). All these qualities are considered courtship adaptations that have been favored through sexual selection.[133]
Miller is critical of theories that imply that human culture arose as accidents or by-products of human evolution. He believes that human culture arose through sexual selection for creative traits. In that view, many human artifacts could be considered subject to sexual selection as part of the extended phenotype, for instance clothing that enhances sexually selected traits.[2] During human evolution, on at least two occasions, hominid brain size increased rapidly over a short period of time followed by a period of stasis. The first period of brain expansion occurred 2.5 million years ago, when Homo habilis first began using stone tools. The second period occurred 500,000 years ago, with the emergence of archaic Homo sapiens. Miller argues that the rapid increases in brain size would have occurred by a positive feedback loop resulting in a Fisherian runaway selection for larger brains. Tor Nørretranders, in The Generous Man conjectures how intelligence, musicality, artistic and social skills, and language might have evolved as an example of the handicap principle, analogously with the peacock's tail, the standard example of that principle.
Opposing arguments
The role of sexual selection in human evolution has been considered controversial from the moment of publication of Darwin's book on sexual selection (1871). Among his vocal critics were some of Darwin's supporters, such as Alfred Wallace, a believer in spiritualism and a non-material origin of the human mind, who argued that animals and birds do not choose mates based on sexual selection, and that the artistic faculties in humans belong to their spiritual nature and therefore cannot be connected to natural selection, which only affects the animal nature.[10] Darwin was accused of looking to the evolution of early human ancestors through the moral codes of the 19th century Victorian society.
See also
- Human mating strategies
- Hypergamy
- Marriage squeeze
- Parental investment in humans
- Strategic pluralism
References
- ↑ Vogt, Yngve (29 January 2014). "Large testicles are linked to infidelity". Phys.org. http://phys.org/news/2014-01-large-testicles-linked-infidelity.html.
- ↑ 2.0 2.1 Miller G. (2000). The mating mind: how sexual choice shaped the evolution of human nature, London, Heineman, ISBN:0434007412 (also Doubleday, ISBN:0385495161).
- ↑ Brin, David. "Neoteny and Two-Way Sexual Selection in Human Evolution". http://www.davidbrin.com/nonfiction/neoteny1.html.
- ↑ "Sexual Selection and the Mind". https://www.edge.org/conversation/sexual-selection-and-the-mind.
- ↑ Fisher, R.A. (1930) The Genetical Theory of Natural Selection. ISBN:0198504403
- ↑ Edwards, A.W.F. (2000) Perspectives: Anecdotal, Historical and Critical Commentaries on Genetics. The Genetics Society of America (154) 1419:1426
- ↑ Andersson, M. (1994) Sexual selection. ISBN:0691000573
- ↑ Andersson, M. and Simmons, L.W. (2006) Sexual selection and mate choice. Trends, Ecology and Evolution (21) 296:302
- ↑ Gayon, J. (2010) Sexual selection: Another Darwinian process. Comptes Rendus Biologies (333) 134:144
- ↑ 10.0 10.1 Fisher, R. A. (1915). "The evolution of sexual preference". Eugenics Review 7 (3): 184–192. PMID 21259607.
- ↑ Schillaci, M. A. (2006). "Sexual selection and the evolution of brain size in primates". PLOS ONE 1 (1): e62. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0000062. PMID 17183693. Bibcode: 2006PLoSO...1...62S.
- ↑ McElreath, Richard (May 2018). "Sizing up human brain evolution". Nature 557 (7706): 496–497. doi:10.1038/d41586-018-05197-8. PMID 29789743. Bibcode: 2018Natur.557..496M.
- ↑ Richard Dawkins (2009). The Greatest Show on Earth: The Evidence for Evolution. Simon and Schuster. p. 67. ISBN 978-1416597780. https://books.google.com/books?id=CQdDhIgKM4UC&pg=PT67.
- ↑ Darwin, Charles (1871). The Descent of Man and Selection in Relation to Sex. 1 (1st ed.). London: John Murray. p. 256. http://darwin-online.org.uk/content/frameset?pageseq=269&itemID=F937.1&viewtype=side.
- ↑ Darwin, Charles (1871). The Descent of Man and Selection in Relation to Sex. 2 (1st ed.). London: John Murray. p. 402. http://darwin-online.org.uk/content/frameset?pageseq=419&itemID=F937.2&viewtype=sidee.
- ↑ Darwin, Charles (1871). The Descent of Man and Selection in Relation to Sex. 1 (1st ed.). London: John Murray. p. 398. http://darwin-online.org.uk/content/frameset?pageseq=269&itemID=F937.1&viewtype=side.
- ↑ Charles Darwin (1882). The Descent of Man and Selection in Relation to Sex. London: John Murray. p. 578. http://darwin-online.org.uk/content/frameset?itemID=F955&viewtype=text&pageseq=1.
- ↑ Charles Darwin (1882). The Descent of Man, and Selection in Relation to Sex. AMS Press. pp. 605. ISBN 978-0404084097. https://archive.org/details/descentmanandse14darwgoog. "The races of man differ from each other, and from their nearest allies, in certain characters which are of no service to them in their daily habits of life, and which it is extremely probable would have been modified through sexual selection"
- ↑ Darwin, C. (1936) [1888]. The Descent of Man and Selection in Relation to Sex. reprint of 2nd ed., The Modern Library, New York: Random House.
- ↑ Gluckman, P. D.; Hanson, M. A. (25 July 2006). "Changing times: The evolution of puberty". Molecular and Cellular Endocrinology 254–255: 26–31. doi:10.1016/j.mce.2006.04.005. PMID 16713071.
- ↑ Jablonski, N. G. (2006). Skin: a natural history. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press. p. PP13.
- ↑ Jablonski, N. G.; Chaplin, G. (2010). "Human skin pigmentation as an adaptation to UV radiation". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 107 (Suppl 2): 8962–8968. doi:10.1073/pnas.0914628107. PMID 20445093. Bibcode: 2010PNAS..107.8962J.
- ↑ Principles of Human Evolution, by Roger Lewin, Robert Foley. [page needed]
- ↑ Morris, Desmond (2007). "Breasts". The Naked Woman. Macmillan. ISBN 978-0312338534. https://books.google.com/books?id=Wa9zntiEKeAC&pg=PA144.
- ↑ Dawkins, Richard (2006). The Selfish Gene (30th anniversary ed.). p. 158 endnote. "It is not implausible that, with natural selection refining their diagnostic skills, females could glean all sorts of clues about a male's health, and the robustness of his ability to cope with stress, from the tone and bearing of his penis."
- ↑ Dixson, A. F. (2009). Sexual selection and the origins of human mating systems. Oxford University Press. pp. 61–65. ISBN 978-0191569739. https://books.google.com/books?id=VRTniKE2liYC.
- ↑ Miller, G.F. (1998), "How mate choice shaped human nature: A review of sexual selection and human evolution" in Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology.
- ↑ In a theoretical paper[Usurped!] published in the journal Evolutionary Psychology in 2004, Gallup and coauthor, Rebecca Burch, conjecture that, "A longer penis would not only have been an advantage for leaving semen in a less-accessible part of the vagina, but by filling and expanding the vagina, it also would aid and abet the displacement of semen left by other males as a means of maximizing the likelihood of paternity." – "Secrets of the Phallus: Why Is the Penis Shaped Like That?", ScientificAmerican.com.
- ↑ 29.0 29.1 Grammer, Karl; Fink, Bernhard; Møller, Anders P.; Thornhill, Randy (1 August 2003). "Darwinian aesthetics: sexual selection and the biology of beauty". Biological Reviews 78 (3): 385–407. doi:10.1017/s1464793102006085. ISSN 1469-185X. PMID 14558590.
- ↑ 30.0 30.1 Rhodes, Gillian; Chan, Janelle; Zebrowitz, Leslie A.; Simmons, Leigh W. (7 August 2003). "Does sexual dimorphism in human faces signal health?". Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences 270 (Suppl 1): S93–S95. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2003.0023. ISSN 0962-8452. PMID 12952647.
- ↑ 31.0 31.1 Price, Michael E.; Pound, Nicholas; Dunn, James; Hopkins, Sian; Kang, Jinsheng (2 January 2013). "Body Shape Preferences: Associations with Rater Body Shape and Sociosexuality". PLOS ONE 8 (1): e52532. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0052532. PMID 23300976. Bibcode: 2013PLoSO...852532P.
- ↑ Buss, David (2019). "Women's Long-Term Mating Strategies". Evolutionary Psychology: The New Science of the Mind (6th ed.). Routledge. ISBN 978-0429590061. https://books.google.com/books?id=Sn6JDwAAQBAJ.
- ↑ 33.0 33.1 33.2 33.3 Feinberg, D. R.; Jones, B. C.; Law Smith, M. J.; Moore, F. R.; DeBruine, L. M.; Cornwell, R. E.; Hillier, S. G.; Perrett, D. I. (1 February 2006). "Menstrual cycle, trait estrogen level, and masculinity preferences in the human voice". Hormones and Behavior 49 (2): 215–222. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2005.07.004. PMID 16055126.
- ↑ "Strategies for Animal Survival 2 (Animal Studies Series)". Chip Taylor Communications. https://www.kanopystreaming.com/product/strategies-animal-survival-2.
- ↑ Shaw, Fionna (2009). Influence of female 2D:4D ratio on attractiveness of male vocal and facial masculinity.
- ↑ 36.0 36.1 Moore, F. R.; Coetzee, V.; Contreras-Garduño, J.; Debruine, L. M.; Kleisner, K.; Krams, I.; Marcinkowska, U.; Nord, A. et al. (23 June 2013). "Cross-cultural variation in women's preferences for cues to sex- and stress-hormones in the male face". Biology Letters 9 (3): 20130050. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2013.0050. ISSN 1744-9561. PMID 23536442.
- ↑ Scammell, Emily; Anderson, Ryan C. (2020). "Female Mate Copying: Measuring the Effect of Mate-Relevant Information Provided by Former Partners". Evolutionary Psychological Science 6 (4): 319–327. doi:10.1007/s40806-020-00239-9.
- ↑ 38.0 38.1 O'Connor, Jillian J. M.; Fraccaro, Paul J.; Pisanski, Katarzyna; Tigue, Cara C.; Feinberg, David R. (31 July 2013). "Men's Preferences for Women's Femininity in Dynamic Cross-Modal Stimuli". PLOS ONE 8 (7): e69531. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0069531. ISSN 1932-6203. PMID 23936037. Bibcode: 2013PLoSO...869531O.
- ↑ Welling, Lisa L. M.; Jones, Benedict C.; DeBruine, Lisa M.; Smith, Finlay G.; Feinberg, David R.; Little, Anthony C.; Al-Dujaili, Emad A. S. (1 November 2008). "Men report stronger attraction to femininity in women's faces when their testosterone levels are high". Hormones and Behavior 54 (5): 703–708. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2008.07.012. PMID 18755192.
- ↑ Bird, Brian M.; Welling, Lisa L. M.; Ortiz, Triana L.; Moreau, Benjamin J. P.; Hansen, Steve; Emond, Michael; Goldfarb, Bernard; Bonin, Pierre L. et al. (1 September 2016). "Effects of exogenous testosterone and mating context on men's preferences for female facial femininity". Hormones and Behavior 85: 76–85. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2016.08.003. PMID 27511452. https://zone.biblio.laurentian.ca/handle/10219/2617.
- ↑ Puts, David A.; Jones, Benedict C.; DeBruine, Lisa M. (1 March 2012). "Sexual Selection on Human Faces and Voices". The Journal of Sex Research 49 (2–3): 227–243. doi:10.1080/00224499.2012.658924. ISSN 0022-4499. PMID 22380590.
- ↑ "How Does Evolution Occur?". necsi.edu. http://necsi.edu/projects/evolution/evolution/5parts/evolution_5parts.html.
- ↑ 43.00 43.01 43.02 43.03 43.04 43.05 43.06 43.07 43.08 43.09 43.10 43.11 Puts, D., Carrier, D., and Rogers, A.R. (in press). Contest competition for mates and the evolution of human males. In: Handbook of Human Mating. Buss, D.M. and Durkee, P., eds. Oxford University Press. [ISBN missing][page needed]
- ↑ Mitani, J. C.; Gros-Louis, J.; Richards, A. F. (June 1996). "Sexual Dimorphism, the Operational Sex Ratio, and the Intensity of Male Competition in Polygynous Primates". The American Naturalist 147 (6): 966–980. doi:10.1086/285888. ISSN 0003-0147. http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/285888.
- ↑ Abe, T (2003-10-01). "Sex differences in whole body skeletal muscle mass measured by magnetic resonance imaging and its distribution in young Japanese adults". British Journal of Sports Medicine 37 (5): 436–440. doi:10.1136/bjsm.37.5.436. ISSN 0306-3674. PMID 14514537. PMC 1751351. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/bjsm.37.5.436.
- ↑ Hoffman, Steven G.; Schildhauer, Mark P.; Warner, Robert R. (July 1985). "The Costs of Changing Sex and the Ontogeny of Males Under Contest Competition for Mates". Evolution 39 (4): 915–927. doi:10.2307/2408690. ISSN 0014-3820. PMID 28561353. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/2408690.
- ↑ Archer, John (December 2004). "Sex Differences in Aggression in Real-World Settings: A Meta-Analytic Review". Review of General Psychology 8 (4): 291–322. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.8.4.291. ISSN 1089-2680. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.8.4.291.
- ↑ Ellis, Lee; Hershberger, Scott; Field, Evelyn; Wersinger, Scott; Pellis, Sergio; Geary, David; Palmer, Craig; Hoyenga, Katherine et al. (2013-05-13). Sex Differences. doi:10.4324/9780203838051. ISBN 978-0203838051. http://dx.doi.org/10.4324/9780203838051.
- ↑ Ryan, Michael J. (1995-02-03). "Offsetting Advantages: Sexual Selection. Malte Andersson. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ, 1994. 599 pp.". Science 267 (5198): 712–713. doi:10.1126/science.267.5198.712. ISSN 0036-8075. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.267.5198.712.
- ↑ Archer, John (August 2009). "Does sexual selection explain human sex differences in aggression?" (in en). Behavioral and Brain Sciences 32 (3–4): 249–266. doi:10.1017/S0140525X09990951. ISSN 0140-525X. PMID 19691899. https://www.cambridge.org/core/product/identifier/S0140525X09990951/type/journal_article.
- ↑ Smith, John Maynard; Parker, G.A. (February 1976). "The logic of asymmetric contests". Animal Behaviour 24 (1): 159–175. doi:10.1016/s0003-3472(76)80110-8. ISSN 0003-3472. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0003-3472(76)80110-8.
- ↑ Hodges-Simeon, Carolyn R.; Gaulin, Steven J. C.; Puts, David A. (December 2010). "Different Vocal Parameters Predict Perceptions of Dominance and Attractiveness" (in en). Human Nature 21 (4): 406–427. doi:10.1007/s12110-010-9101-5. ISSN 1045-6767. PMID 21212816.
- ↑ Feinberg, D.R.; Jones, B.C.; Little, A.C.; Burt, D.M.; Perrett, D.I. (March 2005). "Manipulations of fundamental and formant frequencies influence the attractiveness of human male voices" (in en). Animal Behaviour 69 (3): 561–568. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.06.012. https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0003347204003987.
- ↑ Puts, David Andrew; Gaulin, Steven J.C.; Verdolini, Katherine (July 2006). "Dominance and the evolution of sexual dimorphism in human voice pitch" (in en). Evolution and Human Behavior 27 (4): 283–296. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2005.11.003. https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S1090513805000966.
- ↑ 55.0 55.1 Puts, David A.; Hill, Alexander K.; Bailey, Drew H.; Walker, Robert S.; Rendall, Drew; Wheatley, John R.; Welling, Lisa L. M.; Dawood, Khytam et al. (2016-04-27). "Sexual selection on male vocal fundamental frequency in humans and other anthropoids". Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 283 (1829): 20152830. doi:10.1098/rspb.2015.2830. ISSN 0962-8452. PMID 27122553. PMC 4855375. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2015.2830.
- ↑ Fessler, Daniel M.T.; Tiokhin, Leonid B.; Holbrook, Colin; Gervais, Matthew M.; Snyder, Jeffrey K. (January 2014). "Foundations of the Crazy Bastard Hypothesis: Nonviolent physical risk-taking enhances conceptualized formidability" (in en). Evolution and Human Behavior 35 (1): 26–33. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2013.09.003. https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S1090513813000950.
- ↑ Craig, Belinda M.; Nelson, Nicole L.; Dixson, Barnaby J. W. (May 2019). "Sexual Selection, Agonistic Signaling, and the Effect of Beards on Recognition of Men's Anger Displays" (in en). Psychological Science 30 (5): 728–738. doi:10.1177/0956797619834876. ISSN 0956-7976. PMID 30908116. http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1177/0956797619834876.
- ↑ Guthrie, R. (1970). Evolution of human threat display organs. Evolutionary Biology, 4, 257–302.
- ↑ Hill, Alexander K.; Hunt, John; Welling, Lisa L.M.; Cárdenas, Rodrigo A.; Rotella, Michelle A.; Wheatley, John R.; Dawood, Khytam; Shriver, Mark D. et al. (September 2013). "Quantifying the strength and form of sexual selection on men's traits" (in en). Evolution and Human Behavior 34 (5): 334–341. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2013.05.004. https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S1090513813000615.
- ↑ Kordsmeyer, Tobias L.; Hunt, John; Puts, David A.; Ostner, Julia; Penke, Lars (July 2018). "The relative importance of intra- and intersexual selection on human male sexually dimorphic traits" (in en). Evolution and Human Behavior 39 (4): 424–436. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2018.03.008. https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S1090513817304105.
- ↑ Wolff, S. E., & Puts, D. A. (2010). "Vocal masculinity is a robust dominance signal in men". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 64(10), 1673–1683. doi:10.1007/s00265-010-0981-5
- ↑ 62.0 62.1 Carrier, David R. (2011-05-18). "The Advantage of Standing Up to Fight and the Evolution of Habitual Bipedalism in Hominins". PLOS ONE 6 (5): e19630. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0019630. ISSN 1932-6203. PMID 21611167. Bibcode: 2011PLoSO...619630C.
- ↑ Darwin, Charles (1871). The descent of man, and selection in relation to sex. London: J. Murray. doi:10.5962/bhl.title.2092. http://dx.doi.org/10.5962/bhl.title.2092.
- ↑ Thorpe, S.K.S.; Crompton, R.H.; Wang, W.J. (2004-02-08). "Stresses Exerted in the Hindlimb Muscles of Common Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) during Bipedal Locomotion". Folia Primatologica 75 (4): 253–265. doi:10.1159/000078937. ISSN 0015-5713. PMID 15316153. http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000078937.
- ↑ Marzke, M. W.; Wullstein, K. L.; Viegas, S. F. (November 1992). "Evolution of the power ("squeeze") grip and its morphological correlates in hominids" (in en). American Journal of Physical Anthropology 89 (3): 283–298. doi:10.1002/ajpa.1330890303. ISSN 0002-9483. PMID 1485637. https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/ajpa.1330890303.
- ↑ Napier, John (November 1962). "Fossil Hand Bones from Olduvai Gorge". Nature 196 (4853): 409–411. doi:10.1038/196409a0. ISSN 0028-0836. Bibcode: 1962Natur.196..409N. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/196409a0.
- ↑ 67.0 67.1 Young, Richard W. (January 2003). "Evolution of the human hand: the role of throwing and clubbing" (in en). Journal of Anatomy 202 (1): 165–174. doi:10.1046/j.1469-7580.2003.00144.x. ISSN 0021-8782. PMID 12587931.
- ↑ Young, Richard W. (2009-03-18). "The ontogeny of throwing and striking" (in en). Human_ontogenetics 3 (1): 19–31. doi:10.1002/huon.200800013. https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/huon.200800013.
- ↑ Plavcan, J.Michael; van Schaik, Carel P. (April 1997). "Interpreting hominid behavior on the basis of sexual dimorphism". Journal of Human Evolution 32 (4): 345–374. doi:10.1006/jhev.1996.0096. ISSN 0047-2484. PMID 9085186. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/jhev.1996.0096.
- ↑ Carrier, David R.; Morgan, Michael H. (February 2015). "Protective buttressing of the hominin face: Buttressing of the hominin face" (in en). Biological Reviews 90 (1): 330–346. doi:10.1111/brv.12112. PMID 24909544. https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/brv.12112.
- ↑ Papadatou-Pastou, Marietta; Martin, Maryanne; Munafò, Marcus R.; Jones, Gregory V. (September 2008). "Sex differences in left-handedness: A meta-analysis of 144 studies.". Psychological Bulletin 134 (5): 677–699. doi:10.1037/a0012814. ISSN 1939-1455. PMID 18729568. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0012814.
- ↑ Medland, Sarah E.; Duffy, David L.; Wright, Margaret J.; Geffen, Gina M.; Hay, David A.; Levy, Florence; van-Beijsterveldt, Catherina E.M.; Willemsen, Gonneke et al. (January 2009). "Genetic influences on handedness: Data from 25,732 Australian and Dutch twin families". Neuropsychologia 47 (2): 330–337. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.09.005. ISSN 0028-3932. PMID 18824185. PMC 2755095. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.09.005.
- ↑ Coren, Stanley; Halpern, Diane F. (1991). "Left-handedness: A marker for decreased survival fitness.". Psychological Bulletin 109 (1): 90–106. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.109.1.90. ISSN 1939-1455. PMID 2006231. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.109.1.90.
- ↑ Raymond, M.; Pontier, D.; Dufour, A. B.; Møller, A. P. (1996-12-22). "Frequency-dependent maintenance of left handedness in humans". Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences 263 (1377): 1627–1633. doi:10.1098/rspb.1996.0238. ISSN 0962-8452. PMID 9025310. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.1996.0238.
- ↑ Richardson, Thomas; Gilman, R. Tucker (2019-02-21). "Left-handedness is associated with greater fighting success in humans". Scientific Reports 9 (1): 15402. doi:10.1038/s41598-019-51975-3. PMID 31659217. PMC 6817864. Bibcode: 2019NatSR...915402R. http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/555912. Retrieved 2022-12-12.
- ↑ Fillingim, R. B., King, C. D., Ribeiro-Dasilva, M. C., Rahim-Williams, B., & Riley, J. L. (2009). Sex, gender, and pain: a review of recent clinical and experimental findings. The journal of pain, 10(5), 447–485. Fitch, W. T., & Giedd, J. (1999).
- ↑ Riley, Joseph L; Robinson, Michael E; Wise, Emily A; Myers, Cynthia D; Fillingim, Roger B (January 1998). "Sex differences in the perception of noxious experimental stimuli: a meta-analysis". Pain 74 (2): 181–187. doi:10.1016/s0304-3959(97)00199-1. ISSN 0304-3959. PMID 9520232. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0304-3959(97)00199-1.
- ↑ Billat, Véronique L.; Demarle, Alexandre; Slawinski, Jean; Paiva, Mario; Koralsztein, Jean-Pierre (December 2001). "Physical and training characteristics of top-class marathon runners". Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise 33 (12): 2089–2097. doi:10.1097/00005768-200112000-00018. ISSN 0195-9131. PMID 11740304.
- ↑ Davies, C. T. M.; Thompson, M. W. (August 1979). "Aerobic performance of female marathon and male ultramarathon athletes". European Journal of Applied Physiology and Occupational Physiology 41 (4): 233–245. doi:10.1007/bf00429740. ISSN 0301-5548. PMID 499187. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/bf00429740.
- ↑ Vugt, Mark Van; Cremer, David De; Janssen, Dirk P. (January 2007). "Gender Differences in Cooperation and Competition". Psychological Science 18 (1): 19–23. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01842.x. ISSN 0956-7976. PMID 17362372. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01842.x.
- ↑ Brooks, James; Onishi, Ena; Clark, Isabelle R.; Bohn, Manuel; Yamamoto, Shinya (2021-02-24). "Uniting against a common enemy: Perceived outgroup threat elicits ingroup cohesion in chimpanzees". PLOS ONE 16 (2): e0246869. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0246869. ISSN 1932-6203. PMID 33626062. Bibcode: 2021PLoSO..1646869B.
- ↑ McDonald, Melissa M.; Navarrete, Carlos David; Van Vugt, Mark (2012-03-05). "Evolution and the psychology of intergroup conflict: the male warrior hypothesis". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 367 (1589): 670–679. doi:10.1098/rstb.2011.0301. ISSN 0962-8436. PMID 22271783. PMC 3260849. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2011.0301.
- ↑ Wrangham, Richard W. (1999). "Evolution of coalitionary killing". American Journal of Physical Anthropology 110 (S29): 1–30. doi:10.1002/(sici)1096-8644(1999)110:29+<1::aid-ajpa2>3.0.co;2-e. ISSN 0002-9483.
- ↑ 84.0 84.1 Berglund, Anders; Bisazza, Angelo; Pilastro, Andrea (August 1996). "Armaments and ornaments: an evolutionary explanation of traits of dual utility". Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 58 (4): 385–399. doi:10.1111/j.1095-8312.1996.tb01442.x. ISSN 0024-4066.
- ↑ Puts, David A. (May 2010). "Beauty and the beast: mechanisms of sexual selection in humans". Evolution and Human Behavior 31 (3): 157–175. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.02.005. ISSN 1090-5138. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.02.005.
- ↑ Frederick, David A.; Haselton, Martie G. (August 2007). "Why Is Muscularity Sexy? Tests of the Fitness Indicator Hypothesis" (in en). Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 33 (8): 1167–1183. doi:10.1177/0146167207303022. ISSN 0146-1672. PMID 17578932. http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1177/0146167207303022.
- ↑ Puts, David A.; Jones, Benedict C.; DeBruine, Lisa M. (2012-03-01). "Sexual Selection on Human Faces and Voices" (in en). The Journal of Sex Research 49 (2–3): 227–243. doi:10.1080/00224499.2012.658924. ISSN 0022-4499. PMID 22380590. https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/full/10.1080/00224499.2012.658924.
- ↑ Buss, David M. (March 1989). "Sex differences in human mate preferences: Evolutionary hypotheses tested in 37 cultures". Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1): 1–14. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00023992.
- ↑ 89.0 89.1 Fisher, Maryanne L. (2022). "Female Intrasexual Competition". The Cambridge Handbook of Evolutionary Perspectives on Sexual Psychology: Volume 3: Female Sexual Adaptations 3: 91–117. doi:10.1017/9781108943567.006. ISBN 9781108943567.
- ↑ Campbell, Anne (April 1999). "Staying alive: Evolution, culture, and women's intrasexual aggression". Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22 (2): 203–214. doi:10.1017/s0140525x99001818. PMID 11301523.
- ↑ 91.0 91.1 Benenson, Joyce F.; Webb, Christine E.; Wrangham, Richard W. (2022). "Self-protection as an adaptive female strategy". Behavioral and Brain Sciences 45: e128. doi:10.1017/S0140525X21002417. PMID 34742359.
- ↑ 92.0 92.1 92.2 92.3 92.4 92.5 92.6 92.7 Fisher, Maryanne L.; Krems, Jaimie Arona (26 January 2023). "An Evolutionary Review of Female Intrasexual Competition". The Oxford Handbook of Human Mating: 378–C14.P201. doi:10.1093/oxfordhb/9780197536438.013.27. ISBN 978-0197536438.
- ↑ Benenson, Joyce F. (August 2009). "Dominating versus eliminating the competition: Sex differences in human intrasexual aggression". Behavioral and Brain Sciences 32 (3–4): 268–269. doi:10.1017/s0140525x0999046x.
- ↑ Campbell, Anne (5 December 2013). "The evolutionary psychology of women's aggression". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 368 (1631): 20130078. doi:10.1098/rstb.2013.0078. PMID 24167308.
- ↑ Reynolds, Tania; Baumeister, Roy F.; Maner, Jon K. (2018). "Competitive reputation manipulation: Women strategically transmit social information about romantic rivals". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 78: 195–209. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2018.03.011.
- ↑ Rosvall, Kimberly A. (1 November 2011). "Intrasexual competition in females: evidence for sexual selection?". Behavioral Ecology 22 (6): 1131–1140. doi:10.1093/beheco/arr106. PMID 22479137.
- ↑ Campbell, Anne (2013-12-05). "The evolutionary psychology of women's aggression" (in en). Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 368 (1631): 20130078. doi:10.1098/rstb.2013.0078. ISSN 0962-8436. PMID 24167308.
- ↑ 98.0 98.1 Fisher, Maryanne L.; Krems, Jaimie Arona (2023-01-26), "An Evolutionary Review of Female Intrasexual Competition", The Oxford Handbook of Human Mating (Oxford University Press): pp. 378–C14.P201, doi:10.1093/oxfordhb/9780197536438.013.27, ISBN 978-0-19-753643-8, http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780197536438.013.27, retrieved 2023-12-09
- ↑ Moore, F.R.; Law Smith, M.J.; Taylor, V.; Perrett, D.I. (May 2011). "Sexual dimorphism in the female face is a cue to health and social status but not age" (in en). Personality and Individual Differences 50 (7): 1068–1073. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2011.01.026. https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S019188691100047X.
- ↑ Puts, David A. (May 2010). "Beauty and the beast: mechanisms of sexual selection in humans" (in en). Evolution and Human Behavior 31 (3): 157–175. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.02.005. https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S1090513810000279.
- ↑ Puts, David A.; Jones, Benedict C.; DeBruine, Lisa M. (2012-03-01). "Sexual Selection on Human Faces and Voices" (in en). The Journal of Sex Research 49 (2–3): 227–243. doi:10.1080/00224499.2012.658924. ISSN 0022-4499. PMID 22380590. https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/full/10.1080/00224499.2012.658924.
- ↑ 102.0 102.1 102.2 Dixson, Barnaby J. W. (January 2022). "Sexual Selection and the Evolution of Human Appearance Enhancements" (in en). Archives of Sexual Behavior 51 (1): 49–55. doi:10.1007/s10508-021-01946-5. ISSN 0004-0002. PMID 33721143. https://link.springer.com/10.1007/s10508-021-01946-5.
- ↑ Norgan, Ng (September 1997). "The beneficial effects of body fat and adipose tissue in humans*" (in en). International Journal of Obesity 21 (9): 738–746. doi:10.1038/sj.ijo.0800473. ISSN 0307-0565. PMID 9376885. https://www.nature.com/articles/0800473.
- ↑ Lassek, W; Gaulin, S (January 2008). "Waist-hip ratio and cognitive ability: is gluteofemoral fat a privileged store of neurodevelopmental resources?" (in en). Evolution and Human Behavior 29 (1): 26–34. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2007.07.005. https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S1090513807000736.
- ↑ Singh, Devendra (1993). "Adaptive significance of female physical attractiveness: Role of waist-to-hip ratio." (in en). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 65 (2): 293–307. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.65.2.293. ISSN 1939-1315. PMID 8366421. http://doi.apa.org/getdoi.cfm?doi=10.1037/0022-3514.65.2.293.
- ↑ Hochberg, Ze'ev; Gawlik, Aneta; Walker, Robert S (December 2011). "Evolutionary fitness as a function of pubertal age in 22 subsistence-based traditional societies" (in en). International Journal of Pediatric Endocrinology 2011 (1): 2. doi:10.1186/1687-9856-2011-2. ISSN 1687-9856. PMID 21860629.
- ↑ Lassek, William D.; Gaulin, Steven J. C. (2018-10-01). "Do the Low WHRs and BMIs Judged Most Attractive Indicate Higher Fertility?" (in en). Evolutionary Psychology 16 (4): 147470491880006. doi:10.1177/1474704918800063. ISSN 1474-7049. PMID 30296846.
- ↑ Mahoney, Sean M.; Reudink, Matthew W.; Contina, Andrea; Roberts, Kelly A.; Schabert, Veronica T.; Gunther, Emily G.; Covino, Kristen M. (March 2022). "A tail of plumage colouration: disentangling geographic, seasonal and dietary effects on plumage colour in a migratory songbird" (in en). Journal of Avian Biology 2022 (3). doi:10.1111/jav.02957. ISSN 0908-8857. https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/jav.02957.
- ↑ Loyau, Adeline; Jalme, Michel Saint; Sorci, Gabriele (September 2005). "Intra‐ and Intersexual Selection for Multiple Traits in the Peacock ( Pavo cristatus )" (in en). Ethology 111 (9): 810–820. doi:10.1111/j.1439-0310.2005.01091.x. ISSN 0179-1613. Bibcode: 2005Ethol.111..810L. https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1439-0310.2005.01091.x.
- ↑ Carmen, Rachael A.; Guitar, Amanda E.; Dillon, Haley M. (June 2012). "Ultimate Answers to Proximate Questions: The Evolutionary Motivations behind Tattoos and Body Piercings in Popular Culture" (in en). Review of General Psychology 16 (2): 134–143. doi:10.1037/a0027908. ISSN 1089-2680. http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1037/a0027908.
- ↑ 111.0 111.1 111.2 Elliot, Andrew J.; Pazda, Adam D. (2012-04-13). Clifton, Julian. ed. "Dressed for Sex: Red as a Female Sexual Signal in Humans" (in en). PLOS ONE 7 (4): e34607. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0034607. ISSN 1932-6203. PMID 22514643. Bibcode: 2012PLoSO...734607E.
- ↑ Dubuc, Constance; Winters, Sandra; Allen, William L.; Brent, Lauren J. N.; Cascio, Julie; Maestripieri, Dario; Ruiz-Lambides, Angelina V.; Widdig, Anja et al. (2014-11-07). "Sexually selected skin colour is heritable and related to fecundity in a non-human primate" (in en). Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 281 (1794): 20141602. doi:10.1098/rspb.2014.1602. ISSN 0962-8452. PMID 25253459.
- ↑ Setchell, Joanna M; Jean Wickings, E; Knapp, Leslie A (2006-09-22). "Signal content of red facial coloration in female mandrills ( Mandrillus sphinx )" (in en). Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 273 (1599): 2395–2400. doi:10.1098/rspb.2006.3573. ISSN 0962-8452. PMID 16928644.
- ↑ Higham, James P; Kimock, Clare M; Mandalaywala, Tara M; Heistermann, Michael; Cascio, Julie; Petersdorf, Megan; Winters, Sandra; Allen, William L et al. (2021-03-26). Barrett, Louise. ed. "Female ornaments: is red skin color attractive to males and related to condition in rhesus macaques?" (in en). Behavioral Ecology 32 (2): 236–247. doi:10.1093/beheco/araa121. ISSN 1045-2249. PMID 33814977. PMC 7995641. https://academic.oup.com/beheco/article/32/2/236/6122596.
- ↑ Elliot, Andrew J.; Niesta, Daniela (2008). "Romantic red: Red enhances men's attraction to women." (in en). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 95 (5): 1150–1164. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.95.5.1150. ISSN 1939-1315. PMID 18954199. http://doi.apa.org/getdoi.cfm?doi=10.1037/0022-3514.95.5.1150.
- ↑ Niesta Kayser, Daniela; Elliot, Andrew J.; Feltman, Roger (October 2010). "Red and romantic behavior in men viewing women" (in en). European Journal of Social Psychology 40 (6): 901–908. doi:10.1002/ejsp.757. ISSN 0046-2772. https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/ejsp.757.
- ↑ Schwarz, Sascha; Singer, Marie (January 2013). "Romantic red revisited: Red enhances men's attraction to young, but not menopausal women" (in en). Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 49 (1): 161–164. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2012.08.004. https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0022103112001564.
- ↑ Haselton, Martie G.; Mortezaie, Mina; Pillsworth, Elizabeth G.; Bleske-Rechek, April; Frederick, David A. (January 2007). "Ovulatory shifts in human female ornamentation: Near ovulation, women dress to impress" (in en). Hormones and Behavior 51 (1): 40–45. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2006.07.007. PMID 17045994. https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0018506X06002509.
- ↑ Puts, David A.; Bailey, Drew H.; Cárdenas, Rodrigo A.; Burriss, Robert P.; Welling, Lisa L. M.; Wheatley, John R.; Dawood, Khytam (2013-01-01). "Women's attractiveness changes with estradiol and progesterone across the ovulatory cycle". Hormones and Behavior 63 (1): 13–19. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.11.007. ISSN 0018-506X. PMID 23159480. https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0018506X12002796.
- ↑ Catena, Tikal M.; Simmons, Zachary L.; Roney, James R. (September 2019). "Do women's faces become more attractive near ovulation?" (in en). Hormones and Behavior 115: 104560. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2019.07.008. PMID 31310761. https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0018506X19301333.
- ↑ Gangestad, Steven W; Thornhill, Randy (2008-05-07). "Human oestrus" (in en). Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 275 (1638): 991–1000. doi:10.1098/rspb.2007.1425. ISSN 0962-8452. PMID 18252670.
- ↑ 122.0 122.1 Manning, John (2009). The Finger Ratio. Faber & Faber. ISBN 978-0571215409.
- ↑ MacKintosh, J. (2001). "The antimicrobial properties of melanocytes, melanosomes and melanin and the evolution of black skin.". Journal of Theoretical Biology 211 (2): 101–113. doi:10.1006/jtbi.2001.2331. PMID 11419954. Bibcode: 2001JThBi.211..101M.
- ↑ Kirchweger, Gina (1 February 2001). "The Biology of . . . Skin Color". Discover. http://discovermagazine.com/2001/feb/featbiology/.
- ↑ Rogers, Alan R.; Iltis, David; Wooding, Stephen (2004). "Genetic Variation at the MC1R Locus and the Time since Loss of Human Body Hair". Current Anthropology 45: 105–108. doi:10.1086/381006.
- ↑ 126.0 126.1 Jablonski, Nina G.; Chaplin, George; Chaplin (2000). "The evolution of human skin coloration". Journal of Human Evolution 39 (1): 57–106. doi:10.1006/jhev.2000.0403. PMID 10896812. http://www.bgsu.edu/departments/chem/faculty/leontis/chem447/PDF_files/Jablonski_skin_color_2000.pdf.
- ↑ Elias, Peter M; Menon, Gapinathan; Wetzel, Bruce J; Williams, John (Jack) W (2010). "Barrier Requirements as the Evolutionary "Driver" of Epidermal Pigmentation in Humans". American Journal of Human Biology 22 (4): 526–537. doi:10.1002/ajhb.21043. PMID 20209486.
- ↑ Jablonski, N. G.; Chaplin, G. (2010). "Colloquium Paper: Human skin pigmentation as an adaptation to UV radiation". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 107 (Suppl 2): 8962–8968. doi:10.1073/pnas.0914628107. PMID 20445093. Bibcode: 2010PNAS..107.8962J.
- ↑ Juzeniene, Asta; Setlow, Richard; Porojnicu, Alina; Steindal, Arnfinn Hykkerud; Moan, Johan (2009). "Development of different human skin colors: A review highlighting photobiological and photobiophysical aspects". Journal of Photochemistry and Photobiology B: Biology 96 (2): 93–100. doi:10.1016/j.jphotobiol.2009.04.009. PMID 19481954.
- ↑ Frost, P. (2008). "Sexual selection and human geographic variation". Journal of Social, Evolutionary, and Cultural Psychology 2 (4): 169–191. doi:10.1037/h0099346. http://137.140.1.71/jsec/articles/volume2/issue4/NEEPSfrost.pdf.
- ↑ Cunningham, M.R.; Roberts, A.R.; Barbee, A.P.; Druen, P.B.; Wu, C-H. (1995). "Their ideas of beauty are, on the whole, the same as ours": consistency and variability in the cross-cultural perception of female physical attractiveness". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 68 (2): 261–279. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.68.2.261.
- ↑ Geoffrey Miller, The Mating Mind, p.111; published 2001
- ↑ Klasios, J. (2013). "Cognitive traits as sexually selected fitness indicators". Review of General Psychology. pp. 428–442. http://psycnet.apa.org/journals/gpr/17/4/428/.
Further reading
- Buss, David M. (2016). The Evolution of Desire: Strategies of Human Mating (3rd ed.). New York: Basic Books. ISBN 978-0465097760.
- Miller, Geoffrey F. (2000). The Mating Mind: How Sexual Choice Shaped the Evolution of Human Nature (1st ed.). New York: Doubleday. ISBN 978-0385495165. https://archive.org/details/matingmind00geof.
Original source: https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Sexual selection in humans.
Read more |